Are positive trait attributions for the deceased caused by fear of supernatural punishments?: A triangulated study by content analysis and text mining.
Yu, Chong Ho
Why do we have religious beliefs? Cognitive psychologist Jesse
Bering attempted to answer this question by adopting a naturalistic
explanation. Specifically, according to Bering's psychological
theories, we are born with the innate idea that there is a spiritual
realm in which the deceased continue to exercise their will and other
cognitive functions. The belief that things do not end with this life
becomes the foundation of our moral order. Bering and his colleagues
conducted a series of studies to support this claim, such as the
experiment of puppet play (Bering, Blasi, & Bjorklund, 2005), the
experiment of trait attributions by photos, the content analysis of
obituaries, and the experiment of ghost story (Bering, McLeod, &
Shackelford, 2005). This project replicated and extended the study of
Bering et al.'s content analysis regarding the perceptions of dead
agents. Findings indicate that explaining religion by natural instinct
is questionable, and it is the conviction of the author that
interpretation should not go beyond what the data entail.
Utilizing the framework of cognitive psychology and evolutionary
psychology, Bering traced our tendency of believing in the supernatural
to instinct. More specifically, even though events in the universe are
random, we tend to find a pattern or purpose in these events. This
tendency was developed among our ancestors throughout the history of
evolution. Many people are helpless when facing unfortunate events, and
use adaptive or coping mechanisms to optimize negative outcomes that are
out of their control, such as putting their faith on an external agent
(e.g., God) (Bering, 2003, 2012; Bering & Johnson, 2005). This
inclination of seeing random events as designed for a purpose by God is
known as "teleological reasoning" (Bering, 2006, p.453), an
idea that resembles Kelemen and Rosset's (2009) notion of
promiscuous teleology. Moreover, Bering (2002a) hypothesized that humans
have a natural tendency to perceive cognitive systems as continuing to
function after death, and this disposition might be the psychological
foundation of religion. The underlying mechanism of this inclination is
called the "theory of mind" (Bering, 2006, p.253), which is a
research program that has been extensively studied by numerous
psychologists (e.g. Avis & Harris, 1991; Flavell, Flavell, &
Green, 1983; Gopnik & Astington, 1988; Richert & Barrett, 2005;
Wigger, 2011; Wigger, Paxson, & Ryan, 2012). In this view, humans
are said to be capable of attributing mental states to others even
though these mental states are not directly observable. Because humans
have a theory about how the mind works, people think they know what it
is like to be dead or how dead people think about us. Based on the
previous theory of mind, Bering (2002b) proposed the existential theory
of mind (EToM), which is "a biologically based, generic explanatory
system that allows individuals to perceive meaning in certain life
events" (p. 4).
In an attempt to empirically substantiate his claims, Bering et al.
(2005) conducted a content analysis by extracting trait attributions
from 496 obituaries published in the New York Times. The trait codes and
the categories were adopted from the Evaluation of Other Questionnaire
(EOOQ), as shown in Table 1 (Shapiro, 1988). The original EOOQ has four
categories (achievement-relatedness, social skills, subjective
well-being, and kindness/morality), but in Bering et al.'s (2005)
study, only three were included (achievement-relatedness, social skills,
and kindness/morality). In these obituaries, Bering and his team found
that pro-social (kindness) and morality-related attributes of the
deceased appeared more frequently than other types of qualities, such as
achievements and social skills. The significant difference between the
frequencies of different trait categories was confirmed by
one-dimensional Chi-square analysis ([X.sup.2] = 214.75; p < .001).
Bering and his associates suggested that the "ultimate cause"
in positive attributions to dead agents is a result of adaption that
could be traced back to our ancestors (p. 366, 371). These positive
attributions are linked to the fear of supernatural punishments.
Specifically, if dead agents are believed to be aware of what the living
people said and did, people would behave better, and eventually it could
strengthen the moral framework. However, this conclusion is questionable
in two aspects. First, if this phenomenon is due to our natural
inclination rather than cultural factors, it should be verified in a
cross-cultural context, but no replication in other cultural contexts
has been made. Second, even if the phenomenon is cross-cultural, is fear
of supernatural punishments the only reason that people say nice words
about the pro-social and morality-related traits of the deceased? To
investigate the first issue, this author replicated and extended the
same type of study in a different cultural setting, namely, the United
Kingdom. At the end, Bering's theoretical framework was unpacked
and counter-arguments against his theory were explained in detail.
The Naturalistic Paradigm
For the last several centuries numerous academic endeavors have
been made in an attempt to explain religion. Generally speaking, these
approaches could be summarized into two categories: naturalistic
explanations and cultural explanations. The works of Bering, Bloom
(2004), Boyer (1994, 2001), Guthrie (1993), and Barrett (2004) can be
classified as the naturalistic approach, though there are disagreements
among these authors. In Barrett's (2004) view, the human cognitive
structure carries certain hardwired dispositions that lead us to embrace
supernatural beliefs. Specifically, because our ancestors who lived in a
dangerous world had to be highly alert constantly, they may have
misperceived many ordinary things as a signal of threat. Nevertheless,
it is better to make a Type I error (false alarm) than a Type II error
(miss a real threat). For example, it is better for a hunter to mistake
a rock for a tiger than to mistake a tiger for a rock. This
psychological inclination, which is called hyperactive agency detection
device (HADD), makes us receptive to imagined and invisible agents. For
instance, if a flood destroys a village, it is better to assume that the
river spirit is angry and take action, such as offering sacrifices to
the river spirit than assuming otherwise and remaining still. Barrett
(2004) suggested that religious concepts and practices, which emerged
from communities, strengthened moral and social order, and equipped
religious people with survival advantages over non-religious people.
