Gender differences in predictors of anticipated division of household labor in Christian students.
Chan, Catherine S. ; Hall, M. Elizabeth Lewis ; Anderson, Tamara L. 等
The factors leading to the division of household labor in marriage
has been the subject of research for many years. Historically, the
division of household labor has been influenced by factors such as the
industrial revolution, which moved economic productivity out of the home
and resulted in a split between household labor and economic
productivity, with women taking on the larger part of household labor.
In recent decades, research has documented the existence of gender
divisions in the types of household labor performed (Berk & Berk
1979). Blair and Lichter (1991) divided household tasks into gender
stereotypical categories based on data from the 1988 National Survey of
Families and Households, which asked 3,190 married and cohabiting
couples about their time spent on eight household chores. Women were
found to spend more time preparing meals, washing dishes and cleaning up
after meals, cleaning house, and washing, ironing, and mending clothes.
Men were found to spend more time doing outdoor and household
maintenance, and auto maintenance and repair. Men and women were found
to spend a similar amount of time grocery shopping, and paying the
bills. The present study explores how religiously-influenced views on
household labor intersect with contemporary gender threat theory in
predicting anticipated division of household labor in a sample of
Christian undergraduate students.
Past research focused on theories that explain the division of
household labor based on external resources like time and money, with
inconsistent results. However, more recently Brines (1994) and
Greenstein (2000) introduced an alternative explanation for household
division of labor: gender performance theory. Gender performance theory
argues that the division of household labor is determined by efforts to
maintain one's masculinity or femininity. As such, the theory
suggests that an individual will perform the household tasks that would
secure his or her identity as a man or woman. The gender performance
process suggests that couples who stray from their stereotypical gender
roles compensate for their role deviation by exaggerating stereotypical
behaviors. When couples stray from their stereotypical gender roles
their identity as a woman/wife or as a man/husband is threatened. To
compensate for the deviation from the stereotype, they adjust their
household behaviors to divide the chores in a more stereotypical manner.
This theory is consistent with compensatory masculinity theory (Babl
1979), which states that men defensively exaggerate their masculinity
when their sex-roles are threatened.
In support of this theory, both Brines (1994) and Greenstein (2000)
found that the division of housework became more traditional when
stereotypical roles were breeched. As wives' income increased
beyond that of the husbands, wives' hours of housework increased,
and husbands' hours of housework decreased. Furthermore, Brines
(1994) found that husbands' hours of housework increased initially
when they lost their jobs, but when husbands were unemployed long term,
their hours of housework decreased. Schneider (2011), in contrast, found
in a large, contemporary, time-diary study that women did perform their
prescribed gender roles in a more stereotypical fashion when gender
roles were breached; however, men did not. Schneider noted that
re-analysis of Brines' and Greenstein's data found that when
the 2%-3% of lowest earning men were excluded from the samples, their
data did not support gender performance theory in men. He concluded that
women perform gender through housework, but men do not.
Qualitative research has also found evidence of gender performance
theory in other aspects of domestic life (Kroska 2008; Tichenor 2005;
Zuo & Bian 2001). One qualitative study found that men were
considered incapable and lacking in ambition when they were not
providing the main financial support (Zuo & Bian 2001). To minimize
the amount of gender deviation within a couple, wives that were earning
more than their husbands downplayed their financial contribution, and
increased their housework contribution and child care so as to combat
opinions of being a "bad mother" or being selfish,
non-feminine, and irresponsible. In addition, the term provider was
limited to financial contribution when wives were earning more than
their husbands, but expanded to include emotional needs when husbands
were not the main financial contributor (Tichenor 2005). To compensate
for their gender deviant behaviors, men and women attempted to minimize
their deviant behaviors and emphasize traditional behaviors.
