首页    期刊浏览 2024年09月21日 星期六
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Strengths assessment, academic self-efficacy, and learning outcomes in a Christian University sample.
  • 作者:Sutton, Geoffrey W. ; Phillips, Sheri ; Lehnert, Alina B.
  • 期刊名称:Journal of Psychology and Christianity
  • 印刷版ISSN:0733-4273
  • 出版年度:2011
  • 期号:March
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:CAPS International (Christian Association for Psychological Studies)

Strengths assessment, academic self-efficacy, and learning outcomes in a Christian University sample.


Sutton, Geoffrey W. ; Phillips, Sheri ; Lehnert, Alina B. 等


Associated with the recent surge of interest in positive psychology is the hypothesis that students are more productive learners when the focus is on their strengths rather than their weaknesses (Clifton, Anderson, & Schreiner, 2006). In a recent meta-analysis, Robbins et al. (2004) reported moderate relationships between psychosocial and study skill (PSS) factors and two common measures of college outcomes: cumulative grade point average (GPA) and retention. Of the PSS factors, the best predictors of GPA were academic self-efficacy and achievement motivation. In addition, PSS factors yielded incremental contributions to the prediction of college outcomes beyond the traditional predictors of standardized tests and high school GPA. In this article, we report the results of two studies that examine the contribution of strengths to college outcomes. Specifically, we examined the contribution of strengths as defined by the StrengthsFinder (SF) instrument in combination with standardized test scores (American College Test, ACT; Scholastic Assessment Test, SAT) and academic self-efficacy (ASE). We concluded with a discussion of the role of strengths-assessment in a college population.

Strengths

The idea of recognizing a person's talents or strengths associated with productivity has a long history. There are references to Chinese examinations used to select people for government service nearly 4,000 years ago (Aiken & Groth-Marnat, 2006). In addition, Hebrew texts report that Huram's strengths in working with bronze led to his employment in Israel's first Temple (1 Kings 7:13). In the last century, Maslow (1943) philosophized about the potential for human growth, once we transcend our basic needs. By 1990, Csikszentmihalyi published Flow, which summarized two decades of research on the intense feelings of enjoyment people experience when they employ their talents in the pursuit of challenging goals.

In this article, we focused on strengths, defined by Buckingham and Clifton (2001), as "... consistent near perfect performance in an activity" (p. 25). Specifically, they operationally defined strengths using the StrengthsFinder instrument, a 180-item assessment. The responses are associated with 34 strength themes. The user's report provides the top five strength themes along with descriptive information about each theme.

Two characteristics of the SF pose a challenge for those interested in evaluating the contribution of the SF. First, the SF measures individual strengths and includes ipsative strategies rather than a strictly normative approach. Second, the SF authors stress the value of considering five salient themes from among 34 strengths rather than other alternatives that might assess contributions from all of the strengths.

The individual differences approach poses a challenge for those accustomed to explaining behavior in terms of normative models. Using an ipsative approach, if two people have empathy as their top strength, there is no guarantee they possess equal amounts of empathy. Conversely, if two people obtain the same empathy score on a normative measure, an observer can assume those individuals perceive themselves to possess approximately the same amount of empathy. One may reasonably ask the question, what is the value of knowing that people perceive they have strengths if, in fact, those self-rated strengths may actually be weaknesses relative to other members of a particular group such as a classroom, task force, or committee? Upon reading the technical notes, it seems the SF is a hybrid measure that combines elements of both normative and ipsative measures (Lopez, Hodges, & Harter, 2005). Clearly, if the SF were a completely ipsative measure, it would be impossible to understand a person based on a list of strengths. Advisors, educators, and employers would be left in a fog of subjectivity that would obfuscate reasonable attempts at the usual activities of advising and guiding learners. However, we argue, in reality, all notions of personality are derived from experiences in social groups. That is, we learn about our propensities for empathy, harmony, and arranging from our experiences with others. We tend to like the things we do well and dislike the things we do poorly. We learn notions of doing well and doing poorly from years of feedback from the adults and peers in our lives. The way people respond to items on the SF is based on what they have learned about themselves in relationship to other people.