Following the naturalistic approach, Bloom (2004) contended that
humans are prone to be dualists because mind-body dualism is compatible
with common sense. For instance, while children accept the brain as
responsible for some aspects of mental life, such as solving math
problems, they simultaneously deny the brain having something to do with
loving one's brother. It is natural for humans to postulate that
there is some entity beyond the body, and therefore people are receptive
to supernatural beliefs. In a similar vein, Boyer (2001) asserted that
our minds are well-prepared for religion due to natural selection.
Religious people perceive their god in anthropomorphic terms. In other
words, humans tend to conceptualize a god that is in many aspects like
us; but this deity is much more powerful than humans. We have intuitions
about what gods should look like, which religious concepts are good, and
we project our images onto the supernatural world. This cognitive
preparedness for religion cannot be exhaustively explained by cultural
diffusion.
The Cultural Paradigm
The cultural and sociological approaches can be traced back to
Freud (1927/1975; 1929/1962) and Marx (1844/1977). According to Freud,
religion is a mass neurosis resulting from our interaction with a
stressful environment. In order to alleviate our anxiety, we project the
father figure onto the image of God. In Marx's view, religion is a
response to social and economic oppression, and the ruling class uses
religion to justify the status quo. Although apparently both the
naturalistic and cultural groups are similar to each other in the sense
that religion is said to result from human-environment interaction, the
assumptions of the two groups are vastly different. In the naturalistic
perspective, religion can be subscribed by any people in any culture
because the tendency is internally driven. On the contrary, in the
Freudian and Marxist views, when the psychological, social, and cultural
conditions that make us vulnerable to superstition are no longer
present, people will not see a need for religion. Some modern authors
went even further to claim that religion is a form of cultural virus
spreading from mind to mind and from culture to culture (e.g. Craig,
2010; Dawkins, 1990, 2008; Dennett, 2007; Ray, 2009).
In a similar vein to Freud and Marx, these modern authors believe
that when our culture is improved by scientific reasoning and citizens
are endowed with better education, religion will be debunked and lose
its attractiveness. Based on his studies regarding secularization in
Europe and apostasy in the US, Zuckerman (2008, 2011) argued that while
people could be programmed to embrace religious beliefs, they could also
be deprogrammed to walk away from religion. When European societies
(e.g. Denmark and Sweden) are highly developed and people have a high
level of well-being, they no longer value religion. While the preceding
approaches emphasized the cultural conditions that breed or deter
religious ideas, other cultural approaches focus on the dynamic or
evolutionary aspect of religion. For example, equipped with training in
theology and church history, Armstrong (1994) illustrated how the
concept of God, as a reflection of cultural traits, changed throughout
history. According to Armstrong, each generation comes up with a
different set of ideas about God, based upon the need, fear, vision, and
emotion of the culture. Similarly, utilizing resources from archeology,
history, and evolutionary psychology, Wright (2009) argued how the idea
of God evolved over time due to the necessity of particular cultural
contexts. Interestingly, empirical studies seem to support the cultural
approach to religion. For example, Nelsen and Kroliczak (1984) found a
decline in Americans' belief in a punishing God, and this coincides
with a decline of parenting by using the "God will punish you"
approach. Further, Granqvist and Kirkpatrick (2004) suggested that
people's attachment to a supernatural power is, to some extent,
parallel to people's interpersonal relationships. Empirical studies
found that people who lack a firm attachment to their parents tend to
believe in a loving god. This explanation is known as the compensation
model (Hood, Hill, & Spilka, 2009). In other words, our perception
of God, to some degree, depends on parenting and other cultural factors.
Combining the Naturalistic and Cultural Approaches
In recent years, some researchers have merged the naturalistic and
cultural approaches together. For example, the Cultural Evolution of
Religion Consortium (Centre for Human Evolution, Cognition, and Culture,
2013) adopted gene-cultural co-evolution as the theoretical starting
point. Their research endeavors are devoted to researching questions
surrounding whether religious beliefs derived from an over-projection of
theory of mind, as well as how religion and pro-social behaviors, such
as honesty and fairness, are related. It seems to be a promising
research agenda because this author does not view natural dispositions
and cultural influences as mutually exclusive. Like learning math,
science, music, art, and many other things, religious ideas can be
better-understood in the light of nature-nurture interactions.
