Spiritual formation among doctoral psychology students in explicitly Christian programs.
Fisk, Laura K. ; Flores, Marcel H. ; McMinn, Mark R. 等
Several explicitly Christian doctoral programs have emerged and
become accredited by the American Psychological Association in recent
decades (Johnson & McMinn, 2003). These programs typically hire
faculty who endorse a Christian statement of faith and include
curriculum pertaining to the integration of psychology and Christianity.
Most explicitly Christian doctoral programs also show admission
preference to students who express endorsement of and personal
commitment to Christian beliefs.
According to Slife and Reber (2009), psychology tends to have a
naturalist narrative emerging from modernism that may actually compete
with a Christian narrative, formed from theological anthropology. If
this is the case, then even Christian students admitted to explicitly
Christian doctoral training programs might experience a degree of faith
degradation throughout training. Given the centrality of faith in the
identity of these programs, and the students they admit, this seems an
important area of research. How does attending an explicitly Christian
doctoral program affect the spiritual development of students?
Past research, mostly unpublished, has generated mixed results.
Pearce (1996) found that during the course of graduate school many
students move toward a more relational, intimate, interactive
relationship with God. Hofer (2004) did a 9-year follow-up with the
participants in Pearce's study, finding the participants reported
having an even deeper, stronger, and more important relationship with
God than when in the program. But not all research suggests increased
faith experience during graduate school. Edwards (2006) found a linear
decline in faith commitment over the course of three years among
clinical psychology graduate students at a Christian university. Whereas
Edwards found a decline on all 8 subscales of the God Image Inventory
(GII) as well as decreased church attendance, Mullis (2008) reported
positive change on 2 of the 8 GII subscales over a similar 3-year span.
From the limited research available, it seems likely that faith
development during graduate school is a multidimensional research issue,
calling for studies that look at various aspects of religious and
spiritual beliefs and practices. Moreover, it may not be a linear
phenomenon. Students may have critical periods where faith is enhanced
or compromised, and these periods may occur either during or after
graduate studies.
Most of the prior studies investigating spiritual development of
students in explicitly Christian doctoral programs have focused on how
students perceive God (God image) or attachment to God. These are
important variables, but many more dimensions of faith warrant
investigation. How is it that students explain the events of their own
lives as well as the often-troubling events they see in their patients
and clients? How do students use faith to cope with the inevitable
struggles they experience in graduate school? To what extent is faith
part of their problem solving as they encounter fatigue and other
challenges of training? The present studies consider an array of
variables that pertain to spiritual development, including locus of
control, religious coping, attribution, awareness of God, relationship
with God, religious problem solving, fatigue, and social support.
Moreover, previous studies have focused only on students in one
institution. The present studies are collaborative multisite endeavors
involving students from five Christian doctoral training programs in
clinical psychology. In an effort to foster collaboration and avoid
competition, we have not computed or reported results for individual
programs. Rather, we offer these findings in aggregate form in an effort
to explore spiritual development among students in Christian doctoral
training programs.
Based on the work of Slife and Reber (2009), we hypothesized that
various markers of students' spiritual development will show
declines throughout an academic year of training in explicitly Christian
doctoral programs. Given the mixed findings of past studies, and the
exploratory nature of the current studies, we held this hypothesis
lightly.
Study 1
Participants and Procedures
Participants were graduate students drawn from five explicitly
Christian doctoral training programs. Faculty collaborators were
identified at each of the five programs. These faculty members helped
recruit participants for the study after the required institutional
review procedures were completed. Participants completed a consent form,
pretest measures, and demographic information at the beginning of the
2010-2011 academic year. Participants were then asked to complete a
posttest at the end of the academic school year. At pretest,
participants provided a code consisting of the last four digits of their
social security number. The four-digit code allowed pretest data to be
matched with posttest data in a way that ensured confidentiality. No
incentive was offered at the beginning of the year (Time 1)
administration, and a $2 incentive was offered at the end of the year
(Time 2) administration. All questionnaires from the pretest and
posttest were returned within the academic year the study was conducted.
A total of 218 students completed the pretest at the beginning of
the academic year and 157 students who completed the pretest also
completed the posttest (72% completion rate). Of these, 55 (35%) were
male and 102 (65%) were female. The mean age was 27 (SD = 6.0), with a
minimum of 20 and maximum age of 55. There were 51 first-year students
(32.5%), 49 second-year students (31.2%), 36 third-year students
(22.9%), 16 fourth-year students (10.2%), and 5 fifth-year students
(3.2%). The majority, 108 respondents, reported their ethnicity to be
European-American (68.8%), followed by 12 Hispanic/Latino (7.6%), 12
Asian-American/Pacific Islander (7.6%), 11 African-American (7.0%), and
1 Native American (0.6%). Eleven participants reported themselves as
other ethnicity (7.0%).
Measures
Locus of Control. Levenson's (1974) Multidimensional Locus of
Control Scale is a 20-item self-report scale, which asks participants to
choose the determinants of their life reinforcements. Three factors
include "Powerful Others Control," "Internal
Control," and "Chance Control." The reliability for the
scale is adequate (Coefficient alpha: P scale = .77, I scale = .64, C
scale = .78). The items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 ("Strongly Disagree") to 5 ("Strongly
Agree"). As used in the Wong-McDonald and Gorsuch's (2000)
research, 8 items assessing God Control were added to the measure.