Recent validity studies indicate SF theme scores do correspond with subscales on normative measures, including the Big Five, the 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF), and the California Personality Inventory(CPI) (Harter & Hodges, 2003; Schreiner, 2006).

Hayes (2001) provided a technical report on the SF in an Appendix to Buckingham and Clifton's (2001) explanation of the strengths program. Using a question and answer format, Hayes explained the conceptual basis for the SF and referred to a four-dimensional model. "StrengthsFinder is based on a general model of positive psychology. It captures personal motivation (Striving), interpersonal skills (Relating), self-presentation (Impacting), and learning style (Thinking)" (p. 248). Gallup provided a grouping of the 34 strengths into the four dimensions, which we reproduced in Table 1. We found only one unpublished study that evaluated this four-dimensional model of strengths. Brashears and Baker (n.d.) examined the utility of the four SF dimensions along with other admissions data to predict GPA in a sample of 41 students during 2001. They found a high positive correlation between Thinking and SAT (r = .744) and moderate negative correlations between SAT and Relating (r = -.458) and Striving (r = -.461). However, correlations between SF dimensions and the ACT were small. In our studies, we explored the stability of the SF themes in two samples and the intercorrelations between the four SF dimensions, admissions scores, and GPA. In support of the strengths model, and indirect support of the SF as a measurement tool, recent studies found that strengths development activities, based on the SF strength themes, promote educational outcomes (Cantwell, 2005; Lehnert, 2009; Louis, 2008).

Academic Self-Efficacy

Much of the research on self-efficacy stems from the work of Bandura (1977). In general, self-efficacy refers to a belief that one can complete a given task. Bandura found judgments about the degree of self-efficacy are associated with the activities and settings a person chooses (Bandura, 1982). Because self-efficacy judgments influence a person's activities, those judgments can have considerable functional value. For example, when faced with difficult situations, those who have doubts about their own capabilities reduce their efforts or may even give up. However, those who have a strong sense of efficacy use greater effort to overcome those difficult situations and therefore accomplish the task (Bandura, 1982). Anticipation of the types of outcomes largely depends on the beliefs people have about how well they will be able to perform in certain situations. People who are highly efficacious will expect favorable outcomes; whereas, those who are not highly efficacious will expect negative outcomes (Bandura & Locke, 2003).

In our research, we were primarily interested in academic self-efficacy, beliefs students had about their ability to perform well in an educational setting. Researchers found that students with higher self-efficacy tended to be more motivated and earned higher grades (Lent, Brown, & Larkin, 1984; Schunk, 1983). Self-efficacy was also a stronger predictor of academic achievement than hope (Hacket, Betz, Casas, & Rocha Singh, 1992).

More recently, Chemers, Hu, and Garcia (2001) found children with high efficacy persisted longer and used more efficient problem-solving strategies than did children with low efficacy. They also found that students who enter college with confidence in their ability to perform well academically significantly outperformed those students with less confidence. In addition, students with higher expectations for academic success had higher performances. We included a measure of academic self-efficacy in study two to see if this form of self-efficacy would make a significant contribution to explaining academic performance beyond that accounted for by general ability.

Research Overview

We conducted two studies to evaluate the stability of the StrengthsFinder on a Christian university campus and to explore the relationships between the SF themes and measures of academic ability and learning. In addition, we added academic self-efficacy in the second study.

Study 1

Method

Participants. The sample contained 528 valid records (women = 352, men = 176). On average, the women were age 18.88 (SD = .49) and the men were age 19.50 (SD = 1.02). Most students were of European descent (468). Other ethnic backgrounds were African-American (16), Hispanic/ Latino (26), Asian or Pacific Islander (7), and other (11). Most students were from an Assemblies of God religious background (391) but a substantial portion was from various charismatic, nondenominational, and other backgrounds (127).