Explaining "Away" Religion
Bering et al's (2005) research agenda is built upon and
coincides with many other existing naturalistic theories of religion,
such as teleological reasoning, common-sense dualism, and the theory of
mind. Nonetheless, Bering (2003) regarded the explanation of religion by
hyperactive agency detection device as insufficient because this
so-called natural tendency is still environmentally-induced. In his
view, the majority of people turn to religion because of subjective
negative experiences, not objective events in a hostile environment. It
is clear that Bering rejected the environmental or cultural approach,
and his highly naturalistic theory of religion can be epitomized by the
following passage:
Default inferences that are typically
associated with religious thinking
(e.g., belief in the continuity of personal
consciousness after death;
belief in an abstract intentional agency
as the arbiter of life events, and
the creator of species and natural
inanimates) are not activated by culturally
transmitted religious concepts,
but instead give rise to religious concepts
themselves. (p. 245) [emphasis
in the original]
More importantly, to Bering the finding of our natural tendency
towards supernatural beliefs can explain "away" religion. The
pre-supposition that the belief of God is nothing more than an illusion
is explicitly expressed in the following passage (Bering, 2012):
Nature may have had a few tricks up
her sleeve to ensure that we would
fall hook, line, and sinker for these
spectacular ruses. Ultimately, of
course, you must decide for yourself
whether the subjective psychological
effects created by your evolved cognitive
biases reflect an objective reality,
perhaps as evidence that God
designed your mind to be so receptive
to Him. Or, just maybe, you will
come to acknowledge that, like the
rest of us, you are a hopeless pawn
in one of natural selection's most successful
hoaxes ever--and smile at the
sheer ingenuity involved in pulling it
off, at the very thought of such mindless
cleverness. One can still enjoy
the illusion of God, after all, without
believing Him to be real. (p. 8)
In an interview Bering voiced his objection towards people changing
their mind about their belief of God, such as a death-bed conversion,
which Evangelical Christians utilize to advocate their course. According
to Bering, death-bed conversions may simply be a psychological illusion,
which is irrelevant to the objective reality (Mulvey, 2012). This line
of reasoning is questionable because although one's subjective
feeling may or may not reflect the objective reality, it does not
necessarily imply that the perception must be an illusion. For example,
when I perceive that my wife loves me, it is possible that I may be
right or may be wrong. I can construct a psychological explanation of my
feelings based on Maslow's humanistic psychology. However, even if
there is a convincing psychological theory to explain my tendency of
looking for love, it still cannot lead to a firm conclusion that my wife
does not love me at all. To be blunt, religion as a result of cognitive
bias is a pre-supposition, not a logical necessity deduced from sound
reasoning or substantiated by empirical data. Examining Bering's
interpretive framework requires another article in its own right. In
this article the author intends to focus on Bering's study of
obituaries.
Methodology
Bering et al.'s (2005) study on trait attributions in
obituaries has certain limitations. First, the sample was obtained from
a relatively religious country (USA). About 37% of Americans attend
church on a regular basis (Pew Research Center, 2012). Moreover, a
Gallup poll (2011) shows that 92% of Americans believe in the existence
of God. An earlier study indicates that only 6% of Americans considered
themselves "atheists" (Norris & Inglehart, 2004). Thus,
the wording patterns found in the New York Times ' obituaries and
the idea of afterlife among the Americans can be a cultural product,
instead of a natural tendency. Another shortcoming of Bering et
al.'s (2005) study is the use of pre-built trait categories only.
This approach is not completely data-driven and the coders were guided
by certain preconceptions. Although a high inter-rater reliability was
reported in their study, only one single method, content analysis, was
used. Put bluntly, there was no triangulation. In the following section,
the author will discuss what remedies were employed to improve this
study. Third, Bering et al's (2005) data were sourced from one
newspaper only, and thus there is a risk of sampling bias. Last, the
result of one-dimensional Chi-square test was reported in Bering et
al.'s (2005) study, but it lacks the detail of the composition of
the three categories in EOOQ. If the frequencies of the trait codes in
the three categories are uneven (e.g., more codes about kindness and
morality than those of achievement), the Chi-square test result merely
reflects the unfair starting points.
Population and Sample
Another sample is needed in order to further examine Bering et
al.'s (2005) notion. In contrast to the United States, churchgoers
in the United Kingdom are rare. According to the British Social
Attitudes Survey conducted by NatCen Social Research (2011), 56% of
Britons who belong to or were brought up in a religious household never
attend religious services or meetings. Among these people, only 14%
attend church weekly. The statistical report compiled by
Archbishops' Council (2012) shows a much lower figure when the
entire population is taken into consideration: only 1.75 percent (1.1
million out of 62.47 million) of British people attend church on a
weekly basis. Secularization is by no means a new phenomenon in the UK.
A 2004 study conducted by Norris and Inglehart (2004) reveals that 39%
British do not believe in God. An independent report released by BBC
(2004) in the same year came to a similar conclusion: 44% British are
non-believers. Based on statistics and other evidence, Berger, Davie,
and Fokas (2008) described the characteristic of America as
"religious" and that of Europe, including the UK, as
"secular." They illustrated this discrepancy by sharing
Berger's own experience in England: When he was traveling in
London, he asked the hotel concierge for the direction to an Anglican
Church. Surprisingly, the receptionist mistakenly thought that the
Church of England is the Roman Catholic Church. Based on extensive
interviews with children in the UK, Hay (2006) argued that children
raised in the UK still have a high degree of spirituality. Does this
imply that after all the US and the UK are not vastly different in terms
of spirituality and religiosity? It is crucial to point out that Hay
subscribes to a universal position of spirituality, which means that
children across all cultures have the capability of actualizing their
potential spirituality. However, cultural pressures and the decline in
institutional religion suppress their pursuit of spiritual lives; this
is what had happened in the UK.
If the perception of active dead agents is really natural or
a-cultural, then the trait attributions found in the US sample should
also be observed in the UK. In Bering's study all the observations
were selected from one source: the New York Times. In the current study
obituaries published in two UK newspapers, namely, Guardian and
Independent, served as the accessible population. The reason for using
two data sources instead of one is obvious: counting on one single data
source might result in a sampling bias. As a countermeasure, in this
project, 339 obituaries printed in February 2013 were copied from these
two sources (Guardian = 298, Independent = 41). Because these UK
newspapers occasionally print obituaries from other countries, the
following inclusion criteria were adopted: The deceased had to be born
or live in the United Kingdom. During the screening process it was found
that Independent printed more obituaries of non-British people than
Guardian, and as a result, the numbers of observations from the two
sources are asymmetrical. In this sample, 257 decedent people were males
and 82 were females.
Data Analysis
As mentioned before, two of the shortcomings of Bering et
al.'s study are the use of pre-built trait categories only, the use
of a single approach, and the use of a single data source. As a remedy,
two types of qualitative data analysis were utilized in this project:
content analysis based on EOOQ using MAXQDA (VERBI GmbH, 2013) and text
mining without pre-built categories implemented in SPSS Text Analysis
(IBM SPSS Inc., 2011). In addition to using EOOQ, the human coders
generated new attributes when the meaning of the passage could not fit
into any pre-existing codes in EOOQ.