Surrender Scale. Wong-McDonald and Gorsuch's (2000) Surrender
Scale is a 12-item measure of religious coping. Surrender involves
actively releasing one's will to trust in God's providence and
goodness. Items are based on a 5point Likert-type scale ranging from 1
("Strongly Disagree") to 5 ("Strongly Agree"). The
Surrender Scale was derived from 30-items originally written based on
the biblical concept of surrender (Matt. 10:39; John 10:10). The 12-item
version of the scale has high internal consistency, with Wong-McDonald
and Gorsuch (2000) reporting a Cronbach's alpha of .94.
Spiritual Assessment Inventory. Hall and Edwards's (2002)
Spiritual Assessment Inventory (SAI) is a five factor, 47-item scale
measuring two dimensions of spiritual development based on a
relationship with God. The two dimensions are Awareness of God and
Quality of Relationship with God. The internal consistency of the
47items is high (Cronbach's alpha: Awareness = .95, Disappointment
= .90, Realistic Acceptance = .83, Grandiosity = .73, Instability =
.84). The items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1
("Not True At All") to 5 ("Very True").
Religious Problem-Solving Scale. The Religious Problem-Solving
Scale (Pargament et al., 1988) is a 36-item scale that measures how one
distinguishes the different degrees of responsibility assigned to self
or God in solving problems and the level of initiative taken in problem
solving. The measure provides three different subscales entitled
Self-Directing, Collaborative, and Deferring. Items are based on a
5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ("Never") to 5
("Always"). The internal consistency of the 36 items is high
(Cronbach's alpha: Self-Directing = .91, Collaborative = .93,
Deferring = .89).
Demographics. Participants were also asked their sex, year in
doctoral training, age, highest degree completed, racial/ethnic
identity, religious denomination, and frequency of attending church
services. The question measuring frequency of church attendance was
taken from Koenig, Parkerson, and Meador's (1997) Duke University
Religion Index (DUREL). The church attendance item ranged from 1
("More than once a week") to 6 ("Never"). Also,
participants were asked how important religion is to them with a single
item ranging from 1 ("Not at all. I have no religion.") to 5
("Extremely important. It is the center of my life.").
Results
Descriptive statistics for each subscale are reported in Table 1.
In order to determine the changes among graduate students, a series of
mixed-model repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were
computed--one for each of the various scales used as dependent
variables. Each of these ANOVAs had a repeated measures factor (the
scores at the beginning and end of the academic year) and a
between-groups factor (the students' year in the program). We
recognize the sizable risk of Type I error that comes with multiple
hypothesis tests, but opted to maintain an alpha of .05 because of the
exploratory nature of this research. As such, significant differences
should be viewed with some caution.
With regard to internal locus of control, both a repeated-measures,
F (1,152) = 4.69, p = .032, and between-groups, F (4,152) = 4.21, p =
.003, effect was found. No significant interaction effects were found.
Students reported an increase of internal control between the beginning
and end of the academic year. Post-hoc comparisons using the Least
Squared Difference (LSD) test revealed that Internal locus of control
was higher for third-year students than for second-year students. No
differences were noted for the External, Powerful Other, or God Control
scales.
Further analyses suggested that differences might exist between
first-year students and all subsequent years. To increase power, a new
binary variable was created to indicate if a student was in the first
year of his or her doctoral program or a subsequent year. This new
binary variable was then used as the between-groups factor in subsequent
analyses. A significant difference was found for the God Control scale
between first years and subsequent years, F (1,154) = 7.07, p = .009.
On the Surrender Scale, which is a measure of religious coping,
differences between groups was noted, but only when clustering second-,
third-, fourth-, and fifth-year students together and comparing them
with first-year students, F (1,154) = 4.79, p = .030. First-year
students reported a greater experience of Surrender than students in
subsequent years. No changes over time or interaction effects were
observed.
With the SAI, a significant decrease from the beginning to the end
of the academic year was found on the Awareness scale, F (1,152) = 7.31,
p = .008. The Awareness scale is designed to measure the
individual's ability to recognize God's communication to self
and through self. Although a significant between-groups effect was not
observed, when the independent variable was collapsed to the binary
variable described earlier, first-year students reported more Awareness
than subsequent students, F (1,155) = 7.64, p = .006. No interaction
effects were found. Similarly, no between-group differences were noted
for the Disappointment, Realistic Acceptance, Grandiosity, and
Instability scales unless the independent variables were collapsed into
the same binary variable in which case Realistic Acceptance was higher
for first-year students than for students in subsequent years, F (1,
146) = 5.69, p = .018, and also Grandiosity, F (1,155) = 3.98, p = .048.
