Attributions and success in new venture creation among Canadian nascent entrepreneurs.
Diochon, Monica ; Menzies, Teresa V. ; Gasse, Yvon 等
The question of why some new ventures are successfully brought to
fruition while others are not is one that continues to elude
researchers. Generally, we know that the rate of firm failure is
inversely related to age (Thornhill and Amit, 2003; Sapienza, 1991).
While Beaver (2003) claims that failure rates among new businesses are
ten times as high as those of larger established firms, other estimates
of failure range anywhere from 50 to 95%. Not surprisingly, the high
rate of failure among small businesses, particularly new ones, has been
a concern among policymakers for some time (Storey 1994). Over the past
decade, the importance of this concern has increased as policymakers
focus their efforts on encouraging new firm formation.
In light of the relationship between a firm's age and failure,
nascent ventures--those in the gestation stage of start-up--would be
expected to have the highest rate of demise. Yet, gestation is an aspect
of the entrepreneurial process about which very little is known (Gartner
et al., 2004). One of the major stumbling blocks in acquiring more
knowledge of business formation has been the lack of "real
time" study of the start-up process. Currently, the Entrepreneurial
Research Consortium (ERC)--comprised of researchers in nine
countries--is addressing this issue through a research project commonly
referred to as the Panel Study of Entrepreneurial Dynamics (PSED) (for
further details on the consortium see Gartner et al., 2004).
Given how little is known about the factors influencing firm
formation, there is a pressing need for research. Since entrepreneurs
have been shown to differ from other people with regard to certain
aspects of cognition (Baron, 1998; Palich and Bagby, 1995; Kahneman and
Lovallo, 1993), specifically, this paper explores whether individual
differences in attributional style are associated with later success in
bringing a business to fruition. Drawing on the attributional framework
presented by Shaver (2004), we investigate this issue using four years
of real time data collected from Canadian nascent entrepreneurs. The
results of this investigation should be valuable to policymakers.
Without a better understanding of firm formation, how can policy
measures aimed at encouraging new firm formation be effective?
Prospective entrepreneurs, too, have a vested interest in knowing more
about what contributes to "success" and "failure."
The paper begins by reviewing what we know about success and
failure in a start-up context. It then explores the usefulness of
attribution theory in explaining events and behaviour. Next it assesses
the conceptual framework proposed by Shaver (2004) as a means of
predicting the success and failure of nascent entrepreneurs in
establishing new ventures. Hypotheses are proposed. The method section
describes the research design, sampling and data collection process as
well as the measures and analysis employed. Results are then presented
and discussed.
Literature Review
New Venture "Success" and "Failure": What Does
it Mean?
Researchers have been attempting to explain the "success"
and "failure" of new ventures for some time. However,
investigations have been criticized for focusing largely on
post-founding activities, for focusing only on those who succeeded in
forming a business, and for hindsight bias (recollections of
pre-founding). Definitions, or lack thereof, also have been problematic
(see Diochon, Menzies and Gasse, 2005a for further discussion).
"Success" is a term that means different things to
different people (Rogoff, Lee and Suh, 2004; Beaver, 2003).
Entrepreneurs are likely to define success differently than the
venture's external stakeholders, such as lenders or customers. For
example, one entrepreneur might define success in terms of whether the
business can generate an income of $25,000, another might define it
according to whether (s)he achieves personal fulfilment, while a lender
might define it according to whether the business can service its debt.
"Failure," being the antonym of "success," also
would mean different things to different people.
In the literature, many terms are used synonymously with both
"success" ("survival" or "continued
viability") and "failure" ("closure" or
"bankruptcy") even though they do not mean the same thing
(Rogoff, Lee and Suh, 2004; Beaver, 2003). Indeed, a business can be
abandoned without failing such as when an owner becomes ill. Given the
wide range of meanings employed, it is not surprising that the
literature has come under attack for being full of misinformation or for
making inferences that are unjustified (Stokes and Blackburn, 2002).
Within the context of start-up, a central and unresolved issue is
that of defining success in forming a business. Much of the difficulty
in determining the point when the transition is made from becoming to
being a new business can be traced to the fact that firm founding is a
process, rather than a discrete event (Diochon, Menzies and Gasse 2005a;
Carter et al. 2004). Yet, without being able to determine that something
did or did not happen (ie. that a business is formed), how are
explanations possible for why some people succeed (in forming a firm)
while others do not?
We contend that what is most problematic about the lack of explicit
definition (Carland, Carland and Carland, 1995), is the validity of the
measures. For example, while it is common to operationally define a new
firm as one that has been incorporated within the past twelve months,
this excludes all unincorporated businesses. With the aim of addressing
the definitional void and establishing a measurement foundation that is
clear and meaningful, we offer the following definition of a new
business: a recently formed commercial organization that provides goods
and services for sale (Diochon, Menzies and Gasse, 2005a). Arguably, it
is important not only for the business to start but to stay in business.
Doing so would constitute "success" in forming a firm. We deal
with the issue of operationalizing the above definition in the method
section.
To address the question of why some people "succeed" in
establishing a new firm while others do not, we turn to the psychology
literature. People, generally, attempt to develop explanations of their
own and others' behaviour (Shaver et al., 2001; Weiner, 1985;
Heider, 1958). Broadly, efforts to answer the question "why"
are described in the field of social psychology by the term
attribution--the processes through which an individual seeks to identify
the causes of events, others' behaviour, or practically anything
that is encompassed by their experience (see Pittman, 1993).