It is important to point out that text mining does not merely count
the frequency of recurring words. Rather, text mining utilizes
computational linguistics, artificial intelligence, and natural language
processing to build categories based on the context of the words and
phrases (Mehler & Kohler, 2007; Singhal, 2001; Yu, Jannasch-Pennell,
& DiGangi, 2011). One of the major advantages of text mining is that
the algorithm always yields consistent results whereas the quality of
content analysis via manual coding is affected by human factors, such as
order effect, subjective judgment, and fatigue. Furthermore, data-driven
text mining is fully compatible with the exploratory character of
qualitative research. However, although the initial category building is
automated, this does not mean that the researcher hands over the entire
judgment to the computer program; rather, the researcher may rename or
regroup the categories if the modification can yield a more meaningful
set of categories. In other words, human judgment, whether it is
exercised in content analysis or text mining, is still indispensable in
qualitative research. Both approaches can complement each other; this
type of "method triangulation" enables the researcher to see
whether or not the results can converge into the same conclusion.
Although Bering employed one-dimensional Chi-square analysis in his
study, it is argued here that Chi-square analysis is inappropriate to
the data set collected in the current study, because among the 38 items
listed in the original EOOQ, 26 of them fall into the category of
"kindness/morality" while only 3 items belong to
"achievement-relatedness." The composition of EOOQ is detailed
in Table 2. This imbalance in the composition of the trait codes would
give kindness and morality an advantage if the Chi-square test by
category is used. To rectify the situation, the descriptive statistics
of individually coded variables in EOOQ was reported. Only the codes
generated by text mining were analyzed by Chi-square tests in order to
determine whether or not the frequency of certain categories
significantly outnumbers others.
Results
The obituaries were imported into MAXQDA for content analysis. No
negative attributes of EOOQ, such as "hypocritical" and
"selfish" were found in the obituaries, and thus these
categories are not considered useful. Table 3 shows the new codes
created by the human coders when the meaning of the passage did not fit
into any items in EOOQ. The new codes were classified according to the
EOOQ categories. After additional trait attributes were discovered and
appended into the original EOOQ table, it was found that traits related
to kindness and morality are still dominant (see Table 4).
Of the 2,012 traits that were extracted from the data, 312 belonged
to the category of achievement-relatedness (14.84%), 1,427 belonged to
kindness/morality (67.89%), 249 belonged to social skills (11.85%), and
114 made reference to subject well-being (5.42%). However, as mentioned
before, there are more codes in kindness/morality than other categories,
and thus this coding result may be a product of uneven distributions of
codes. When the traits were considered individually, a different story
emerged. Table 5 displays the top 20 most frequently recurring codes
extracted from the data. The top three belong to achievement-relatedness
(accomplished, talented, intelligent), with an additional two also from
this category (hard-working and creative). Three belong to
kindness/morality (helpful, kind, and loving) and one belongs to social
skills (good sense of humor).
The matrix in Table 6 displays the inter-relationships among the
most common co-occurring codes. Figure 1 is a visual presentation of the
inter-relationships of the codes. Both the matrix and the chart were
generated by MAXQDA. According to the table and the code relation chart,
"accomplished" and "talented" co-occurred most often
whereas "intelligent", "hard-working", and
"helpful" also seem to be strongly associated with
"accomplished."
Based on the context of 1,142 extracted words and phrases, SPSS
Text Analytics identified ten common themes or categories as shown in
Table 7. Although not all are directly related to the characters of the
deceased, these themes or categories can still reflect personal
attributes of the dead. For example, many passages in the obituaries
focused on academic achievement and employment. In the eyes of the
obituaries' authors, these are important elements that can define
their decedent relatives. The common themes recognized by text mining
overwhelmingly concentrate on the domain of achievement. A Chi-square
analysis between the frequencies of trait categories yields a
significant difference (X= 399.4, p < .0001). The category web
generated by SPSS, as shown in Figure 2, is equivalent to the code
relation chart created by MAXQDA. The category web depicts that there
are strong associations among the traits "academic,"
"employment," and "artistic."
Discussion
This replicated study zooms into only a small portion of
Bering's psychology of religion. Without an overview one may wonder
why this author devoted tremendous efforts into a triangulated
mixed-method study to examine a seemingly trivial issue: How did people
write obituaries? To show the significance of the current research
problem and how it fits into the overall agenda of Bering's
psychology of religion, in this section his notions will be scrutinized
through a wide-angle lens at both the theoretical and empirical levels.
Issues at the Theoretical Level
One of the theoretical issues of Bering's research program is
that it builds upon certain strong but unsupported assumptions. Bering
and his research team assume that events in the world are random and
purposeless, but we try to extract existential meanings out of the
random occurrences in the natural world by imaging an intrusive and
authoritarian eye in the sky (Bering & Johnson, 2005). However, how
can we know that events in the universe are truly random and aimless?
Take judging whether a string of digits is random or not as an example.
At first glance this sequence is random: 100111100110111001101.
Nevertheless, one can observe a recurring pattern after seeing the
subsequent numbers: 1001111001101110011011001111001101110011011
00111100110111001101. Chaitin (1975) found that any attempt to decide
the randomness of a sufficiently long binary string is inherently doomed
to fail. We misjudge events as random because we can only examine finite
data in the short run. Length matters!