The database also contained information about scholastic ability and progress. On average, student GPA was 3.23 (sd = .57) and they had earned 39 92 (SD = 19 96) course credits at the time of testing. In addition, combined SAT (M = 1040.20, SD = 162.95) and ACT (M = 23.23, SD = 4.39) scores were available.

Measures. We used the ACT and SAT scores as measures of student learning potential. Most of the students had taken the ACT but a substantial portion had taken the SAT. In this study, we converted the SAT scores into ACT scores based on conversion tables provided by Dorans, Lyu, Pommerich, and Houston (as cited in Schneider & Dorans, 1999). For the small subsample (n = 47) that had taken both tests, the correlation between SAT and ACT scores was r = .84 (p < .001, two-tailed). The correlation between the obtained SAT scores and the scores converted to ACT scores supported the viability of the conversion procedure (n = 154, r = .94, p < .001, two-tailed).

In a technical report, Lopez, Hodges, and Harter (2005) reported research documenting the development and validation of the SF. Internal consistency values were adequate for most strength themes (33 of 34 above coefficient alpha = .65) for a sample of 706 Gallup associates. In another analysis, test-retest values ranged between .60 and .80 for most strength themes. Item validity indicated the average item to assigned strength theme correlations were 6.6 times greater than overall average item to strength theme correlations. Harter and Hodges (2003) conducted a validity study and found support for predicted relationships between select SF strength themes and Big Five personality scales.

Recently, and more relevant to higher education settings, Schreiner (2006) reported reliability data from a 2005 study of 438 students from 14 colleges and universities that revealed mean test-retest reliability of .70 across the 34 SF strength themes. The sample produced lower reliability values for Activator (.52), Consistency (.53), and Maximizer (.55). The values for the other 31 scales ranged from .60 to .80. The stability of strength themes was 52% for the top three strength themes remaining in the top five on retesting. The average internal consistency was [alpha] = .61. The sample yielded low internal consistency values (coefficient alpha) for Activator (.42), Arranger (.48), Belief (.57), Consistency (.47), Context (.51), Ideation (.45), Input (.51), Maximizer (.56), Relator (.46), Self-Assurance (.49), Significance (.55), and Strategic (.51). The values for the remaining scales were between .60 and .80.

Schreiner (2006) reported 128 significant correlations between the SF strength themes and subtests from either the CPI or the 16PF, as evidence of construct validity. Only four SF strengths were not significantly related to subscales on other measures (Context, Individualization, Maximizer, Restorative). Hierarchical cluster analysis revealed 95% of the SF item pairs met a criterion of 70% associated with the expected SF strength theme.

Design and procedures. This correlational study is based on an examination of archival data. Thus, ACT and SAT scores along with GPA were extracted from a student database. A student's top five strength themes were obtained from the results of the StrengthsFinder and entered into a database along with their gender, age, and accumulated credits. All names were removed from the database we analyzed.

Because the school only had a list of the top five strength themes for each student, without descriptive statistics such as means and standard deviations, we elected to construct an index based on the aforementioned SF constructs of Relating, Impacting, Thinking, and Striving. We formed the index by adding one point for each person's top five strength themes that were a part of one of the four dimensions. Thus, each person could earn a score ranging from 0 to 5 depending on how many of the 34 strength themes were associated with a particular dimension. Following the study, we found a similar approach in an unpublished manuscript by Brashears and Baker (n.d.).

Results and discussion

Based on the 512 records, the five most frequently occurring strength themes were Belief (182), Adaptability (159), Developer (150), Positivity (135), and Empathy (134). Descriptive statistics for the four strength dimensions of Relating, Impacting, Thinking, and Striving indicated a normal distribution with measures of skew and kurtosis within acceptable limits (+/- 1.5) (George & Mallery, 2006). The intercorrelations for the four dimensions were significantly negatively correlated. In addition, converted ACT and three strengths dimensions (Impacting, Striving, Thinking) were significantly correlated with GPA (see Table 2).