With the interpretation of a situation often more important as a
determinant of future action than the facts (Robbins, Coulter and
Stuart-Kotze, 2003; Martocchio and Ford, 1985), the meaning people give
to events and behaviour is of heightened significance. Since we know
that each person experiences a somewhat different reality, it is
important to try and understand what that person's perceived
reality is, how it is being interpreted, and the implications for
action. One theoretical framework that enables us to do that is
attribution theory. In the next section, we review the usefulness of
this theory in a new venture context.
Attribution Theory in a New Venture Context
Attribution theory was originally proposed by Heider (1958). With
regard to task success, he argued that three things were required: an
intention to perform the task, exertion in the direction of the
intention, and a personal ability that exceeded the difficulty of the
task. Weiner (1985) later represented these elements by two dimensions:
locus of causality and stability of the cause. As shown in Table 1, the
locus of causality refers to whether the cause(s) of events or behaviour
is considered to be internal or external (to the person). The stability
of the cause refers to whether the cause is capable of rather immediate
change. For example, a person's ability is deemed internal and
stable since it does not really change quickly. However, effort is
considered internal and variable since the extent to which a person
tries can change from one situation to another. Among the major external
causes, task difficulty is the relatively stable factor whereas luck is,
by definition, variable.
Given attribution theory's usefulness in explaining
intentional behaviour (Anderson, 1991), Shaver (2004) has argued it is
particularly appropriate in understanding firm formation which involves
domain-specific intentional action that is focused on achieving a
desired outcome. The activities engaged in during the start-up process
reflect a series of choices (Shaver and Scott, 1991). Arguably, the
reasons entrepreneurs give for their choices are likely to provide
insight into eventual success (or failure) in forming a firm, among
other things.
In examining his/her behaviour or an event, a person will try to
determine if its "successful" accomplishment (or
"failure") was due to personal factors, such as ability or
external factors, such as luck; and whether the cause was stable or
unstable (Weiner, 1985). Ultimately, the outcome could be attributed to
ability, task difficulty, effort, or luck (as illustrated in the table).
Whether "success" or "failure" is attributed to
internal causes or external causes will have implications for the future
(Shaver et al., 1996; Weiner, 1986).
Attribution theory is said to be one of the few psychological
theories that can deal with a key characteristic of
entrepreneurs--persistence after setbacks or failure (Shaver, 2004). If
an entrepreneur attributes the cause of setbacks or failure to external
factors (such as size of the market or the number of competitors), there
is no reason not to try again. However, if the cause is attributed to
internal factors--specifically stable ones such as ability--then the
entrepreneur would be unlikely to view starting a second venture as
appealing (Cardon and McGrath, 1999). Alternatively, if the cause is an
internal variable one, such as insufficient effort or poor strategy, a
failure to establish a business need not imply that another attempt will
also fail. Rather than feeling helpless in the face of the problem, the
entrepreneur is motivated to solve it. In this case, the individual
wants to learn from his/her mistakes, and to apply that learning so
future problems can be avoided.
According to Shaver (2004), the utility of an attributional
approach in explaining current (as opposed to past) activity lies in the
fact that there are reliable individual differences in explanatory style
(Peterson and Villanova, 1988). Since events like establishing a new
venture would have more than one cause, it would be reasonable to expect
that people would disagree about what might be the cause. "In such
a case, people whose predilections lead them to look for causes internal
to people may concentrate on the enduring personal characteristics.
Alternatively, people whose predilections lead them to look for
changeable external causes are more likely to concentrate on factors
outside the person" (Shaver, 2004: 207).
Shaver (2004) argues that individual differences in explanatory
style, coupled with the notion that attributions may be related to
entrepreneurial persistence, suggest that an entrepreneur's reasons
for entering business might be related to later success (or lack
thereof) in establishing a new venture.
Like every process, business start-up takes place over time,
involving both positive and negative situations. Indeed, it is well
known that entrepreneurs face high levels of uncertainty, novelty and
time pressure--factors which tend to overload information-processing
capacity and increase entrepreneurs' susceptibility to a number of
cognitive biases (Forbes, 2005; Baron, 1998). In particular,
entrepreneurs may be more susceptible to the self-serving bias than
others (Baron, 1998). The self-serving bias refers to a tendency for
individuals to offer internal attributions for positive events and
provide external attributions for negative events. Indeed, Gartner and
Shaver (2002) report evidence of a self-serving bias among US nascent
entrepreneurs in the PSED. The evidence showed that nascent
entrepreneurs described opportunities as internal and stable while
problems were described as external and variable.
This suggests that nascent entrepreneurs would have a tendency to
perceive starting a business positively and would be more likely to
offer internal (and stable) attributions in describing their reasons for
starting a business. On the other hand, problems or setbacks would be
perceived negatively, and would be afforded attributions that are
external (and variable). Many researchers refer to the "contrasting
patterns of attribution (the typical 'self-serving' pattern
shown by most people and its opposite) as discrete attributional
styles" (Baron, 1998: 284).
Baron (1998) suggests that among entrepreneurs, one factor that
might differentiate those who are successful from those who are not is
that "successful" entrepreneurs are less susceptible to the
self-serving bias. Accordingly, because the thinking of successful
entrepreneurs is influenced less by the self-serving bias, these
individuals are better able to establish positive relationships
necessary to the survival or growth of their business (such as with
investors or suppliers). In presenting his argument, Baron cites an
extensive body of literature indicating the self-serving bias can be a
major source of interpersonal conflict. This conflict arises when
individuals who work together become aware of each other's tendency
to take credit for positive outcomes and blame the other person for
negative results. Baron goes on to argue that successful entrepreneurs
have been shown to be more adept than unsuccessful ones at forging
positive relationships with their stakeholders. Using this reasoning in
a start-up context, we would argue that nascent entrepreneurs who are
successful in starting (and sustaining) a business would be less
susceptible to the self-serving bias and, therefore, better able to
establish the key interpersonal relationships necessary to bring their
venture to fruition.