Furthermore, it is common for researchers to use a 5-point or
7-point Likert scale for measuring attitudes. When we try to detect a
pattern or relationship between two single survey items based on a
Likert scale, the regression model, as shown in Figure 3, suggests that
the data points are nothing more than random noise. Nevertheless, when
variables are put together to form two composite scores, a pattern
emerges, as depicted by Figure 4. Again, it is one of our cognitive
deficiencies that we tend to jump into the conclusion based on a small
amount or partial data (Kahneman, 2011). Size matters.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
The Central Limit Theorem (CLT) is another intriguing phenomenon in
mathematics. No matter how messy or noisy the underlying population is,
the end result of repeated sampling is always a sampling distribution
with a normal pattern. Pascal Triangle, also known as Quincunx, further
substantiates the orderliness of world events. Even if balls are
randomly poured into a Pascal Triangle, this stochastic process always
produces a fairly normal distribution. Are events in the world purely
random and so-called structure is imposed by our wishful thinking or
false belief? By far all major mathematical theorems suggest the
otherwise. One may argue that these mathematical phenomena are not the
same as human affairs. It is important to point out that certain
mathematical phenomena can be actualized by statistical data. Take the
preceding regression model as an example again. One can collect real
data to verify the pattern. Instead of using simulated populations in
the Central Limit Theorem, one can perform repeated sampling using real
populations, and it is guaranteed that the results would be the same. In
short, the initial assumption that events are random is an unsupported
claim (Yu, 2014).
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
Bering denies the pattern or organization out there; rather, in his
view this perceived pattern arises within our mind as a result of our
internal organizing algorithms. Bering and Shackelford (2004) argued
that certain Darwinian algorithms are grounded in human conscious
motives, which have inserted themselves at the level of the gene. In
discussing supernatural beliefs as an evolutionary byproduct, again
Bering (2006) wrote, "Perhaps human minds have a genetic
predisposition toward supernatural belief" (p.142). However, is
there any biological evidence substantiating these claims? What types of
gene carrying such motives and beliefs had been discovered? Bering and
his associates did not provide the readers with any detail, but the
devil is in the detail. This "leap of faith" is pervasive in
the research enterprise of evolutionary psychology. The genetic and
biological causal mechanism of human mind is at most a metaphor. It is a
useful fiction or a convenient model that the human mind is compared to
a computer or a machine running "Darwinian algorithms," but
there is no direct evidence that this model is mapping to the objective
reality (Gantt, Melling, & Reber, 2012).
In response to Bering's claim that religious ideas are innate,
Pyysiainen (2003) poised an explicit challenge by saying, "I
don't think it is possible to take religiosity to be innate in the
sense that religious concepts qua religious concepts were genetically
determined ... religion can be no more innate than football or
politics" (p. 219). One may find biological evidence that humans
like sports and politics, but this disposition does not necessarily lead
to such activities. In a similar vein, Barrett (2011) found terms like
innate and hard-wired problematic because virtually everything we do has
a biological base. It is strange to regard some of the cognitive
functions as part of our brain circuitry whereas some are not. Put
simply, every cognitive activity is hardwired. Therefore, besides
religious concepts, the notion of innateness can be well applied to
almost all situations. Why do we like music? It is an innate idea
resulted from evolutionary adaption. Why do we enjoy sex? It comes from
internal compulsion so that humans can enhance their survival fitness.
Why do girls like certain colors? It is innate, too. When a theory can
explain almost everything, it explains nothing.
There are other weaknesses in Bering's theory in terms of
explanation. In comparing the naturalistic and cultural approaches to
psychology of religion, Barrett (2003) pointed out that the cultural
approach can evade difficult questions regarding where the idea of
supernatural fear came from. However, Bering's naturalistic model
must bite the bullet and face the question. If this is an innate idea,
how could it arise in the first place? Did the concept of active dead
agent result from an interaction with a ghost or from a hallucination?
Bering (2002a) offered this explanation: "Ghosts come from
within" (p. 293) in the way that we project human qualities to the
image of ghosts. Simply put, ghost and other religious concepts are
intuitive. In all fairness toward the arguments presented by Bering,
sometimes intuition could be accepted as a legitimate answer to certain
mental phenomena. For example, at the present time we still do not fully
understand the origin of mathematical concepts. According to Godel,
"Despite their remoteness from sense experience, we do have
something like a perception also of the objects of set theory, as is
seen from the fact that the axioms force themselves upon us as being
true. I don't see any reason why we should have any less confidence
in this kind of perception, i.e. in mathematical intuition, than in
sense perception" (cited in Lindstrom, 2000, p. 123; Yu, 2004).
However, there is a subtle difference between mathematical intuitionism
and Bering's intuitionism. Respectable mathematicians never use
intuition or innateness to assert that mathematical concepts are false
beliefs! On the contrary, Penrose (1989) admired the internal structure
and beauty of mathematical concepts. After reviewing the Cauchy integral
formula, the Riemann mapping theorem, and the Lewy extension property,
Penrose proposed that the beauty and structure of mathematics might have
a divine origin.
In the discussion of supernatural agents, Bering and Johnson (2005)
maintained that making supernatural causal inferences may be adaptive
because it enabled our ancestors to control events through
"implanting false beliefs, repairing false beliefs, manipulating
emotion" (p.119) ... etc. It is obvious that the control mechanism
is stated in a negative term and a judgmental tone (e.g. "false
belief" and "manipulating emotions"). Is there conclusive
evidence that all supernatural or religious beliefs are false? Since
neutrality is a common protocol widely adopted by psychologists, it is
odd to see such language used in his scholarly writing. Additionally, on
one hand Bering (2006) recognized the adaptive function of supernatural
fear, such as counteracting bold and dangerous miscalculations,
discouraging people from social deviance ... etc., but on the other hand
he insisted that religion is nothing more than an "accidental
byproduct," (p.143) "a spandrel or an exaptation"
(p.146). If a computer algorithm can effectively prevent my computer
from virus infection and enhance the system efficiency, but I insist
that the software module is just an advantageous by-product, would any
reasonable person suspect that I have a strong bias against the computer
programmer or the software company? The usage "by-product,"
"spandrel," or "exaptation" implies that religious
or supernatural beliefs must be false and therefore the benefit resulted
from a false religion is nothing but an unintended, happy accident.