No interaction effects were found. No repeated-measures differences or
interaction effects were observed for these scales. The SAI also
includes an Impression Management Scale, which is intended to assess the
extent to which respondents are trying to give a favorable impression of
their spirituality. Impression Management scores decreased from the
beginning to end of the year, F (1,152) = 9.49, p = .002. A
between-groups differences was also noted, F (4, 152) = 2.62, p = .037,
with Impression Management being significantly lower for second-year
students than for first-year students (based on a LSD post hoc test). An
interaction effect was also observed with students in the first year
decreasing more significantly from pretest to posttest than students in
subsequent years, F(4, 152) = 3.22, p = .014.
The Self-Directing subscale of the Religious Problems Solving Scale
showed no significant difference over time. using the binary cohort
variable, a significant difference was noted between the first years and
subsequent years, F (1,155) = 8.024, p = .005, with first-year students
reporting less self-directed problem solving than students in subsequent
cohorts. On the collaborative scale a significant difference was found
between program years, F (4, 152) = 2.51, p = .044. Post hoc comparisons
using the LSD test revealed that first-year students reported more
collaborative religious problem solving than second-year students. The
Deferring scale from the Religious Problem Solving Scale also revealed
significant differences between program years, F (4,152) = 2.83, p =
.027, with a LSD post hoc comparison indicating higher scores for
first-year students than for fourth-year students. No differences were
noted over time for the Collaborative and Deferring scale, nor were
interaction effects observed.
On the Importance of Religion item, a significant difference was
found between the beginning and end of the year, F (1,135) = 5.27, p =
.023, with students reporting being more committed at the beginning of
the academic year than at the end of the school year. Cohort differences
were observed when using the binary variable as the between-groups
variable, with first-year students reporting greater importance of
religion than those in subsequent years, F (1,138) = 5.84, p = .017. No
significant interaction effects were found.
Regarding church attendance, students reported more frequent
attendance at the beginning of the school year than at the end, F
(1,146) = 5.94, p = .016. A between-groups difference was also observed,
F (4,146) = 2.79, p = .028, and post hoc LSD tests revealed that
first-year students reported more church attendance than second- or
fourth-year students. A significant interaction was also observed, F
(4,146) = 2.50, p = .045, with first-year students showing a greater
reduction in church attendance from pretest to posttest than other
cohorts.
Summary of Findings
on numerous measures we observed a change in reported spiritual
perceptions and behaviors from the beginning to the end of the academic
year. While students reported an increased internal locus of control,
they also reported decreased importance of religion, awareness of
God's presence, and church attendance from pretest to posttest. We
also noted that differences between cohorts, when they occurred, always
involved first-year students differing from students in other cohorts.
These differences were noted for God Control, Surrender, Awareness and
Realistic Acceptance of God's work in one's life, all three
types of religious problem solving, importance of religion, and church
attendance. To some extent these differences may be related to higher
Grandiosity and Impression Management scores on the SAI among first-year
students, but is unlikely to account for the magnitude of these
consistent differences. The possibility of an ideological shift in
spiritual perception, particularly one that occurs during the first year
of training, prompted the second study. We expected that perceived
social support and/or fatigue may be related to depleted spiritual
awareness during the first two years of training.
Study 2
Participants and Procedures
Participants were first- and second-year graduate students drawn
from four of the same explicitly Christian doctoral training programs
involved in Study 1. As with Study 1, faculty collaborators were
identified at each of the four programs and after required institutional
review procedures these faculty members helped recruit participants for
the study during the 2012-2013 academic year. Participants completed a
consent form along with a pretest questionnaire packet that included the
last four digits of their social security numbers within the first three
weeks of the fall, 2012 semester. They then completed a posttest
questionnaire at the end of the academic year. The four-digit code
allowed pretest data to be matched with posttest data. No incentive was
offered for participating.
A total of 175 students completed the pretest at the beginning of
the academic year and 140 students who completed the pretest also
completed the posttest (80% completion rate). Of these, 47 (34%) were
male and 93 (66%) were female. The mean age was 25 (SD = 3.9), with a
minimum of 20 and maximum age of 47. There were 76 first-year students
(54.3%) and 64 second-year students (45.7%). The majority, 96
respondents, reported their ethnicity to be European-American (69.1%).
An additional 17 respondents described themselves to be Hispanic/Latino
(12.2%), 13 Asian-American/Pacific Islander (9.4%), 5 African-American
(3.6%), and 3 Native American (2.2%). Five participants reported
themselves as other ethnicity (3.6%).
Measures
Spiritual Assessment Inventory. As with Study 1, Hall and
Edwards's (2002) Spiritual Assessment Inventory (SAI) was
administered in Study 2.
Multidimensional Social Support Survey.
The Multidimensional Social Support Survey (Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet,
& Farley, 1988) is a 12item scale measuring the level of perceived
support from Family, Friends, Significant Other, and Total Support. The
instrument was adapted to include perceived Faculty Support, making it a
16item scale with four subscales plus a total score. Items are based on
a 7-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 ("Very Strongly
Disagree") to 7 ("Very Strongly Agree"). Cronbach's
alpha for total support is 0.88. Test-retest reliability coefficients
are 0.72 (significant other), 0.85 (family), and 0.75 (friends).
Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory. The Multidimensional Fatigue
Inventory (Smets, Garssen, Bonke, & Haes, 1995) measures five
dimensions of fatigue: General Fatigue, Physical Fatigue, Mental
Fatigue, Reduced Activity, and Reduced Motivation. The instrument
consists of 20 items and uses a 5-point Likert-type scale where 1 is
"No, that is not true" and 5 is "Yes, that is true."
Cronbach's alpha for the five subscales are 0.83 (General Fatigue),
0.81 (Physical Fatigue), 0.82 (Reduced Activity), 0.71 (Reduced
Motivation), and 0.86 (Mental Fatigue).
Demographics. Participants were also asked their sex, year in
doctoral training, age, highest degree completed, racial/ethnic
identity, frequency of attending church services, and frequency of
prayer. Frequency of attending church was rated on 6-point scale ranging
from 1 ("Never") to 6 ("More than once a week").
Note that the wording of this item differs from the item used in Study
1, such that Study 1 church attendance scores are inversely related to
reported church attendance and in Study 2 church attendance scores are
directly related to reported attendance. Frequency of prayer was rated
on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 ("Never") to 6 ("More
than three times a day"). As with Study 1, participants were asked
how important religion is to them with a single item ranging from 1
("Not at all. I have no religion.") to 5 ("Extremely
important. It is the center of my life.").
Results
Descriptive data for each subscale are reported in Table 2. As with
Study 1, a series of mixed-model repeated measures analyses of variance
(ANOVAs) were computed. Each of these ANOVAs had a repeated measures
factor (the scores at the beginning and ending of the academic year) and
a between-groups factor (being first- or second-year in the program).
Again, an alpha of .05 was used because of the exploratory nature of
this research.
No differences were reported for either the between-groups or
repeated-measures factor on the MSPSS Significant Other, Family Support,
or Friends Support scales. On the Faculty Support scale, students
reported greater levels of faculty support at the end of the academic
year than at the beginning, F (1,138) = 12.03, p = .001. No
between-groups or interaction effects were found.
A repeated-measures effect was found for General Fatigue, F (1,138)
= 15.79, p < .001, with fatigue at the end of the academic year being
significantly higher than at the beginning. However, this effect appears
to be due to the higher end-of the-year fatigue levels experienced by
second-year students as evidenced by a significant interaction effect, F
(1,138) = 7.33, p = .008. First-year students did not report increased
general fatigue from the beginning to the end of the year. A similar
pattern was seen with Reduced Motivation. A repeated-measures effect was
found, F (1,138) = 17.53, p < .001, but the reduced motivation over
time was fully accounted for by changes in second-year students, F
(1,138) = 5.31, p = .023. A repeated-measures effect revealed increased
levels of Physical Fatigue over time, F(1,138) = 5.17, p = .024, and
also increased Mental Fatigue, F (1,138) = 8.06, p = .005. No
between-groups or interaction effects were observed. No
repeated-measures or between-groups effects were observed on Reduced
Activity.
On the SAI, no between-groups effects were found for the Awareness,
Realistic Acceptance, or Instability scales. On the Disappointment scale
a repeated measures effect was found, with participants reporting
greater disappointment with God at the end of the year than at the
beginning, F(1,138) = 6.44, p = .012. Similarly, scores on the
Grandiosity scale were higher at the end of the year than at the
beginning, F (1,138) = 4.79, p = .030. Impression Management scores were
lower at the end of the year than at the beginning, F (1,138) = 7.75, p
= .006. No between-groups or interaction effects were found for any of
the SAI scales.
Participants reported less church attendance at the end of the year
than at the beginning, F (1,138) = 7.88, p = .006, and they also
reported praying less frequently, F (1,137) = 8.31, p = .005, and
finding religion to be less important than at the beginning of the year,
F (1,110) = 5.67, p = .019. No between-groups or interaction effects
were observed for self-reported church attendance, prayer, or importance
of religion.
In designing Study 2, we wondered if fatigue or lack of social
support might be mediating variables for changes in religious
commitment. This does not appear to be the case as neither yearend
Social Support (total) nor year-end General Fatigue was correlated with
any of the religious commitment variables (prayer, church attendance, or
importance of religion) at the end of the year.
Summary of Findings
Table 3 offers a summary of the repeated measures findings from
Studies 1 and 2, and Table 4 provides a summary of the between-groups
findings. All of the between-groups findings in Table 4 are from Study
1, as Study 2 did not reveal any differences between first and
second-year students.
Discussion
Collectively, the findings from Studies 1 and 2 suggest that
students gain in internal attributions over the course of an academic
year, and retreat from spiritual attributions, religious coping, and
religious activities. This is consistent with Edwards' (2006)
longitudinal findings that showed decreased faith commitment throughout
training in an explicitly Christian doctoral program.
It is tempting to view the findings from Study 1 as longitudinal
data and suggest that students come in with stronger faith commitments
than they leave graduate school with, but such a conclusion stretches
beyond what can be properly inferred because the longitudinal nature of
the studies reported here are limited to one academic year. Moreover,
the numerous differences between cohorts in Study 1 were not found in
Study 2. In part, this may be related to the restricted sampling range
in Study 2, where only first-and second-year students were surveyed.