Accepting the notion that a self-serving bias would lead people to
offer external attributions for negative events, then those nascent
entrepreneurs who report abandoning their efforts to start and stay in
business would be expected to provide external attributions for doing
so.
Based on the preceding discussion, we propose the following
hypotheses:
H1: Nascent entrepreneurs will predominantly offer internal, stable
attributions in describing positive situations.
H2: Nascent entrepreneurs will predominantly offer external,
variable attributions in describing negative situations.
H3: Nascent entrepreneurs who are successful in starting a business
will be less susceptible to the self-serving bias than nascent
entrepreneurs who have given up on their efforts to start a business.
H4: Among nascent entrepreneurs who do not persist in their efforts
to start a business, the least likely explanation they will provide for
giving up will be internal stable reasons.
H5: Nascent entrepreneurs who provide internal stable reasons for
giving up will be less likely to view the prospect of starting another
venture as appealing than nascent entrepreneurs who provide other
reasons for giving up.
Method
Research Design, Sampling, and Data Collection
The data for this article were obtained from a national
longitudinal study of business startups being conducted by the Canadian
members of the ERC. While the primary objective of the Consortium is to
provide systematic, reliable data on the basic features of the startup
process, a second objective is to provide reliable data on the factors
that would further our understanding of how and why new firms emerge,
grow, decline, die or remain stable. To achieve these objectives, the
research design had to fulfil a number of criteria including:
standardization of procedures (for selecting a representative sample and
for developing systematic descriptions of the entrepreneurial process);
incorporation of all major perspectives in data collection (so that the
direct and interactive influences can be identified); longitudinal
design (to track the process); and the inclusion of a wide range of
scholarly input for research design and analysis (to deal with the
complexity inherent in the project) (see Reynolds, 2000).
Given the scope of the start-up process and the factors influencing
it, the questionnaires that were developed consisted largely of
fixed-response questions, although a few open-ended questions were
asked. The questions dealing with attributions were developed by a
fifteen-member team of Consortium scholars. This team was responsible
for developing the person variables portion of the research instrument
which included nine topics, of which "attributions for success and
failure" is one. Moreover, they were responsible for the
reliability and validity of the various items and measures used (for
further detail on item development, see Gartner et al., 2004).
In accordance with the procedures established by the ERC, we
engaged SOM, a national polling firm, to select a representative sample
of people engaged in the start-up process (nascent entrepreneurs) in
Canada during the winter of 2000. From the initial 49,763 randomly
selected telephone numbers, there were 29,855 usable numbers. Using a
Computer Assisted Telephone Interviewing System (CATIS) interviews were
completed with individuals in 21,116 households.
The initial screening interview produced a sample of 593 as some
households had multiple nascent entrepreneurs. Out of these, 463
qualified for the longer telephone interview by answering affirmatively
to all three screening questions (1) and 416 agreed to participate in
the follow-up interview. In June 2000 detailed data collection began,
using phone and mail survey instruments, covering a wide range of topics
(which are detailed in Gartner et al., 2004).
At this point, the respondent's status as a nascent
entrepreneur was double-checked. This involved determining whether the
respondent was still in the gestation phase of the start-up process.
Approximately one fifth (19%) of the respondents were dropped at this
stage as they were too far advanced to be considered in the gestation
phase of the entrepreneurial process. Among the remaining respondents,
it was not possible to locate and contact about 24%. Another 17% of the
remaining group would not or could not complete the phone interview.
A total of 151 verified and accessible nascent entrepreneurs
completed the initial detailed telephone interview. This represents a
59% response rate of those that could be contacted and were eligible.
Follow-up interviews were completed at 12-month intervals: 2001 (n=132),
2002 (n=119), 2003 (n=104), and 2004 (n= 91). The follow-up interviews
enabled us to track activities and determine whether a firm was founded.
In this article we analyze attributional data from the detailed
phone interviews along with data on the venture's start-up status.
A venture that achieved operating status in any year of follow-up and
maintained that status through to the 48-month follow-up was considered
a success (as it had started and stayed in business). As noted above,
our sample in 2004 consisted of 91 of the original 151 (60%). Among
these 91, the distribution of startup status responses is: operating
business (22 responses, 24%), active startup (6 responses, 7%), inactive
startup (4 responses, 4%), abandoned start-up (59 responses, 65%). We
excluded the active (still trying to start) and inactive (no current
start-up efforts but venture not abandoned) start-ups from our analysis
since the final outcome of these 10 startup efforts is yet to be
determined. Consequently, our results are based on the 81 of the
original 151 (54%) that had either succeeded in establishing and
sustaining an operating business or abandoned their start-up efforts.
Considering that the data for our analysis involved four phone
interviews, each a year apart, this is a very good response rate. In
comparison, Gartner and Shaver (2002) report a US response rate of 55%
with only two follow-up interviews.
Initial Attributional Coding
To examine the relationship between attributions and success in
starting a business, the present article draws on data collected from
five open-ended questions included in the phone interview:
"Why are you starting this business?"
"Why do you expect your business to be successful?"
"What major problems have you had in starting this
business?"
"What major problems do you expect in the future?"
"What is the most important reason why you gave up?"
All responses were coded by one of the principal researchers and an
undergraduate research assistant according to the protocol developed and
documented by Shaver et al. (2001). Prior to coding the sample's
responses, training (which took about five hours) in the coding process
was conducted using item sets provided by Shaver et al. (2001).