Last but not least, the notion that believing in God emerges from
natural mechanisms is self-defeating. If belief in God is misguided by
many natural tendencies, including teleological reasoning, mind-body
dualism, perception of active dead agents, and so on, how did Bering and
other psychologists overcome these overwhelmingly natural dispositions?
Did they have another set of natural tendencies that made them immune to
this type of psychological illusion? How did the New Atheism movement
gain popularity in the UK? How did Great Britain, once a Christian
nation, turn into a secular state and lose a majority of their church
members within a few decades? Did the cognitive structures of the
British people gradually transform within half a century? In short, it
is premature to affirm that a naturalistic explanation is sufficient to
explain or explain away religion.
Issues at the Empirical Level
Although Bering's research team conducted experiments and
utilized quantitative data in their investigation, it is doubtful
whether their data can adequately support their claims. Occasionally
Bering and his colleagues cited certain cases in an attempt to support
the notion of supernatural fear as our moral foundation. For example, in
1970 Katherine Power killed a security guard in a bank and got away. In
1993 she turned herself in to the police because she had a
"contract with God" (cited in Bering & Johnson, 2005,
p.131). However, how many criminals are like Katherine Power? What is
the percentage of fugitives that are willing to turn themselves in to
the authorities unconditionally? If many felons, driven by fear of
supernatural judgment, would eventually walk into the prison, then we
would not need the FBI, DEA, and ATF. In the perspective of statistics
and probability, the logical conclusion should be that the majority of
people are not afraid of supernatural punishment!
This study was triangulated by analyses performed in two software
packages (MAXQDA and SPSS Text Analytics) in two different modes:
content analysis by human coders and text mining by algorithms. However,
the results converge into a coherent conclusion. The content-analyzed
data shows that the most frequent recurring traits are those related to
achievements. Similarly, text mining reveals that the common themes
emphasized by the authors of the obituaries were those linked to
achievements (e.g. academic, employment). Therefore, this study casts
doubt on Bering et al.'s (2005) claim on the perception of dead
agents. In order to further investigate whether the pattern of positive
attributions for dead agents is natural or cultural, this author planned
to replicate the same type of study in different cultures, especially
societies that are more secular than the US.
As mentioned before, Bering and his colleagues suggested that the
positive trait attributions of the dead agents are motivated by fear of
supernatural punishment. However, this is mere interpretation. The data
collected by Bering et al. (2005) does not necessarily lead to this
interpretation. Bering et al. (2005) made the same type of assumption in
the study of trait attributions by photos. In this study, participants
were asked to rate the traits of three strangers displayed in photos. A
week later they returned to continue the study but were told that one of
the persons had died over the weekend. Afterwards, on average,
participants rated the dead person more favorably than others, and again
this result was interpreted by fear of supernatural punishment. However,
there are alternate explanations to this outcome. For example, the works
of a deceased artist may become more valuable because this artist can no
longer produce any more paintings. Similarly, we may say nice things
about the dead out of sympathy. However, data collected in the current
study cannot allow us to identify the cause of positive attributions,
either. Thus, this author and his research team planned to replicate the
study of trait attributions by photos with a different sample and with
one additional component in the research design: ask the participants
why they gave higher ratings to the dead person. It is maintained here
that direct evidence is more credible than indirect inference.
References
Archbishops' Council. (2012). Church Statistics 2010/11:
Parochial attendance, membership and finance statistics together with
statistics of licensed ministers for the Church of England, January to
December. Retrieved from
http://www.churchofengland.org/media/1477827/2010_11church-statistics.pdf
Armstrong, K. (1994). A history of God: The 4,000-year quest of
Judaism, Christianity and Islam. New York, NY: Ballantine Books.
Avis, J., & Harris, P. L. (1991). Belief-desire reasoning among
Baka children: Evidence for a universal conception of mind. Child
Development, 62, 460-467.
Barrett, J. L. (2003). Epidemiological and nativist accounts in the
cognitive study of culture: A commentary on Pyysiainen's innate
fear of Bering's ghosts. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3,
226-232.
Barrett, J. L. (2004). Why would anyone believe in God? Lanham, MD:
AltaMira Press.
Barrett, J. L. (2011). Cognitive science, religion, and theology:
From human minds to divine minds. West Conshohocken, PA: Templeton
Press.
Berger, P., Davie, G., & Fokas, E. (2008). Religious America,
secular Europe? : A theme and variation. Burlington, VT : Ashgate.
Bering, J. M. (2002a). Intuitive conceptions of dead agents'
minds: The natural foundations of afterlife beliefs as phenomenological
boundary. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 2, 263-308.
Bering, J. M. (2002b). The existential theory of mind. Review of
General Psychology, 6, 3-24.
Bering, J. M. (2003). Religious concepts are probably epiphenomena:
A reply to Pyysiainen, Boyer, and Barrett. Journal of Cognition and
Culture, 3, 244-254.
Bering, J. M. (2006). The cognitive psychology of belief in the
supernatural: Belief in a deity or an afterlife could be an
evolutionarily advantageous by-product of people's ability to
reason about the minds of others. American Scientist, 94(2), 142149.
Bering, J. M. (2006). The folk psychology of souls. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences, 29, 453-498.