Expected Division of Household Labor
In addition to exploring actual division of labor, research has
explored college students' attitudes about gender roles and
anticipated division of household labor. This research has important
implications, as gender role attitudes and anticipated roles influence
educational factors such as the choice of majors and aspirations for
further education. Gender role attitudes are becoming more egalitarian
over time, especially among women and college students (Bryant 2003;
Kaufman 2005). However, the trend toward egalitarian gender role
attitudes does not always translate into more egalitarian anticipated
division of household labor. One study found that college students who
endorsed more egalitarian gender roles still reported traditional
anticipated divisions of labor (Ferber & Young 1997). Participants
reported desiring more equal gender roles, but expected reality to
reflect more traditional and unequal divisions of labor. It is also
possible that this discrepancy reflects a social desirability bias, as
egalitarian gender roles are perceived as more acceptable in educational
settings. If this is the case, the discrepancy in attitudes suggests
that students continue to have fairly traditional expectations regarding
the division of household labor (Askari et al. 2010; Fetterolf &
Eagley 2011; Fulcher & Coyle 2011). However, to date no studies have
specifically studied the relationship between gender performance theory
and anticipated division of household labor.
Sanctification
Religious beliefs and values contribute additional complexity to
the study of gender roles and division of household labor, as religion
plays a significant role in structuring gendered beliefs and behavior
(Davidman 1991; Hawley 1994; Yadgar 2006). Research among conservative
Christians demonstrates that as a whole, they are more conservative in
their views about gender roles than the general population (Brinkerhoff
& MacKie 1985; Jensen & Jensen 1993; Lehrer 1995; Morgan 1987),
although there is also considerable within-group variation in these
views. Quantitative research shows that more conservative religious
couples in general adhere to a more traditional division of household
labor than non-conservative couples (Ellison & Bartkowski 2002).
Qualitative research also illustrates this dynamic. For example,
Bartkowski (1999), in an ethnographic study, found evidence that women
continued to perform the majority of the housework, even when employed,
in order to align themselves with the typical conservative Christian
view that women should be in charge of the household.
Given the centrality of traditional gender roles in conservative
Christianity, centering on men as breadwinners and women as homemaker
roles, it is likely that the gender performance of individuals in this
tradition are influenced by their religiously-driven beliefs and values
on the topic. Mahoney et al. (1999) argued persuasively that proximal
concepts and measures of religion are superior to distal concepts in
understanding the influence of religion on areas of human functioning.
Much research on religion involves only global indicators of
religiousness (e.g., single items on religious affiliation). While the
role of religion in the distribution of household tasks has been studied
from a distal perspective, that is, in terms of affiliation with a
religious tradition, it has not been studied through the lenses of
proximal religious constructs. This is particularly problematic given
indications that a great deal of variability with respect to gender
roles can be found within conservative sectors of Christianity
(Gallagher 2004).
The proximal construct of sanctification can contribute to the
understanding of the relationship between religiosity and division of
household labor, by taking into account how the division of household
labor is perceived from a religious perspective, that is, how it is
"sanctified." Mahoney et al. (2003) developed a theoretical
model around this concept of sanctification, defining sanctification as
a psychological process through which aspects of life are perceived by
people as having divine character and significance. They proposed that
when aspects of life are sanctified, people will invest more resources
to preserve and protect them (Mahoney et al. 2005; Mahoney et al. 2003;
Pargament & Mahoney 2005), will experience spiritual emotions with
respect to those aspects of life, and will derive benefits such as
greater satisfaction, meaning, pleasure, and better functioning from the
sanctified area of life (Mahoney et al. 2003). Sanctification is divided
into two constructs: perception of sacred qualities and manifestation of
God. The perception of sacred qualities represents the degree to which
an individual prescribes spiritual adjectives to a construct. The
manifestation of God represents the degree to which a construct has
divine and spiritual significance.
Although no research exists regarding the sanctification of the
division of household labor, sanctification has been studied with
respect to marriage. Mahoney et al. (1999) found that couples that
sanctified their marriages, as measured by the perceived sacred
qualities scale and manifestation of God scale, were more protective of
their relationships and approached conflicts in a more collaborative
manner. Similarly, Baker et al. (2009) showed that couples who perceived
their marriage and responsibilities in marriage to be ordained by God,
exhibited more traditional marriage roles. Though Baker et al. (2009)
did not explicitly use the term sanctification of marriage, he described
marriages to have divine qualities through descriptors like
"ordained and accountable to God." Given that the
sanctification of marriage impacts individuals' behaviors in the
marriage context, sanctification, defined as the degree to which
participants perceive anticipated division of household labor to be an
expression of their religious and theocentric beliefs and experiences,
may also influence anticipated behaviors in this area.