What might account for the changes in faith that occurred over an
academic year in both studies? We present several possibilities here,
recognizing that all of them call for additional research and
conversation.
Eroding of Faith
Underlying assumptions common in professional psychology may
contribute to some eroding of faith (Slife & Reber, 2009). Students
may experience incongruence in their faith narrative as exposed to the
more naturalistic and modernistic perspectives inherent in contemporary
psychology. Perhaps even explicitly Christian doctoral programs fail to
fully explore the clash of narratives students face in their training,
and students resolve this by becoming more naturalistic in their
assumptions and attributions.
Relatedly, students encounter pain and struggles in their clients
and patients that may end up affecting their ways of understanding the
world. Difficult questions may be raised in students' minds as they
attempt to make meaning of the suffering they see in others (Kunst,
Bjorck, & Tan, 2000).
Enhanced Self-Efficacy
We would expect students to gain increasing confidence in their own
abilities throughout an academic year. Increases in internal locus of
control bring a greater sense of mastery and intrinsic affective reward
(Leotti & Delgado, 2011). Students' ability to self-direct also
becomes apparent as they feel they can problem-solve and handle
situations. Students may feel a lesser need for God as a result, or
perhaps they developed a more nuanced view of attributions that enhances
their sense of self-efficacy.
This calls for more research. As students gain an internal sense of
control, and greater self-efficacy, to what extent does it affect the
way they understand and experience God? In one sense, greater
self-efficacy could lend itself well to a collaborative understanding of
faith. For those who come into graduate school with a more deferential
understanding of faith, this shift may be disconcerting, and perhaps
healthy. Again, this needs further investigation before any conclusions
can be drawn.
Rearranging Faith
Students may rearrange their faith throughout an academic year.
This rearrangement could be as simple as how faith is reported, or as
complicated as developing an increasingly complex understanding of God.
With regard to reporting faith experiences, students report more
socially acceptable answers in the beginning of the school year than at
the end--especially first-year students. As students become less
concerned about impression management, this may allow for greater
transparency regarding their authentic relationship with God. Given the
explicitly Christian nature of the graduate programs studied, students
may feel internal pressure to appear more spiritual than they actually
believe themselves to be (Mullis, 2008). The changes observed in these
studies may actually be changes in how spiritual and religious
experiences are reported more than changes in religious and spiritual
beliefs and activities.
In further exploring the concept of rearrangement of faith,
researchers may choose to examine spiritual impression management
closely. This may mirror other pressures in doctoral training where
students also feel a need to impress. The discoveries regarding
impression management in the current study may lead future researchers
to determine the schemas developed in impressing professors, supervisors
or cohort members. A qualitative study might be helpful in exploring
students' transition to graduate school, as well as exploring the
spiritual experience students have throughout an academic year.
At a more complex level, students may also be reconstructing an
understanding of self and God in the process of studying psychology from
a Christian perspective. If faith is rearranged during graduate training
in explicitly Christian programs, this may bode well for the future
spiritual formation of students in these programs. Pearce (1996) found
many students had moved toward a more relational, intimate, and
interactive relationship with God during graduate school, and in a
9-year follow-up on Pearce's study Hofer (2004) found participants
had an even deeper, stronger, and more important relationship with God
after completing the program. Again this calls for more research,
perhaps especially narrative research on students' experiences of
faith. What helps particular graduate students move toward more
relational, intimate, and interactive ways of knowing God, and how can
this be fostered in our training programs?
Fatigue
A fourth possible explanation relates to limited physical stamina
and overall self-care of students. A rigorous academic schedule can be
demanding, with some level of renewal occurring in summer months. Time
demands may also squeeze out behaviors that promote spiritual
development, such as church attendance and private devotional
reflection. Edwards (2006) reported a decrease in church attendance over
the course of training, and both studies reported here showed less
church attendance at the end of an academic year than at the beginning.
In Study 1, we found an interaction effect with first year students
declining over the academic year more than other students. This could
suggest that students who are inclined to stop attending church do so
right away in training, which results in smaller declines from the
beginning to the end of an academic year among more advanced cohorts.
The ubiquity of fatigue among students will not surprise training
directors, faculty members, or students. The demands of graduate school
can require sacrificing in other areas of life in order to manage
responsibilities. Many students have families, part-time jobs, and other
demands outside of the program. Initiating one's own spiritual
support during the four years may be a challenge for some students. This
fatigue hypothesis was our default explanation after seeing the results
of Study 1, but the lack of relationship between fatigue and religious
behavior variables in Study 2 argues against it.
Conclusion
From the studies reported here it is clear that students report
changes in religious coping, attributions, understandings of God, and
religious behaviors over the course of an academic year. There may also
be changes over the years of training, perhaps especially between the
first and second year of training, though four- or five-years
longitudinal designs will be essential to determine the nature of these
changes.
Graduate school is often experienced as a complex blend of
adventure, exciting new growth, stress, challenge, and even depletion.
Amidst all the changes that occur throughout an academic year, and
throughout five years or more of doctoral training, it seems reasonable
that spiritual changes will occur. We are just beginning to learn the
nature of these changes. As continued research allows us to understand
them better, we may be able to craft training programs to better meet
the spiritual formation needs of students.