Bering, J. M. (2012). The belief instinct: The psychology of souls,
destiny, and the meaning of life. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.
Bering, J. M, Blasi, C. H., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). The
development of 'afterlife' beliefs in religiously and
secularly schooled children. British Journal of Developmental
Psychology, 23, 587-607.
Bering, J. M., & Johnson, D. (2005). "O Lord...you
perceive my thoughts from afar": Recursiveness and the evolution of
supernatural agency. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 118-142.
Bering, J. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2004). The causal role of
consciousness: A conceptual addendum to human evolutionary psychology.
Review of General Psychology, 8, 227-248.
Bering, J. M., McLeod, K., & Shackelford, T. K (2005).
Reasoning about dead agents reveals possible adaptive trends. Human
Nature, 16, 360-381.
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes' baby: How the science of child
development explains what makes us human. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Boyer, P. (1994). The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive
theory of religion. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of
religious thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
British Broadcasting System (BBC). (2004). What the world thinks of
God. Retrieved from http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/shared/spl/hi/programmes/wtwtgod/pdf/wtwtogod.pdf
Centre for Human Evolution, Cognition, and Culture (2013). Cultural
Evolution of Religion Consortium. Retrieved from http://www.hecc.ubc.ca/
Chaitin, G. J. (1975). Randomness and mathematical proof.
Scientific American, 232(5), 47-52.
Craig, J. (2010). Religious virus. Hants, UK: John Hunt Publishing.
Dawkins, R. (1990). The selfish gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Dawkins, R. (2008). The God delusion. New York, NY: Mariner Books.
Dennett, D. (2007). Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural
phenomenon. Penguin Books.
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L. (1983).
Development of the appearance-reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology,
15, 95-120.
Freud, S. (1927/1975). The future of an illusion. New York, NY: W.
W. Norton.
Freud, S. (1929/1962). Civilization and its discontents. New York,
NY: W. W. Norton.
Gallup. (2011). More Than 9 in 10 Americans Continue to Believe in
God. Retrieved from http://www.gallup.com/poll/147887/americans-continue-believe-god.aspx
Gantt, E. E., Melling, B. S., & Reber, J. S. (2012). Mechanisms
or metaphors? The emptiness of evolutionary psychological explanations.
Theory & Psychology, 22, 823-841.
Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's
understanding of representational change, and its relation to the
understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction.
Child Development, 59, 26-37.
Granqvist, P., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2004). Religious
conversion and perceived childhood attachment: A metaanalysis.
International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 14, 223-250.
doi:10.1207/s15327582ijpr1404_1.
Guthrie, S. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hay, D. (2006). The spirit of the child. Philadelphia, PA: Jessica
Kingsley Publishers.
Hood, R. W., Hill, P. C., & Spilka, B. (2009). The psychology
of religion: An empirical approach(4th ed.). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
IBM SPSS Inc. (2011). IBM SPSS text analytics for survey version 4
[Computer software]. Armonk, NY: Author.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar,
Straus and Giroux.
Kelemen, K., & Rosset, E. (2009). The human function
compunction: Teleological explanation in adults. Cognition, 111,
138-143.
Lindstrom, P. (2000). Quasi-realism in mathematics. Monist, 83,
122-149.
Marx, K. (1844/1977). Critique of Hegel's 'philosophy of
right1. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Mehler, A., & Kohler, R. (2007). Aspects of automatic text
analysis. Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Mulvey, S. (2012, August). Jesse Bering interview. Ask an Atheist.
Retrieved from http://askanatheist.tv/2012/08/18/jessie-bering-interview/
NatCen Social Research. (2011). The British Social Attitudes
Survey. Retrieved from
http://ir2.flife.de/data/natcen-social-research/igb_html/index.php?bericht_id=1000001&index=&lang=ENG
Nelsen, H. M., & Kroliczak, A. (1984). Parental use of the
threat "God will punish": Replication and extension. Journal
for the Scientific Study of Religion, 23, 267-277. DOI: 10.2307/1386041.
Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular:
Religion and politics worldwide. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Press.
Penrose, R. (1989). The emperor's new mind: Concerning
computers, minds, and the laws of physics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Pew Research Center. (2012). "Nones" on the rise.
Retrieved from http://www.pewforum.org/2012/10/09/nones-on-the-rise/
Pyysiainen, I. (2003). On the "innateness" of religion: A
comment on Bering. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3, 218-225.
Ray, D. (2009). The god virus: How religion infects our lives and
culture. London, UK: ICP Press.
Richert, R. A., & Barrett, J. L. (2005). Do you see what I see?
Young children's assumptions about God's perceptual abilities.
International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 15, 283-295.
Shapiro, J. (1988). Relationships between dimension of depression
experience and evaluation beliefs about people in general. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 388-400.
Singhal, A. (2001). Modern information retrieval: A brief overview.
Bulletin of the IEEE Computer Society Technical Committee on Data
Engineering, 24(4), 35-43. VERBI GmbH. (2013). MAXQDA version 11
[Computer software]. Berlin, Germany: Author.
Wigger, J. (2011). See-through knowing: Learning from children and
their invisible friends. Journal of Childhood and Religion, 2(3).
Retrieved from http://www.childhoodandreligion.com/JCR/Volume_2_(2011)_files/Wigger%20May%202011.pdf
Wigger, J., Paxson, K., & Ryan, L. (2012). What do invisible
friends know? Imaginary companions, God, and theory of mind.
International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23(1), 2-14. DOI:
10.1080/10508619.2013.739059.
Wright, R. (2009). The evolution of God. New York, NY: Little,
Brown and Company.