Next, we conducted backwards multiple regression to identify which of the variables that were significantly correlated with GPA might be useful predictors. For the overall model, multiple R = .518 ([R.sup.2] = .268, [R.sup.2.sub.adj] = .262, F(4, 443) = 17.62, p < .001). Only converted ACT scores and Impacting significantly contributed to the model. The Fchange values for models that removed Thinking and Striving were not significant. See Table 3 for the coefficients.

Study 2

In study 2, we attempted to replicate the findings from the previous year (study 1); however, we added a measure of academic self-efficacy because of its value in previous research.

Method

Participants. The sample contained 344 valid records (women = 223, men = 121). On average, the women were age 18.85 (SD = 1.72) and the men were age 20.03 (SD = 4.39). Most students were of European descent (304). Other ethnic backgrounds were African-American (15), Hispanic/Latino (8), Asian or Pacific Islander (8), Native American (2), and other (5). Most students were from an Assemblies of God religious background (208) but a substantial portion was from various charismatic, nondenominational, and other backgrounds (83).

The database also contained information about scholastic ability and progress. On average, the student GPA was 3.21 (SD = .67) and they had earned 30.28 (SD = 22.22) course credits at the time of testing. In addition, SAT (M = 1065.48, SD = 172.83) and ACT (M = 22.83, SD = 4.21) scores were available. As before, we transformed the SAT scores into equivalent ACT scores based on the work of Dorans, Lyu, Pommerich and Houston (as cited in Schneider & Dorans, 1999). Converted ACT scores averaged 22.59 (SD = 4.32) and were significantly correlated with the obtained SAT scores (n = 93, r = .97, p < .001, two-tailed).

Materials. We analyzed the same measures as in the first study except for the addition of academic self-efficacy. We measured academic efficacy using the Academic Self-Efficacy scale (ASE) (Chemers, Hu, & Garcia, 2001). The ASE ([alpha] = .83) is an eight-item scale, which asks participants to rate how well the statements describe them. Items included statements such as "I know how to take notes" and "I am very capable of succeeding at the university." All items were rated on a seven-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (very untrue) to 7 (very true).

Design and procedures. We used the same design as in Study 1; however, we were able to obtain a measure of academic self-efficacy. These measures were collected by the professors of the classes in which students obtained and discussed the results of the StrengthsFinder. All data were entered into an anonymous database for analysis.

Results and discussion

Based on 344 complete records, the five most frequently occurring top five strength themes were Belief (112), Adaptability (101), Restorative (93), Developer (90), and Achiever (85). The first two strength themes were the same as in Study 1. Three of the top five strength themes were the same for both samples. When we examined the top 10 most frequent strength themes, all of them were the same for both samples. Thus, we had reasonable evidence the SF was, on average, a stable measure of strength themes for the student population. See Table 4 for a comparison of strength themes for the two samples.

Descriptive statistics for the four strength dimensions of Relating, Impacting, Thinking, and Striving indicated a normal distribution with measures of skew and kurtosis within acceptable limits (George & Mallery, 2006). The inter-correlations for the four dimensions were significantly negatively correlated. In addition, converted ACT, ASE, and the Impacting strength's dimensions were significantly correlated with GPA (see Table 5).

Next, we conducted backwards multiple regression to identify which of the variables that were significantly correlated with GPA might be useful predictors. For the overall model, multiple R = .548, [R.sup.2] = .30, [R.sup.2.sub.adj] = .29, F(3, 280) = 40.00, p < .001. Only converted ACT scores and ASE significantly contributed to the model (p < .05). The Fchange values for the model that removed Impacting were not significant. See Table 6 for the coefficients.

General Discussion

The stability of strength themes for two samples, obtained a year apart, supports the limited previous findings that the SF measure is relatively stable in a campus population. These findings are interesting because they lead to several observations. First, students attracted to this midsize Christian university from year to year may indeed possess similar strengths; a finding which could have ramifications for marketing, teaching, and retention. Second, the findings may be indicative of the general top five strengths of new college students. Expanded examination of the top strengths of students in colleges nationwide could provide additional information to ascertain whether these top five strengths are unique to this campus or whether they may be indicative of a developmental process.