References
Edwards, C. J. (2006). Measuring how doctoral students' God
concepts change across 3 years of a religiously based clinical
psychology program. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Biola
University, La Mirada, CA.
Hall, T. W., & Edwards, K. J. (2002). The spiritual assessment
inventory: A theistic model and measure for assessing spiritual
development. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41, 341-357.
Hofer, S. L. (2004). The impact of life on former students'
God concepts: A nine-year longitudinal study. (Unpublished doctoral
dissertation). Biola University, La Mirada, CA.
Johnson, W. B., & McMinn, M. R. (2003). Thirty years of
integrative doctoral training: Historic developments, assessment of
outcomes, and recommendations for the future. Journal of Psychology and
Theology, 31, 83-96.
Koenig, H. G., Parkerson, G.R., & Meador, K. G. (1997).
Religion index for psychiatric research: A 5-item measure for use in
health outcome studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 885.
Kunst, J. L., Bjorck, J. P., & Tan, S-Y. (2000). Causal
attribution for uncontrollable negative events. Journal of Psychology
and Christianity, 19, 47-60.
Leotti, L., & Delgado, M. (2011). The inherent reward of
choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1310-1318.
Levenson, H. (1974). Activism and powerful others: Distinictions
within the concept of internal-external control. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 38, 377-383.
Mullis, R. J. (2008). Minding and measuring changes in graduate
students' God images across three years of a religiously based
program in spiritual formation. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
Biola University, La Mirada, CA.
Pargament, K. I., Kennell, J., Hathaway, W., Grevengoed, N.,
Newman, J., & Jones, W. (1988). Religion and the problem-solving
process: Three styles of coping. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 27, 90-104.
Pearce, R. (1996). The impact of graduate school training in
psychology on students' concept of God: A qualitative study.
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Biola University, La Mirada, CA.
Slife, B.D., & Reber, J.S. (2009). Is there a pervasive bias
against theism in psychology? Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical
Psychology, 29, 63-79.
Smets, E. M. A., Garssen, B., Bonke, B., & De Haes, J. C. J. M.
(1995). The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric
qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. Journal of Psychosomatic
Research, 39, 315-325.
Wong-McDonald, A., & Gorsuch, R. L. (2000). Surrender to God:
An additional coping style? Journal of Psychology and Theology, 28,
149-161.
Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K.
(1988). The Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 52, 30-41.
Laura K. Fisk
Marcel H. Flores
Mark R. McMinn
George Fox University
Jamie D. Aten
Wheaton College
Peter C. Hill
Biola University
Theresa Clement Tisdale
Kevin S. Reimer
Azusa Pacific University
Vickey Maclin
Regent University
Winston Seegobin
Kathleen Gathercoal
George Fox University
Correspondence concerning this article can be sent to Laura K.
Fisk, 414 N. Meridian St., #V104, Newberg, OR 97132; lfisk@georgefox.edu
Laura K. Fisk (PsyD in Clinical Psychology, George Fox University)
is currently a Postdoctoral Resident at Villa Medical Clinic and
Chehalem Medical Clinic in Newberg, OR. Her research interests include
spiritual growth among doctoral students and health psychology.
Marcel H. Flores (MA in Clinical Psychology, George Fox University)
is Assistant Professor of Psychology at LCC International University in
Lithuania where he and his wife are serving as missionaries. His
research interests include the integration of Christianity and
psychology, psychological trauma and resiliency, and neuropsychological
assessment.
Mark R. McMinn (PhD in Clinical Psychology, Vanderbilt University)
is Professor of Psychology at George Fox University (OR). His research
interests include psychology-church collaboration, technology and
practice, and a positive psychology of food.
Jamie D. Aten (PhD in Counseling Psychology, Indiana State
University) is the Founder and Co-Director of the Humanitarian Disaster
Institute and Dr. Arthur P. Rech and Mrs. Jean May Rech Associate
Professor of Psychology at Wheaton College (Wheaton, IL). Dr.
Aten's interests include the psychology of religion and disasters,
disaster spiritual and emotional care, and faith-based relief and
development.
Peter C. Hill (PhD in Social Psychology, University of Houston) is
Professor of Psychology at the Rosemead School of Psychology, Biola
University (CA) and editor of the Journal of Psychology and
Christianity. Dr. Hill's interests include measurement issues, the
integration of psychology and religion, the psychology of religion, and
positive psychology.
Theresa Clement Tisdale (PhD in Clinical Psychology, Rosemead
School of Psychology, Biola University) is Professor of Graduate
Psychology at Azusa Pacific University (CA) and a third year candidate
at Newport Psychoanalytic Institute (CA). Her academic, clinical, and
research interests include psychoanalysis, psychodynamic psychotherapy,
spirituality and spiritual formation, and the integration of
spirituality/religion in clinical practice.
Kevin S. Reimer (PhD, Fuller Graduate School of Psychology) is
Executive Director, Office of Research & Grants, Azusa Pacific
University. His research program considers religion and culture in
humanitarian exemplarity.