The coding procedure involved the following three steps. Acting
independently, each coder first parsed every answer into the number of
separate explanations it contained. Then for each separate explanation,
the coder first decided whether the explanation identified a factor
internal to the person or a factor in the external environment. Once the
internal/external decision had been made, the coder then decided whether
the explanation identified a stable characteristic (one that would not,
or could not, change in the immediate short term) or a variable
characteristic (one that would, or could, change in the immediate short
term). For example, "Have more independence" is an
internal-stable attribution for a reason to start, whereas
"competition" is an external-variable attribution for a
problem.
Reliability testing was based on the complete sample and was
determined separately for each step in the process. Cohen's kappa
was used to assess intercoder reliability since it accounts for
agreement that could be expected to occur by chance. Despite the
simplicity and widespread use of percent agreement, we did not use this
measure as it is known to be a misleading and inappropriately liberal
measure of intercoder agreement for nominal-level variables. (It
overestimates true intercoder agreement.) Since the kappa index tends to
be more conservative than other indices, a kappa coefficient of 0.61 is
said to represent reasonably good overall agreement (Kvalseth, 1989;
Wheelock et al., 2000; Landis and Koch, 1977).
Table 2 presents the intercoder reliability results using
Cohen's kappa. With one exception (kappa of .517 for the
stable/variable step of reasons for starting), these results show a
reasonably strong degree of reliability. Indeed, the exception does fall
within the range (between 0.41 and 0.60) that some scholars (Landis and
Koch, 1977) contend provide a moderate level of agreement. The few
disagreements in coding that did arise were resolved by discussions
among the two coders.
We confined our analyses to the first-mentioned reason cited, as
the literature has shown that the "top-of-mind" response is
the one perceived to be the most important by a respondent (see, for
example, Miranda, Konya and Havrila, 2005; Lesser and Thumuluri, 2000),
and coded this element into the four attributional categories. Using
SPSS, crosstabulations were conducted to compare categorical variables,
with the significance of relationships determined using chi-square
analysis.
Success in Starting a Business
One approach in operationalizing a concept is to break it down into
different dimensions (Bryman, 1993). Returning to our general definition
provided earlier, we identify three dimensions as important in
meaningfully defining a "new firm": recency or newness, a form
of organization, and sales.
Here we consider a business recently formed (or new) if it became
operational within the previous 12 months, with newness being measured
at the individual level according to self-reports of the status of the
start-up and the reported date when operating status was achieved.
During each follow-up phone interview, the respondents were asked:
"How would you describe the current status of this start-up effort?
Is it now an operating business, still in an active start-up phase,
still a start-up but currently inactive, no longer being worked on by
anyone, or something else?" A categorical variable was used to
classify respondents according to whether their start-up was: operating,
active, inactive, no longer worked on by anyone, or something else.
In adopting any of three main organizational forms--proprietorship,
partnership, or incorportation--goods and services can be provided for
sale. Therefore, self-reports of any of these forms would constitute an
appropriate indicator of organizational form.
Sales of goods and/or services (Busenitz and Murphy, 1996) is one
of the many criteria used in previous studies to identify a new
operating firm. (For a critical assessment of these criteria see Carter,
Gartner and Reynolds, 2004). While sales is the one criterion that would
be an appropriate indicator of being operational for all firms, by
itself, this indicator is problematic. For example, Diochon, Menzies and
Gasse (2005b) found sales to be reported by those who have yet to
consider themselves operational. However, if an individual perceives his
or her start-up to be operational and reports sales of goods and
services, arguably, this would be a valid indicator of firm founding. In
sum, we contend that if a start-up exhibits all three dimensions
outlined above, then it has made the transition from becoming to being a
new business. As indicated earlier, we confined our analysis to those
starting and staying in business and those reporting abandoning their
efforts to start a business at the time of the 48-month follow-up.
Persistence after "Failure"
A categorical variable was used to measure entrepreneurial
persistence after "failure" to establish a new business.
Respondents who reported no longer working on their start-up were asked:
"Would you say that you expect to be involved in another
start-up?" Response categories include: "never again
(n=16)," "under the right conditions (n=28)," "most
certainly (n=13)."
Results
Attributions
Table 3 presents the coding results for the first mentioned
responses to the attributional questions. As predicted, an internal
stable attribution was the type of response most frequently provided in
explaining positive situations--why they are starting a business (44.4%)
and why they expect to be successful (35.4%). This evidence supports
Hypothesis 1.
In terms of how the two negative situations were described--current
problems and expected problems--both were most frequently ascribed
internal variable attributions (50.0% and 61.6% respectively).
Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was not supported. Although the next most
frequently cited main reason for current and expected problems was
external and variable, the evidence indicates that, overall, the sample
does not appear to be very susceptible to the self-serving bias. These
results contrast with those reported in Gartner and Shaver's (2002)
investigation of nascent entrepreneurs in the United States (US). In
their study the most frequently cited cause of problems was external and
variable, with internal and variable the next most-used category.
Interestingly, though, respondents from both studies attributed problems
to variable (external and internal) reasons.
A 4x4 crosstabulation to test for the self-serving bias produced
too many cells with expected values less than 5. Considering the
previous results, we deemed it appropriate to focus on examining the
locus of causality for the first mentioned reason for starting a
business by the first mentioned problem. Table 4 shows that internal
attributions were the most frequently offered first response to both
questions, with no significant relationship between the attributions:
[chi square] (df=1) =.425, [rho]=.515. In other words, there was no
difference between respondents' locus of causality for starting a
business and for the problems encountered. Generally, Canadian nascent
entrepreneurs are not very susceptible to the self-serving bias.
Instead, their attributions tend to demonstrate a distinctly internal
pattern. Indeed, the evidence suggests there may be a cultural
difference between Canadian and US entrepreneurs, whereby Canadians are
more likely to take personal responsibility for setbacks or failures.