The Present Study
The current study contributes to the literature on division of
household labor in two ways. In the first place, it extends the study of
gender performance theory to the anticipated division of household
labor, using a sample of unmarried college students. Within this
context, based on previous research, it is hypothesized that threats to
masculinity and to femininity will produce more traditional gender
displays in men and women, respectively. Given evidence that men and
women may show differences in their performance of gender, with mixed
results for men, gender differences will be explored.
A second objective in this study is to extend our understanding of
the relationship between religion and anticipated division of household
labor, by examining the influence of sanctification of the division of
household labor on students' expected division of household labor.
Sanctification of the division of household labor is hypothesized to
moderate the relationship between gender identity threat and college
students' expected division of household labor. The more
individuals perceive their roles to have divine influence, the less
influence gender identity threat will have on college students'
expected division of household labor. Religiously-based attributions for
behavior set clear and divinely-sanctioned standards for gender
performance, and may provide a protective influence against threats to
gender identity.
Method
Participants
A total of 133 participants were recruited from a Christian liberal
arts university on the west coast. This university requires all students
to sign a statement of faith and code of conduct consistent with its
evangelical Christian commitments, and attendance at religious services.
Seven participants were removed from the dataset as they did not meet
the inclusion criteria of age (>18) and relationship status (single).
of the remaining 126 participants, 48 (38%) were men and 78 (62%) were
women ranging from the ages of 18-36 (M = 20.13, SD = 2.68). Of the 126
participants, 47 (37.3%) were freshman, 21 (16.7%) were sophomores, 29
(23%) were juniors, 23 (18.3%) were seniors, and 6 (4.8%) were 5th
years. The participants were predominantly European American (80,
63.5%), with 24 (19%) participants identifying as Asian/Asian American,
12 (9.5%) as Hispanic/Latino/a, 2 (1.6%) as African American, 6 (4.8%)
as Other, and a remaining 6 (1.6%) participants did not indicate their
ethnicity. Forty participants (32%) reported being in a current
relationship, while 86 (68%) indicated that they were not currently in a
relationship. Of the participants that were in a relationship two (5%)
were engaged. Participants were compensated with course credit in an
introductory psychology course or extra credit for other undergraduate
psychology courses.
Measures and Experimental Manipulation
Gender identity threat. To produce gender identity threat, the
Masculine/Feminine Knowledge Test (Rudman & Fairchild 2004) was
administered. This measure consists of two forms, a masculine knowledge
test and a feminine knowledge test. Each test contains 29-30 multiple
choice questions evaluating broad stereotypical masculine or feminine
knowledge. Participants were randomly assigned to take either the
version of the test corresponding to their own gender, or to the
opposite gender. Despite how participants performed, all participants
were informed that they did well, indicating a high degree of knowledge
about stereotypically masculine or feminine topics. Participants were
given the following results, "Congratulations! You scored in the 96
percentile! This means you scored higher than 96 out of 100 past test
takers on this masculine/feminine knowledge test." Participants
received either gender consistent feedback or gender contradictory
feedback. For example, for women, gender consistent feedback involved
women taking the feminine knowledge test, and being told that they knew
a large amount about stereotypically feminine topics. Gender
contradictory feedback involved women taking the masculine knowledge
test, and being told that they knew a large amount about stereotypically
masculine topics which is contrary to their own gender. This
experimental manipulation has been shown to produce gender threat
(Rudman & Fairchild 2004).
Expected division of household labor. To measure participants'
expected division of household labor, Blair and Lichter's (1991)
stereotypically divided household tasks were rated on a 5-point Likert
scale on how often a participant expected to perform that task (1 =
never, 5 = always). Stereotypically feminine tasks included preparing
meals, washing dishes and cleaning up after meals, cleaning house, and
washing, ironing, and mending clothes. Stereotypically masculine tasks
included outdoor and other household maintenance, auto maintenance and
repair, and breadwinning. Neutral tasks included shopping for groceries,
driving other household members to work, school, other, and paying bills
and keeping financial records. For women, the stereotypically masculine
items were reverse-scored; for men, the stereotypically feminine items
were reverse-scored. The neutral items were not used for the calculation
of item totals. The items were then summed, resulting in a possible
range from 7 to 35. High scores indicate an expected traditional
division of household labor and low scores indicate a more expected
egalitarian division of household labor. The alpha coefficient for the
present study was .79.