Vickey L. Maclin (PsyD in Clinical Psychology, Regent University)
is an adjunct faculty member in Clinical Psychology Program at Regent
University and an adjunct faculty in Master's in Counseling
Programs at South University (VA) and Gordon Conwell Theological
Seminary (NC). Her research and clinical interests are with children and
adolescents, as well as community research with older adults, and
multicultural issues.
Winston Seegobin (PsyD in Clinical Psychology, Central Michigan
University) is Associate Professor of Clinical Psychology at George Fox
University (OR). His research interests include children's
spirituality in cross-cultural contexts, positive psychology and
multicultural psychotherapy, and hope and resilience.
Kathleen A. Gathercoal (PhD in Developmental Psychology, Case
Western Reserve University) is Professor of Psychology at George Fox
University (OR). Her research interests include research methodology and
history of psychology.
Table 1
Means (and Standard Deviations) for Pretest and
Posttest Subscales in Study 1
Year 1 (N = 51) Year 2 (N = 49)
Pre Post Pre Post
Scale/Subscale
Locus of Control
Powerful Others 2.5 (.4) 2.6 (.5) 2.5 (.4) 2.5 (.5)
Internal 3.4 (.5) 3.5 (.4) 3.5 (.5) 3.6 (.5)
Chance 2.2 (.5) 2.4 (.4) 2.3 (.4) 2.3 (.4)
God Control 4.0 (.6) 3.9 (.6) 3.8 (.6) 3.8 (.7)
Religious Coping
Surrender 3.9 (.5) 3.9 (.5) 3.6 (.7) 3.7 (.8)
SAI
Awareness 3.8 (.6) 3.4 (.7) 3.3 (.9) 3.2 (.9)
Realistic Accept 4.3 (.8) 4.2 (.8) 3.9 (.9) 3.9 (.8)
Disappointment 2.4 (.9) 2.7 (.9) 2.6 (1.0) 2.5 (.9)
Grandiosity 1.6 (.5) 1.4 (.5) 1.4 (.5) 1.3 (.4)
Instability 1.9 (.6) 1.9 (.6) 1.9 (.6) 1.8 (.5)
Imp Manage 2.6 (.8) 2.2 (.8) 2.2 (.7) 2.0 (.8)
RPS
Collaborative 3.6 (.6) 3.4 (.6) 3.1 (.7) 3.2 (.7)
Self-Directing 2.3 (.6) 2.6 (.6) 2.8 (.8) 2.8 (.9)
Deferring 2.6 (.6) 2.4 (.5) 2.3 (.6) 2.3 (.7)
Other
Importance 4.6 (.6) 4.5 (.6) 4.3 (1.0) 4.1 (1.0)
Church Attend 1.9 (.8) 2.3 (.8) 2.5 (1.4) 2.7 (1.2)
Year 3 (N = 36) Year 4 (N = 16)
Pre Post Pre Post
Scale/Subscale
Locus of Control
Powerful Others 2.4 (.5) 2.4 (.5) 2.6 (.5) 2.5 (.3)
Internal 3.7 (.4) 3.8 (.4) 3.8 (.4) 3.9 (.3)
Chance 2.1 (.5) 2.3 (.5) 2.3 (.4) 2.4 (.4)
God Control 3.8 (.5) 3.6 (.6) 3.7 (.5) 3.7 (.7)
Religious Coping
Surrender 3.8 (.6) 3.8 (.6) 3.7 (.5) 3.8 (.4)
SAI
Awareness 3.2 (.8) 3.2 (.9) 3.4 (.6) 3.2 (.6)
Realistic Accept 3.9 (1.0) 3.8 (.9) 3.9 (.8) 3.8 (.8)
Disappointment 2.7 (1.1) 2.5 (1.0) 3.4 (.7) 2.3 (.7)
Grandiosity 1.5 (.5) 1.4 (.5) 1.3 (.4) 1.2 (.2)
Instability 1.9 (.6) 1.8 (.7) 1.8 (.7) 1.8 (.5)
Imp Manage 2.0 (.7) 1.9 (.7) 2.3 (.7) 2.3 (.8)
RPS
Collaborative 3.3 (.7) 3.2 (.7) 3.3 (.6) 3.2 (.6)
Self-Directing 2.8 (.7) 2.8 (.7) 2.5 (.7) 2.7 (.6)
Deferring 2.1 (.5) 2.2 (.5) 2.1 (.6) 2.1 (.7)
Other
Importance 4.3 (.8) 4.2 (.9) 4.3 (1.0) 3.9 (1.2)
Church Attend 2.5 (1.0) 2.5 (1.0) 3.1 (1.3) 2.9 (1.4)
Year 5 (N = 5)
Pre Post
Scale/Subscale
Locus of Control
Powerful Others 3.1 (.7) 2.8 (.5)
Internal 3.6 (.3) 3.7 (.3)
Chance 2.6 (.6) 2.4 (.5)
God Control 3.7 (.2) 3.6 (.6)
Religious Coping
Surrender 4.0 (.5) 3.8 (.2)
SAI
Awareness 3.6 (.9) 3.5 (1.0)
Realistic Accept 4.0 (.9) 3.9 (.9)
Disappointment 3.7 (1.3) 3.7 (1.6)
Grandiosity 1.4 (.3) 1.4 (.2)
Instability 2.0 (.5) 1.9 (.6)
Imp Manage 2.0 (.8) 1.8 (.7)
RPS
Collaborative 3.6 (.7) 3.5 (.6)
Self-Directing 3.0 (.6) 2.7 (.7)
Deferring 2.2 (.7) 2.1 (.5)
Other
Importance 4.6 (.6) 4.4 (.6)
Church Attend 2.2 (1.1) 3.1 (.7)
Notes. Scores are reported as Means (Standard Deviations). Year 1
to Year 5 refers to the participant's year in doctoral training.