Overall, the attributions reflect the sample's predilection to
look for internal causes for both positive and negative situations. For
the two positive situations, the vast majority of the sample provided
internal attributions for each (62.9% and 56.9% respectively). In
describing the two negative situations a similar pattern was noted
(61.3% and 72.6%). This same pattern was evident when the responses for
"why start a business" were crosstabulated with "problems
experienced." Respondents were almost twice as likely to attribute
the cause of both to internal factors (see Table 4). Based on this
evidence, it would appear that respondents' descriptions of
positive and negative situations are, to some extent, determined by the
cognitive structures of the respondents, as opposed to the data.
Attributions and Success in Firm Formation
A key objective of this article was to determine whether nascent
entrepreneurs' attributions were related to success in forming a
firm. Table 5 presents the results of analysis for testing whether
nascent entrepreneurs who were successful in starting a business were
less susceptible to the self-serving bias than nascent entrepreneurs who
have given up on their efforts to start a business. Among those who
successfully formed a firm too many cells had expected counts less than
5. Therefore, Hypothesis 3 could not be tested. However, it is
interesting to note that the data presented in Table 5 indicates more of
a susceptibility to the self-serving bias among those who were
successful in creating a firm than among those who were unsuccessful.
Indeed, among those who were successful in starting a business, 28.6%
offered an internal locus of causality in describing why they started
and an external locus of causality in describing problems. Among those
who had abandoned their efforts to start a business, 20.3% were
similarly categorized.
In light of the previous results, we explored whether success in
forming a firm was related to the locus of causality for any of the
first four attributional questions. Only one significant relationship
was found--between success and the first reason given for starting a
business (see Table 6). Nascent entrepreneurs who succeeded in starting
a business were more likely to have an internal locus of causality than
those who were unsuccessful in starting a firm. When describing their
first reason for starting a business, about 40% of those reporting an
internal locus of causality had succeeded in establishing a new business
while only about seven percent of those with an external locus of
causality succeeded in doing so. This suggests that personal reasons,
rather than external market opportunities, are the primary drivers of
decisions to create new ventures that eventually are successful. Tests
of whether a stable internal pattern for "why start," and a
variable internal pattern for problems, would be related to success in
starting showed no significance.
Among those who did not persist in their efforts to start a
business, Table 3 has shown that the most frequently provided response
was an internal stable attribution when asked what the most important
reason was for giving up [[chi square] (df=1) =3.872, [rho]=.049].
Consequently, Hypothesis 4--among nascent entrepreneurs who do not
persist in their efforts to start a business internal stable reasons
will be the least likely explanation of giving up--is not supported.
With 58% (n=33) providing internal reasons for giving up, clearly,
there was no evidence of a "self-serving" bias. Considering
this evidence in terms of the three requirements for task success
presented by Heider, the intention to start a business was present, as
was exertion in the direction of the intention. However, it would appear
that personal ability was not perceived to exceed the difficulty of the
task (starting a business).
In light of these results, it is somewhat surprising that the
prospect of starting another venture was just as appealing to
entrepreneurs who provided internal stable reasons for giving up as
those providing other reasons for abandoning their efforts to start a
business. Table 7 shows there were no statistically significant
differences in expectations for involvement in another venture, based on
the reasons provided for giving up. Therefore, Hypothesis 5 is not
supported. Contrary to theory, 72.7% (n=16) of Canadian nascent
entrepreneurs who attributed the cause of "failure" to
internal stable factors expected to be involved in another start-up.
What possible explanation would there be for this? Analysis of the
various answers in this response category sheds light on the issue by
revealing a number of themes. The most commonly cited reasons for giving
up on the start-up include: taking a job offer/promotion at work (four
individuals); health reasons (four individuals); sale of venture (three
individuals); change in their personal circumstances (one had a baby and
one moved to another town with their partner); and two said they found
it too difficult. These findings suggest that the vast majority of
respondents neither interpret giving up as a negative event nor as a
"failure," thereby helping to explain why they expect to be
involved in another start-up.
It is interesting to consider the results of this study in light of
two noted tendencies among entrepreneurs: perceiving their own
abilities, dedication, and efforts as crucial to success; and
overestimating the odds for success (Baron, 1998). While the first
tendency is evident, the second is less so. These Canadian nascent
entrepreneurs were less likely to provide internal attributions for
future success than they were for current and future problems (Table 3).
Discussion and Conclusion
This paper set out to explore whether individual differences in
attributional style are associated with later success in starting (and
staying) in business. Past research had shown that most people exhibit
cognitive biases in their thinking and that entrepreneurs are more
susceptible to the self-serving bias than others. In expecting to find a
similar pattern among our sample of Canadian nascent entrepreneurs, we
also expected less susceptibility to the self-serving bias among those
who were successful in establishing a business.
Instead of exhibiting a self-serving bias, these nascent
entrepreneurs were found to have an internal locus of causality when
interpreting situations. While positive situations were perceived to be
a result of their abilities (internal/stable), problems were perceived
to be a result of their effort (internal/variable). In terms of the
attributional framework, overcoming these problems would require greater
effort.
With success in establishing a new business neither related to
explanatory style nor to the utilization of a self-serving bias, these
findings indicate that attribution theory is of little use in explaining
why some people succeed in starting a business while others do not.
Although the reasons Canadian nascent entrepreneurs gave for entering
business were related to later success in establishing a new venture,
their other attributions were neither related to success nor to
entrepreneurial persistence.