Sanctification. Mahoney et al.'s (2005) Manifestation of God
in Strivings measure was used to assess participants'
sanctification of their expected division of household labor. While
scales are available for both Perceived Sacred Qualities and
Manifestation of God, only the Manifestation of God measure was used due
to the inapplicability of the adjectives such as blessed, cursed,
heavenly, earthly, etc., in the Perceived Sacred Qualities to
anticipated division of household labor. This self-report measure used a
5 point Likert scale (1strongly disagree to 5-strongly agree) to
indicate the degree to which participants perceived a divine being
playing a role in their division of household labor. Items were edited
to apply to participants' expected division of household tasks.
Participants indicated their level of agreement to the following five
questions: "God played a role in how I expect my spouse and I to
divide up household tasks," "God is present in how my spouse
and I expect to divide up household tasks," "My anticipated
division of household tasks is a reflection of God's will,"
"I expect to experience God through the way my spouse and I divide
up household tasks," "My anticipated division of household
tasks reflects what God wants for me." Mahoney et al. (1999)
reported a coefficient alpha for the original scale of .96. The
coefficient alpha for the current study was .85. Scores were summed to
obtain a total score, and possible scores ranged from 5 to 25 with high
scores representing a high degree of sanctification of anticipated
household labor and low scores indicating a low degree of sanctification
of one's anticipated household labor.
Additional items. In order to further understand the influence of
religiosity on anticipated division of household labor, two additional
items were included. These items were intended to address how household
labor should be divided from a religious perspective: according to the
preferences of the marriage partners ("My future spouse and I will
divide up our household tasks depending on giftedness and time
availability"), and according to divine prescription ("My
future spouse and I will divide up household tasks according to how God
has prescribed").
Procedure
Participants accessed the study online. They were randomly assigned
to either the gender contradictory group (men taking the feminine
knowledge test, and women taking the masculine knowledge test) or gender
consistent group (men taking the masculine knowledge test, and women
taking the feminine knowledge test): 66 (52%) were in the gender
contradictory group, and 60 (48%) were in the gender consistent group.
After agreeing to the informed consent, participants completed the
masculine or feminine knowledge test, expectation of the division of
household labor measure, sanctification measure, and a demographics
questionnaire.
Results
Descriptive Results
Regarding Sanctification, men had a mean of 18.75 (SD=3.52), and
scores ranged from 12 to 25. Women had very similar results, with a mean
of 19.08 (SD=3.56), and a range from 5 to 25. On the Expected Division
of Household Labor scale, men had a mean of 25.53 (SD=1.48), and scores
ranged from 16 to 31. Women had very similar results, with a mean of
26.53 (SD=2.87), and a range from 18 to 34.
First Hypothesis
In the first hypothesis it was predicted that threats to one's
gender identity would result in a more traditional display of household
labor. Participants who received the gender contradictory knowledge test
were predicted to have higher scores on the expected division of
household labor measure, indicating a more traditional expectation of
the division of household labor. Independent t-tests were conducted by
gender comparing means between gender identity threat and no threat on
expected division of labor. There was not a significant difference for
men, t(45) = -.31, p = .38 (threat group, M=23.35, SD=3.10, no-threat
group, M=23.67, SD=3.65). However, there was a significant difference
for women, t(73) = 1.65, p = .05, indicating that women whose gender
identity was threatened (M=27.08, SD=2.91) expected to divide household
labor in a more traditional manner than the no-threat group (M=26,
SD=2.76).
Second Hypothesis
In the second hypothesis, it was predicted that sanctification of
the division of household labor would moderate the relationship between
gender identity threat and college students' expected division of
household labor. This was assessed by using hierarchical multiple
regressions for men and women separately. Sanctification was centered by
subtracting the mean from each sanctification score. Gender identity
threat and centered sanctification were entered in the first step. An
interaction term was created by multiplying gender identity threat and
centered sanctification, and the interaction variable was entered in the
second step. Regression statistics results, using one-tailed
significance, are presented in Table 1.
Sanctification of the division of household labor was found to
moderate the relationship for men ([beta] = .31, p = .05), but not for
women ([beta] = .06, p = .35). The moderation results indicated that at
high levels of sanctification, men whose gender identity was threatened
displayed a more traditional expected division of household labor than
men whose gender identity was not threatened, while the opposite was
true for men at low levels of sanctification of division of household
labor (figure 1). Although sanctification did not moderate the
relationship for women, the overall regression for women was significant
(F = 2.76, p = .04), indicating that together, gender identity threat
and sanctification contributed significantly to expected division of
household labor for women. These results confirm that women whose gender
identity is threatened show more traditional expected division of
household labor, and indicate that women who sanctified their
anticipated roles tended to have more traditional anticipated division
of household labor.