SAI = Spiritual Assessment Inventory. Imp Manage = Impression
Management. RPS = Religious Problem Solving Scale. Importance =
Importance of Religion. Church Attend = Church Attendance.
Table 2
Means (and Standard Deviations) for Pretest and Posttest
Subscales in Study 2
Year 1 (N = 76) Year 2 (N = 64)
Scale/Subscale Pre Post Pre Post
Social Support
Significant Other 6.1 (1.1) 6.0 (1.1) 6.0 (1.4) 5.9 (1.3)
Family 5.9 (1.2) 5.9 (1.1) 5.7 (1.4) 5.5 (1.4)
Faculty 4.6 (1.0) 4.9 (1.1) 4.5 (1.3) 4.9 (1.4)
Friends 5.9 (1.0) 5.8 (.9) 5.9 (1.1) 5.7 (1.1)
Fatigue
General 2.8 (.6) 2.9 (.8) 2.8 (.7) 3.1 (.8)
Physical 2.2 (.9) 2.3 (.9) 2.1 (.8) 2.3 (.9)
Reduced Activity 2.1 (.7) 2.0 (.6) 1.9 (.6) 2.0 (.8)
Reduced Motivation 2.0 (.6) 2.1 (.6) 2.0 (.6) 2.4 (.7)
Mental Fatigue 2.6 (.9) 2.6 (.8) 2.4 (.8) 2.7 (.8)
SAI
Awareness 3.6 (.7) 3.6 (.7) 3.6 (.9) 3.4 (1.0)
Realistic Accept 4.1 (.8) 4.0 (.9) 4.2 (.8) 4.1 (.9)
Disappointment 2.8 (1.1) 3.0 (1.1) 3.1 (1.2) 3.2 (1.1)
Grandiosity 1.6 (.6) 1.8 (.6) 1.5 (.4) 1.6 (.6)
Instability 2.1 (.7) 2.3 (.8) 2.1 (.6) 2.1 (.7)
Impression Manage 2.7 (.7) 2.5 (.7) 2.5 (.8) 2.3 (.8)
Other
Importance of Rel 4.6 (.6) 4.5 (.6) 4.3 (1.0) 4.1 (1.0)
Church Attendance 4.7 (1.2) 4.5 (1.3) 4.6 (1.2) 4.4 (1.3)
Prayer Frequency 5.5 (1.3) 5.3 (1.4) 5.6 (1.3) 5.4 (1.4)
Notes. Scores are reported as Means (Standard Deviations). Year 1
and Year 2 refers to the participant's year in doctoral training.
SAI = Spiritual Assessment Inventory. Importance of Rel =
Importance of Religion. Note that the responses given for church
attendance and prayer frequency get higher with more reported
activity. In contrast, the church attendance score in Study 1
was inversely related to frequency of church attendance reported.
Table 3
Significant Differences from Beginning to End of Academic Year
Finding Study
Increased Internal Locus of Control Study 1
Decreased Awareness of God (SAI) Study 1
Increased Disappointment with God (SAI) Study 2
Increased Grandiosity (SAI) Study 2
Decreased Impression Management (SAI) Studies 1 and 2
Increased Sense of Faculty Support Study 2
Increased Fatigue (General *, Motivation *, Study 2
Physical, Mental)
Decreased Church Attendance Studies 1 and 2
Decreased Frequency of Prayer Study 2
Decreased Importance of Religion Studies 1 and 2
Note. * In these instances, a significant increased fatigue
occurred for second year students, but not for first year students.
Table 4
Significant Differences from Beginning to End of Academic Year
Finding
Internal Locus of Control higher for 3rd year students than 2nd year
God Control higher for 1st year students than for others
Surrender higher for 1st year students than for others
Awareness of God (SAI) higher for 1st year students than for others
Realistic Acceptance (SAI) higher for 1st year students than
for others
Grandiosity (SAI) higher for 1st year students than for others
Impression Management (SAI) higher for 1st year students than for
2nd year students *
Self-directed Problem Solving less for 1st year students than
for others
Collaborative Problem Solving higher for 1st year students than
for others
Deferring Problem Solving higher for 1st year students than for
4th year students
Importance of Religion higher for 1st year students than for others
Church Attendance higher for 1st year students than for 2nd or
4th year students **
Notes.
* An interaction effect shows that Impression Management
decreased more during the training year for first year students
than for students in subsequent years.
** An interaction effect a greater reduction over the academic
year for students in the first year than for students in
subsequent years.