There are several differences between our study and the research of
others that might explain our unanticipated results. First, ours is the
first real time study of nascent entrepreneurs' attributions that
are examined in relation to "success" in both starting and
staying in business. Second, our study is the first to explicitly define
a "new business." Third, our study's measures emerge from
the theoretical perspective taken. In using attribution theory as the
theoretical lens, the focus is on the meaning nascent entrepreneurs give
to their own activities/behaviour and start-up outcomes. As Gartner and
Shane (1995) attest, there are very few articles in the literature that
use measures of entrepreneurship that are theoretically derived.
Finally, ours is the first study that uses a nationally representative
sample to systematically study both those who succeeded in establishing
a new venture and those who did not. Typically, only those who succeed
are studied, as was the case when the attributions of
"successful" entrepreneurs were compared to those of managers
(rather than to those of people who did not "succeed" in
establishing a business).
Fundamentally, these findings have implications for theory
development. In particular they highlight the need to address the
meaning and measurement of key terms. Within this research a
sustainability dimension was incorporated into our definition of success
in new firm formation. After four years of follow-up, 22 people reported
starting and staying in business while 59 reported abandoning their
efforts to start a business. Of these 59, 22% (n=13) had achieved
operating status at one point in time and later gave up. Such findings
underscore the importance of sustainability to the development of a
better understanding of new firm formation.
For policymakers, the findings with respect to why ventures were
abandoned point to the importance of ensuring that the outcomes of
programs designed to encourage new firm formation are being validly
measured and appropriately evaluated. Moreover, could the fact that
Canadian nascent entrepreneurs are more inclined to take personal
responsibility for their actions help explain why the advice and
assistance of government agencies are generally known to be
underutilized? This is an issue for future research.
For prospective entrepreneurs, there is cause for optimism
concerning involvement in the start-up process. Indeed, although 59 of
the 81 nascent entrepreneurs we studied in this article gave up on their
start-up, most did not do so because they failed. The fact that the
majority of these individuals expected to be involved in another
start-up helps confirm that failure is being overstated. Furthermore,
prospective entrepreneurs would benefit from considering their reason(s)
for starting a business as these reasons appear to be related to
starting and staying in business.
Indeed, the unique attributional style of Canadian nascent
entrepreneurs merits further research, particularly on an international
comparative basis. Not only would it be useful to more thoroughly
investigate the role of cultural factors in the entrepreneurial process,
it would also be useful to determine whether attributions change,
subsequent to start-up.
No research is without limitations, and ours is no exception.
Clearly, the sample size and its attrition rate constrain the nature and
extent of analysis that could be conducted. For example, attributions
could not be assessed according to demographic factors. Furthermore, in
basing our conclusions about success on the fourth year follow-up data,
the sample became too small to conduct separate analyses for those who
started but did not stay in business as compared to those who never
started a business and those who started and stayed in business. In
future, research based on a larger sample is recommended.
Despite its weaknesses, this research does contribute to the
literature. Many scholars have argued that attribution style might be
useful in explaining the success of entrepreneurs. However, this is the
first empirical study to investigate the issue among a representative
sample of Canadian nascent entrepreneurs whose success in forming a new
business was determined according to whether they started and stayed in
business. Moreover, by assessing the reasons people gave for abandoning
their efforts to start a business, valuable insight is provided about
participants in the entrepreneurial process that we know very little
about.
Acknowledgements
The authors are most grateful for the generous financial support of
the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and
Industry Canada.
Contact Information
For further information on this article, contact
Dr. Monica Diochon, Schwartz School of Business Administration and
Information Systems, St. Francis Xavier University, Antigonish, Nova
Scotia, Canada B2G 2W5 Tel: 902 867-5412 email: mdiochon@stfx.ca
Dr. Teresa V. Menzies, Faculty of Business, Brock University, St.
Catharines, Ontario, Canada L2S 3A1 Tel: 905 688 5550 x 4118 email:
tmenzies@brocku.ca
Dr. Yvon Gasse, Faculte des sciences de l'administration,
Universite Laval, Sainte-Foy, PQ, Canada G1K 7P4 Tel: 418 656 7960
email: yvon.gasse@mng.ulaval.ca
References
Anderson, C.A. 1991. "How People Think About Causes:
Examination of the Typical Phenomenal Organization of Attributions for
Success and Failure," Social Cognition 9: 295-329.
Baron, R.A. 1998. "Cognitive Mechanisms in Entrepreneurship:
Why and When Entrepreneurs Think Differently Than Other People,"
Journal of Business Venturing 12: 275-94.
Beaver, G. 2003. "Small Business: Success and Failure,"
Strategic Change 12, no. 3: 115-22.
Busenitz, L.W. and G.B. Murphy. 1990. "New Evidence in the
Pursuit of Locating New Businesses," Journal of Business Venturing
11, no. 3: 221-31.
Cardon, M.S. and R.G. McGrath. 1999. "When the Going Gets
Tough ... Toward a Psychology of Entrepreneurial Failure and
Re-Motivation,"
http://www.babson.edu/entrep/fer/papers99/I/I_B/I_B%20Text.htm accessed
on July 16, 2004.
Carland, J.W., J.C. Carland and J.W. Carland, III. 1995. "A
Model of Entrepreneurship: The Process of Venture Creation,"
Retrived March 20, 1997, from
http://www.sbaer.uca.edu/docs/proceedings/95sbi013.txt
Carter, N.M., W.B. Gartner and P.D. Reynolds. 2004. "Firm
Founding." Pp 311-23 in W.B. Gartner, K.G. Shaver, N.M. Carter and
P.D. Reynolds (eds.), Handbook of Entrepreneurial Dynamics: The Process
of Business Creation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Diochon, M., T. Menzies and Y. Gasse. 2005a. "From Becoming To
Being: Measuring Firm Creation," paper presented at the Enterprise
Development Growth and Expansion (EDGE) Conference, Singapore, July
11-13.