Post-hoc analyses. Though the results showed that sanctification
significantly moderated the relationship between gender identity threat
and anticipated division of household labor in men, it did so in the
opposite direction than predicted. In other words, sanctification was
expected to serve a protective role against gender identity threat.
Instead, those who sanctified anticipated division of household labor
appeared much more susceptible to gender identity threats. In order to
understand these results, correlations were run between the two items
assessing how anticipated division of household labor is done from a
religious perspective, and sanctification. While both the
"preference" and the "divinely prescribed" items
correlated positively with sanctification (r = .23, p. = .01 and r =
.66, p. = .00), the "divinely prescribed" item correlated much
more strongly, indicating that people who tend to sanctify the
anticipated division of household labor, also tend to see the division
of household labor as being divinely prescribed (indicating traditional
gender roles).
Discussion
Previous literature has suggested that threats to one's gender
identity result in a more traditional display of division of household
labor (Brines 1994; Greenstein 2000). The current study explored gender
differences in the relationship between gender identity threat and
expected division of household labor. Gender identity threat only showed
a main effect for expected division of household labor for women,
confirming Schneider's (2011) findings of gender differences with
actual division of household labor. However, the present study also
contributes to our understanding of the influence of gender identity
threat by demonstrating that when the moderating effects of
sanctification are taken into account, gender identity threat does
indeed contribute variance to anticipated division of household labor
for men. This finding suggests that the effects of gender identity
threat are not limited to women, but may manifest in more complex ways
in men.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
These findings are further clarified by the results showing that
those who sanctified their anticipated roles, also tended to see these
roles as being divinely prescribed. This suggests that anticipated
traditional divisions of household labor take on additional importance
for people who sanctify them. This heightened sense of obligation to
fulfill traditional gender roles may make these individuals more
susceptible to gender identity threat, resulting in the pattern of
results observed in this sample: more traditional anticipated division
of household labor in the group that sanctified those roles and was
threatened, than in the group that was not threatened. Consequently,
perhaps Schneider's (2011) conclusions that men are not affected by
gender identity threat with respect to division of household labor, must
be qualified. While this may be true for men in general, perhaps men
"act out gender" in household tasks when the performance of
traditional gender roles is important to them. This possibility should
be further explored in married couples and in populations other than
Christian undergraduates for whom traditional roles are central for
religious or cultural reasons.
The Christian affiliation of the sample lent itself to the
exploration of ways in which their religious commitments influence their
anticipated division of household labor. The level of sanctification
did, in fact, influence participants' anticipated division of
household labor. Sanctification of expected division of household labor
was found to be a significant moderator of the gender threat-division of
household labor relationship for men, as noted above, and showed a main
effect for women. The pattern of correlations between the measure of
sanctification, and the two items exploring whether or not the division
of household tasks is seen to be divinely prescriptive is worth noting
in this context. Given the diversity of positions on gender roles in
conservative Christian circles, not all Christians may see the division
of household labor as an area that should be sanctified. Specifically,
those who see the division of household labor as not divinely
prescribed, do not tend to sanctify the division of household labor as
strongly. Future studies should more clearly articulate the differences
between religious views on how roles are divided, and religious views
that sanctify the actual performance of household labor--regardless of
how it is divided.
Understanding the psychological construct of sanctification in this
way helps clarify the results. In the group with low levels of
sanctification of division of household labor, the effects of gender on
anticipated behavior were even more salient than in the group with high
levels of sanctification. In the group with low levels of
sanctification, who tended to have less prescriptive views of gender
roles, it may be that men whose gender identity was not threatened had
their masculinity affirmed and thus displayed a more traditional
division of household labor. For men that were threatened, their
understanding of women appeared to be affirmed, thus they presented a
less traditional display of expected division of household labor. The
opposite was true in the group of men that saw the division of household
labor as an area of life that could be sanctified, as noted earlier. In
this group, God's prescriptions appeared to be the more salient
issue. Thus, in the no-threat group, since their anticipated behavior
seemed to be a result of their understanding of God's will, rather
than their own felt need to be masculine, the masculinity-affirming
manipulation did not inflate scores. However, when their sense of
masculinity was threatened, the stake may have been higher, given
God's perceived interest in this issue, and resulting in a greater
anticipated desire to behave in gender-consistent ways.