Yu, C. H. (2004). Advance in Monte Carlo Simulations and robustness
study and their implications for the dispute in philosophy of
mathematics. Minerva, 8, 62-90. Retrieved from
http://www.mic.ul.ie/stephen/vol8/montecarlo.pdf Yu, C. H. (2014,
January). Beyond randomness and pattern: Are Christian and scientific
worldviews compatible? Paper presented at Southern California Christians
in Science Conference, Azusa, CA.
Yu, C. H., Jannasch-Pennell, A., & DiGangi, S. (2011).
Compatibility between text mining and qualitative research in the
perspectives of grounded theory, content analysis, and reliability.
Qualitative Report, 16, 730-744. Retrieved from
http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR16-3/yu.pdf Zuckerman, P. (2008). Society
without God. New York: NYU Press.
Zuckerman, P. (2011). Faith no more: Why people reject religion?
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Special thanks to Ms. Young Kim, Dr. Anne-Marie Conn, Ms. Clara
Myers, Dr. Brian Eck, Ms. Anna Yu, and Ms. Samantha Douglas for their
valuable input in this project. Correspondence regarding this article
should be addressed to Chong Ho Yu, Ph.D., Associate Professor,
Department of Psychology, Azusa Pacific University, Azusa CA 91702;
chonghoyu@gmail.com; cyu@apu.edu
Chong Ho Yu
Azusa Pacific University
Author
Chong Ho (Alex) Yu is an Associate Professor of the Department of
Psychology at Azusa Pacific University. He has a Ph.D. in educational
psychology (Arizona State University, ASU) with a concentration on
measurement, statistics, and methodological studies, as well as a Ph.D.
in philosophy (ASU) with a specialization in history and philosophy of
science. His research interests include, but are not limited to,
exploratory data analysis, data visualization, data mining, and faith
integration.
Table 1
Codes and Categories in EOOQ
Item number Code Category
in EOOQ
1 Charming Social skills
2 Sorrow Subjective well-being
3 Kind Kindness/morality
4 Creative Achievement-relatedness
5 Good sense of humor Social skills
6 Hypocritical Kindness/morality
7 Friendly Social skills
8 Trustworthy Kindness/morality
9 Wise Achievement-relatedness
10 Phony Kindness/morality
11 Sad Kindness/morality
12 Attractive Social skills
13 Shy Social skills
14 Anxious Subjective well-being
15 Cruel Kindness/morality
16 Fun to work with Social skills
17 Intelligent Achievement-relatedness
18 Happy with themselves Subjective well-being
19 Snobby Kindness/morality
20 Hard-working Kindness/morality
21 Happy with their lives Kindness/morality
22 Good-looking Kindness/morality
23 Ethical Kindness/morality
24 Competent Kindness/morality
25 Efficient Kindness/morality
26 Conceited Kindness/morality
27 Moody Kindness/morality
28 Knowledgeable Kindness/morality
29 Dishonest Kindness/morality
30 Likable Kindness/morality
31 Depressed Kindness/morality
32 Helpful Kindness/morality
33 Easy to get along with Kindness/morality
34 Selfish Kindness/morality
35 Loving Kindness/morality
36 Accomplished Kindness/morality
37 Psychologically healthy Kindness/morality
38 Talented Kindness/morality
Table 2
Original EOOQ Code Composition
Category Frequency Percentage
Achievement-relatedness 3 7.895%
Kindness/morality 26 68.421%
Social skills 6 15.789%
Subjective well-being 3 7.895%
Table 3
New Traits Generated by Human Coders
Code Category
Energetic Subjective well-being
Compassionate Kindness/morality
Inspiring Social skills
Adventurous Achievement-relatedness
Innovative Achievement-relatedness
Modest Kindness/morality
Bravery Kindness/morality
Family-oriented Social skills
Radical Kindness/morality
Spiritual Subjective well-being
Encouraging Kindness/morality
Carefree Subjective well-being
Leadership Achievement-relatedness
Resilient Subjective well-being
Justice Kindness/morality
Table 4
Modified Code Composition
Category Frequency Percentage
Achievement-relatedness 6 11.321%
Kindness/morality 32 60.377%
Social skills 8 15.094%
Subjective well-being 7 13.208%
Table 5
Frequency of EOOQ and Self-Generated Codes
Code Frequency
Accomplished 556
Talented 137
Intelligent 109
Hard-working 105
Bravery 89
Helpful 74
Good sense of humor 72
Kind 63
Justice 55
Creative 54
Compassionate 53
Energetic 51
Leadership 51
Inspiring 50
Likable 50
Loving 48
Adventurous 43
Family-oriented 38
Knowledgeable 37
Resilient 36
Table 6
Code Relationship in the Matrix Form
inspiring justice bravery
inspiring 0 1 0
justice 1 0 2
bravery 0 2 0
leadership 3 1 0
talented 1 0 2
accomplished 6 5 5
helpful i 3 3
hard-working 0 0 2
intelligent 1 0 3
accom-
leadership talented plished
inspiring 3 1 6
justice 1 0 5
bravery 0 2 5
leadership 0 2 6
talented 2 0 ii
accomplished 6 11 0
helpful 2 0 7
hard-working 0 0 7
intelligent 0 1 8
hard-
helpful working intelligent
inspiring 1 0 1
justice 3 0 0
bravery 3 2 3
leadership 2 0 0
talented 0 0 1
accomplished 7 7 8
helpful 0 4 0
hard-working 4 0 4
intelligent 0 4 0
Table 7
Categories Generated by SPSS Text Mining
Category Frequency
Academic 257
Employment 255
Artistic 174
Family-relatedness 143
Membership of associations 121
Leadership 58
Politics 43
Sport 40
Business 31
Military 20