Diochon, M. T. Menzies and Y. Gasse. 2005b. "Exploring the
Relationship Between Start-up Activities and New Venture Emergence: A
Longitudinal Study of Canadian Nascent Entrepreneurs,"
International Journal of Management and Enterprise Development 2, nos
3/4: 408-26.
Forbes, D.P. 2005. "Are Some Entrepreneurs More Overconfident
Than Others?," Journal of Business Venturing 20, no. 5: 623-40.
Ford, J.D. 1985. "The Effects of Causal Attributions on
Decision Makers' Responses to Performance Downturns," Academy
of Management Review 10: 770-86.
Gartner, W.B. and S.A. Shane. 1995. "Measuring
Entrepreneurship Over Time," Journal of Business Venturing 10, no.
4: 283-301.
Gartner, W.B. and K.G. Shaver. 2002. "The Attributional
Characteristics of Opportunities and Problems Described by Nascent
Entrepreneurs in the PSED," paper presented at the Babson-Kauffman
Entrepreneurship Research Conference, Boulder, CO.
Gartner, W.B., K.G. Shaver, N.M.Carter and P.D. Reynolds (eds.).
2004. Handbook of Entrepreneurial Dynamics: The Process of Business
Creation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gray, C. 1998. Enterprise and Culture. London: Routledge.
Heider, F. 1958. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New
York: Wiley.
Kahneman, D. and D. Lovallo. 1993. "Timid Choices and Bold
Forecasts: A Cognitive Perspective on Risk Taking," Management
Science 39: 17-31.
Kvalseth, T.O. 1989. "Note on Cohen's Kappa,"
Psychological Reports 65: 223-26.
Kelley, H.H. and J.L. Michela. 1980. "Attribution Theory and
Research," Annual Review of Psychology 31: 457-501.
Landis, J.R. and G.G. Koch. 1977. "The Measurement of Observer
Agreement for Categorical Data," Biometrics 33: 159-74.
Lesser, J.A. and L.K Thumuluri. 2000. "Beyond Research
Streams: Systematic Integration of Consumer Concepts. Twentieth Century
Postscript," Management Research News 23, nos. 5/6: 55-72.
Retrieved October 1, 2006, from ABI/INFORM Global database. (Document
ID: 290263931).
Miranda, M.J., L. Konya and I. Havrila. 2005. "Shoppers'
Satisfaction Levels Are Not the Only Key to Store Loyalty,"
Marketing Intelligence & Planning 23, nos. 2/3: 220-32.
Martocchio, M.J. and W.L. Gardner. 1987. "The Leader/Member
Attribution Process," Academy of Management Review 12: 235-49.
Miller, A.G. and T. Lawson. 1989. "The Effect of an
Informational Option on the Fundamental Attribution Error,"
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (June 1989) 194-204.
Nisbett, R.E., C. Caputa, P. Legant and J. Maracek. 1973.
"Behavior as Seen by the Actor and as Seen by the Observer,"
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 27: 154-64.
Palich, L.E., and D.R. Bagby. 1995. "Using Cognitive Theory to
Explain Entrepreneurship Risk-Taking: Challenging Conventional
Wisdom," Journal of Business Venturing 10, no. 6: 425-38.
Peterson, C. and P. Villanova. 1988. "An Expanded
Attributional Style Questionnaire," Journal of Abnormal Psychology
97, no. 1: 87-89.
Pittman, T.S. 1993. "Control Motivation and Attitude
Change." In G. Weary, F. Gleichner and K.L. Marsh (eds.), Control
Motivation and Social Cognition. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Reynolds, P.D. 2000. "National Panel Study of U. S. Business
Startups: Background and Methodology." Pp. 153-227 in J.A. Katz
(ed.), Advances in Entrepreneurship, Firm Emergence and Growth.
Stamford, CT: JAI Press Inc.
Rogoff, E.G., M-S. Lee and D-C Suh. 2004. "Who Done It?
Attributions by Entrepreneurs and Experts of the Factors that Cause and
Impede Small Business Success," Journal of Small Business
Management 42, no. 4: 364-76.
Robbins, S.P., M. Coulter and R. Stuart-Kotze. 2003. Management:
Canadian Seventh Edition. Toronto: Pearson Education Canada Inc.
Sapienza, H.J. 1991. "Comets and Duds: Characteristics
Distinguishing High- and Low-Performing High-Potential Ventures,"
Frontiers of Entrepreneurial Research 6: 124-38.
Schermerhorn, J.R. 2005. Management, 8th ed. New York: John Wiley
& Sons, Inc.
Shaver, K.G. 2004. "Attributions and Locus of Control."
Pp. 205-32 in W.B. Gartner, K.G. Shaver, N.M.Carter and P.D. Reynolds
(eds.), Handbook of Entrepreneurial Dynamics: The Process of Business
Creation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Shaver, K.G., W.B.Gartner, E. Crosby, K. Bakalarova, and
E.J.Gatewood. 2001. "Attributions About Entrepreneurship: A
Framework and Process for Analyzing Reasons for Starting a
Business," Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice 26, no. 2: 5-32.
Shaver, K.G., W.B.Gartner, E.J.Gatewood and L.H. Vos. 1996.
"Psychological Factors in Success at Getting into Business."
Pp. 77-90 in P. Reynolds, S. Birley, J.E. Butler, W.D. Bygrave, P.
Davidsson, W.B. Gartner and P.P. McDougall (eds.), Frontiers of
Entrepreneurship Research 1996. Babson Park, MA: Babson College.
http://www.babson.edu/entrep/fer/AWARDS/NFIB/html/nfib_paper.htm,
accessed on June 21, 2005.