Similar to all research, the generalizability of this study is
limited to groups with characteristics similar to the population from
which the sample was drawn. The sample was recruited from a private
Christian, liberal arts university. The demographics of the sample show
that it was largely European American. Further research with different
religious traditions and ethnicities is necessary to confirm the unique
ways in which religiosity intersects with gender identity in predicting
the division of household labor.
Since expected division of household labor is likely to vary
greatly from the actual division of household labor, different factors
may influence this concept, such as current living environment,
roommates, and social desirability. Further research is needed to
understand how these factors may influence how undergraduates anticipate
dividing household labor. These results are important to those working
with undergraduate students of faith, as gender roles influence key
educational variables such as anticipated participation in the
workforce, career choice, and pursuit of graduate studies.
References
Askari, S. F., Liss, M., Erchull, M. J., Staebell, S. E., &
Axelson, S. J. (2010). Men want equality but women don't expect it:
Young adults' expectations for participation in household and child
care chores. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 34, 243-252. doi:
10.1111/j.14716402.2010.01565.x
Babl, J. D. (1979). Compensatory masculine responding as a function
of sex role. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47(2),
252-257. doi:10.1037/0022006X.47.2.252
Baker, E. H., Sanchez, L. A., Nock, S.L., & Wright, J. D.
(2009). Covenant marriage and the sanctification of gendered marital
roles. Jour nal of Family Issues, 30,147-180. doi:10.1177/0
192513X08324109
Bartkowski, J. P. (1999). one step forward, one step back:
"Progressive traditionalism" and the negotiation of domestic
labor in evangelical families. Gender Issues, 17, 37-61.
Berk, R. A., & Berk, S. F. (1979). Labor and leisure at home:
Content and organization of the household day. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Blair, S., & Lichter, D. (1991). Measuring the division of
household labor: Gender segregation of housework among American couples.
Journal of Family Issues, 12(1), 91-113. doi:10.1177/0 19251391012001007
Brines, J. (1994). Economic dependency, gender, and the division of
labor at home. The American Journal of Sociology, 100, 652-688.
Brinkerhoff, M. B. & MacKie, M. (1985). Religion and gender: A
comparison of Canadian and American student attitudes. Journal of
Marriage and Family, 47, 415-429.
Bryant, A. N. (2003). Changes in attitudes toward women's
roles: Predicting gender-role traditionalism among college students. Sex
Roles, 48(3), 131-142. doi:10.1023/A:102245120 5292
Davidman, L. (1991). Tradition in a rootless world: Women turn to
Orthodox Judaism. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Ellison, C. G., & Bartkowski, J. P. (2002). Conservative
Protestantism and the division of household labor among married couples.
Journal of Family Issues, 23, 950-984. doi:10.1177/ 019251302237299
Ferber, M. A., & Young, L. (1997). Student attitudes toward
roles of women and men: Is the egalitarian household imminent? Feminist
Economics, 3(1), 65-83.
Fetterolf, J. C. & Eagly, A. H. (2011). Do young women expect
gender equality in their future lives? An answer from a possible selves
experiment. Sex Roles, 65, 83-93. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9981-9
Fulcher, M. & E. F. Coyle, E.F. (2011). Breadwinner and
caregiver: A cross-sectional analysis of children's and emerging
adults' visions of their future family roles. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 29, 330-346. doi:
10.1111/j.2044835X.2011.02026.X
Gallagher, S. K. (2004). Where are the antifeminist evangelicals?:
Evangelical identity, subcultural location, and attitudes toward
feminism. Gender & Society, 18, 451-472. doi:
10.1177/08912423204266157
Greenstein, T. N. (2000). Economic dependence, gender, and the
division of labor in the home: A replication and extension. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 62(2), 322-335. doi:10.111 1/j.1741
3737.2000.00322.x
Hawley, J. S. (1994). Fundamentalism and gender. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Jensen, L. & Jensen, J. (1993). Family values, religiosity, and
gender. Psychological Reports, 73, 429-430.