Shaver, K.G. and L.R. Scott. 1991. "Person, Process, Choice:
The Psychology of New Venture Creation," Entrepreneurship Theory
and Practice 16, no. 2: 23-45.
Stokes, D. and R. Blackburn. 2002. "Learning the Hard Way: The
Lessons of Owner Managers Who Have Closed Their Businesses,"
Journal of Small Business and Enterprise Development 9, no. 1: 17-27.
Storey, D.J. 1994. Understanding the Small Business Sector. London:
Routledge.
Thomson, A. and C. Gray. 1999. "The Determinants of Management
Development in Small Business," Journal of Small Business and
Enterprise Development 6, no. 2: 113-27.
Thornhill, S. and R. Amit. 2003. "Learning from Failure:
Organizational Mortality and the Resource-Based View," Analytical
Studies Branch Research Paper Series. Ottawa, ON: Statistics Canada.
Wheelock, A., W. Haney and D. Bebell. 2000. "What Can Student
Drawings Tell Us About High-Stakes Testing in Massachusetts?"
TCRecord.org. Available:
http://www.tcrecord.org/Content.asp?ContentID=10634.
Weiner, B. 1985. "An Attributional Theory of Achievement
Motivation and Emotion," Psychological Review 92: 548-73.
--. 1986. An Attributional Theory of Motivation and Emotion. New
York: Springer-Verlag.
(1.) "Among the adults living your household, is there anyone
who, alone or with others, is now trying to start a new venture?"
"Will you be an owner, in part or in whole of this company or
venture that you are trying to launch, alone or with others for your own
business or that of your employer?" "During the last 12
months, have you done anything to help start this new business, such as
looking for equipment or a location, organizing a start-up team, working
on a business plan, saving money, or any other activity that would help
launch a business?"
Monica Diochon, Schwartz School of Business Administration and
Information Systems, St. Francis Xavier University, Antigonish, Nova
Scotia
Teresa V. Menzies, Faculty of Business, Brock University, St.
Catharines, Ontario
Yvon Gasse, Faculte des sciences de l'administration,
Universite Laval, Sainte-Foy, Quebec
Table 1. Intentional Behaviour: Dimensions of Causal Attributions
Stability of the Cause
Locus of Causality Stable Variable
Internal Ability Effort
External Task Difficulty Luck
Table 2. Cohen's Kappa Results for Independent Ratings of the
Three-Step Coding Process of Respondents' Explanations
Cohen's Kappa
Parsing Internal/ Stable/
Explanation of: Answers External Variable
Why business is being started .808 .766 .517
Why business is expected to be .792 .677 .691
successful
Problems experienced .942 .745 .623
Problems expected .908 .867 .701
Why gave up .791 .860 .862
Table 3. Coding Results for Attributions
First First
Average Elements Elements
Attributions Sample Number of Internal Internal
for: (n=81) Elements Stable Variable
Why start (n=81) N 1.75 36 15
% 44.4 18.5
Why expect N 1.85 28 17
success (n=79) % 35.4 21.5
Current problems N 1.39 9 40
(n=80) % 11.3 50.0
Expected problems N 1.37 8 45
(n=73) % 11.0 61.6
Why gave up N 1.41 22 11
(n=57) % 38.6 19.3
First First
Elements Elements
Attributions External External
for: Stable Variable
Why start (n=81) 13 17
16.0 21.0
Why expect 21 13
success (n=79) 26/6 16.5
Current problems 6 25
(n=80) 7.5 31.3
Expected problems 6 14
(n=73) 8.2 19.2
Why gave up 10 14
(n=57) 17.5 24.6
Table 4. Locus of Causality Attributions for Reasons for Starting a
Business by the Reasons for Problems
Problems
Why Start Internal External Total
N % N % N %
Internal 32 65.3 18 58.1 50 62.5
External 17 34.7 13 41.9 30 37.5
Total 49 100.0 31 100.0 80 100.0
[chi square] (df=1) =.425, [rho] =.515 n.s
Table 5. Locus of Causality Attributions for Reasons for Starting a
Business and Reasons for Problems by Firm Formation Status
Current Problems
Firm Formation Status Internal External
N % N %
Successful (a) Why Internal 13 92.9 6 85.7
(operating) Start? External 1 7.1 1 14.3
Total 14 100.0 7 100.0
Unsuccessful (b) Why Internal 19 54.3 12 50.0
(efforts abandoned) Start? External 16 45.7 12 50.0
Total 35 100.0 24 100.0
Current Problems
Firm Formation Status Total
N %
Successful (a) Why Internal 19 90.5
(operating) Start? External 2 9.5
Total 21 100.0
Unsuccessful (b) Why Internal 31 52.5
(efforts abandoned) Start? External 28 47.5
Total 59 100.0
(a.) too many cells had expected counts less than 5.
(b.) [chi square] (df=1) =.105 [rho] =.746 n.s.
Table 6. Firm Formation Success by Locus of Causality for Starting a
Business
Locus of Causality
Internal External Total
Formation Status N % N % N %
Successful (operating) 20 39.2 2 6.7 22 27.2
Unsuccessful 31 60.8 28 93.3 59 72.8
(efforts abandoned)
Total 51 100.0 30 100.0 81 100.0
[chi square] (df=1) =10.115, [rho] =.001
Table 7. Expectations for Future Start-up Involvement by
Reasons for Giving Up
Attributions
Internal
Stable All Other Total
Expectations for N % N % N %
Future Involvement
Never again 6 27.3 10 28.6 16 28.1
Under the right 11 50.0 17 48.6 28 49.1
conditions
Most certainly 5 22.7 8 22.8 13 22.8
Total 22 100.0 35 100.0 57 100.0
[chi square] (df=2) =.014, [rho] =.993 n.s.