Kaufman, G. (2005). Gender role attitudes and college
students' work and family expectations. Gender Issues, 22, 58-71.
Kroska, A. (2008). Examining husband-wife differences in the
meaning of family financial support. Sociological Perspectives, 51(1),
63-90. doi:10.1525/sop.2008.51.1.63
Lehrer, E. L. (1995). The effects of religion on the labor supply
of married women. Social Science Research, 24, 281-301.
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Cole, B., Jewell, T., Magyar, G. M.,
Tarakeshwar, N., ... Phillips, R. (2005). A higher purpose: The
sanctification of strivings in a community sample. The International
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 15(3), 239-262.
doi:10.1207/s15327582ijpr1503_4
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Jewell, T., Swank, A. B., Scott, E.,
Emery, E., & Rye, M. (1999). Marriage and the spiritual realm: The
role of proximal and distal religious constructs in marital functioning.
Journal of Family Psychology, 13(3), 321-338.
doi:10.1037/0893-3200.13.3.321
Mahoney, A., Pargament, K. I., Murray-Swank, A., &
Murray-Swank, N. (2003). Religion and the sanctification of family
relationships. Review of Religious Research, 44, 220-236.
Morgan, M. Y. (1987). The impact of religion on gender-role
attitudes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 301-310.
Pargament, K. I., & Mahoney, A. (2005). Sacred matters:
Sanctification as a vital topic for the psychology of religion. The
International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 15, 179-198.
Rudman, L. A., & Fairchild, K. (2004). Reactions to
counterstereotypic behavior: The role of backlash in cultural stereotype
maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2),
157-176. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.157
Schneider, D. (2011). Market earnings and household work: New tests
of gender performance theory. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73,
845-860. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2011.00851.x
Tichenor, V. (2005). Maintaining men's dominance: Negotiating
identity and power when she earns more. Sex Roles, 53(3/4), 191-205.
doi:10.1007/s11199-0055678-2
Yadgar, Y. (2006). Gender, religion, and feminism: The case of
Jewish Israeli Traditionalists. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 45, 353-370.
Zuo, J., & Bian, Y. (2001). Gendered resources, division of
housework, and perceived fairness. A case in urban China. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 63(4), 1122-1133. doi:10.1111/j.
1741-3737.2001.01122.x
Catherine S. Chan
M. Elizabeth Lewis Hall
Tamara L. Anderson
Biola University
Catherine S. Chan (M.A. in Clinical Psychology, Rosemead School of
Psychology) is a Ph.D. candidate at the Rosemead School of Psychology,
Biola University (CA). Ms. Chan's interests include gender issues,
religion, and Asian American women.
M. Elizabeth Lewis Hall (Ph.D. in Clinical Psychology, Rosemead
School of Psychology) is Professor of Psychology at Biola University
(CA). Dr. Hall's research interests include women and work,
mothering, missions and mental health, and embodiment.
Tamara L. Anderson (Ph.D. in Clinical Psychology, California School
of Professional Psychology, Los Angeles) is Professor of Psychology and
Associate Dean of Graduate Students at Rosemead School of Psychology,
Biola University (CA). Dr. Anderson's research interests include
gender issues, attachment, ethics and law, conflict resolution, in
addition to previous work in the area of eating disorders.
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in
the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. Correspondence
regarding this article should be sent to catherine.s.chan@biola.edu
Table 1
Gender Identity Threat, Sanctification, and Expected Division
of Household Labor.
Criterion Model Variable [beta] p
Expected Model 1
Division of ThreatNon- -.07 .32
Household Threat
Labor (Men Sanctification -.09 .28
Only)
Model 2
ThreatNon- -.002 .50
Threat
Sanctification -.24 .09
ThreatXSanct .31 .05
Expected Model 1
Division of
Household ThreatNon- .18 .07
Labor Threat
(Women only) Sanctification .17 .07
Model 2
ThreatNon- .17 .08
Threat
Sanctification .14 .16
ThreatXSanct .06 .35
Criterion Model [R.sup.2] F p
Expected Model 1 .01 .22 .40
Division of
Household
Labor (Men
Only)
Model 2 .06 2.89 .05
Expected Model 1 .07 2.76 .04
Division of
Household
Labor
(Women only)
Model 2 .00 .15 .35