In addition, the normal distribution of scores and the pattern of correlations provided initial support for the value of the four SF dimensions as indexes. For example, we found significant positive correlations between Thinking and converted ACT scores and significant negative correlations between Impacting and GPA in both samples. So, students motivated primarily by intellectual pursuits (learning) were more likely to have higher ACT scores and students motivated primarily by active pursuits (self-presentation) were more likely to have lower grades. Academic self-efficacy provided significant additional information about GPA beyond college admissions scores alone. In addition, the Impacting dimension of strengths contributed to predicting GPA when controlling for college admissions scores and ASE. Not surprisingly, our findings support previous research that college admissions test scores are positively correlated with GPA (e.g., Garavalia & Gredler, 2002).

What is the context for considering a discussion of strengths? The broad discussion of positive psychology and the focus on identifying strengths as represented in our studies of the SF can be considered in the larger context of identifying and treating symptoms of a disease or identifying and remediating deficits in insight, knowledge, or skills. The paradigms in medicine and psychology have yielded measurable results that can be linked to improvements in health. We would expect most health care providers would agree that an equally important consideration is the identification of strengths and strengths-based activities that can enhance functioning. We suggest several considerations that might frame a fruitful debate on strengths. First, research is needed to identify which model of strengths should guide assessments and interventions. Second, what weight should academic advisors, educators, health care providers, and employers give to models that focus on identifying and remediating human weaknesses versus human strengths. Third, is it possible, or even desirable to develop a multidimensional model that identifies both strengths and weaknesses and offers individuals a broader academic, health, or personal improvement plan based on such a broad assessment?

What is the context for considering our findings? Despite the widespread use of SF on Christian and secular university and business campuses, the PsycINFO database contained no published peer-reviewed studies of the SF. We did find evidence that some researchers recently investigated the value of strengths interventions (Cantwell, 2005; Louis, 2008; Lehnert, 2009) in their dissertation research and had employed the SF to identify the strengths, which were related to the interventions. Given the data from Gallup researchers, usage levels, and unpublished research, there is evidence that Gallup's strengths model has been accorded value as both an assessment of human strengths and as a tool in developing strengths-based activities that could be linked to other outcomes valuable to educators and employers. Because the creators of SF have tied the instrument to positive psychology (Clifton, Anderson, & Schreiner, 2006) and Christianity (Winseman, Clifton, & Liesveld, 2003), it is incumbent on users to evaluate their measures, learning activities, and other strengths-focused programs on Christian and secular college campuses.

What have we added to the research on strengths and the SF? In this article, we provided some data and a basis for initiating a public debate in peer-reviewed publications about the value of strengths in general, and the SF in particular. We cited the technical reports made available by the Gallup researchers and found evidence that the top five themes are relatively stable on a Christian university campus. We also added a minimal amount of evidence to the construct validity of the four dimensions as noted in the small correlations we reported. Moreover, the prominence of the belief strength offers some support for construct validity on a faith-based campus. However, we note, the SF belief strength is not associated with the content of one's beliefs. Nevertheless, further evaluation of belief in other religious populations could be a promising endeavor.

What are the limitations of our research? Clearly, our studies were limited by the restrictive characteristics of the Midwestern Christian university sample. In addition, we did not have a wide array of instruments in the database that might be considered in a broad-based evaluation of the construct validity of the SF and the four-dimensional model.

What recommendations can be offered for users of the SF? Given the evidence from Gallup scientists about the SF instrument and the recent dissertation research, there is an emerging basis to support ongoing research into the value of strengths-based advising and intervention programs applicable to both secular and Christian university programs. Because the ASE measure is short and adds predictive value, it would be relatively easy for schools to include this measure as a part of an advising process and to construct local norms.

What questions might be addressed by further research? In addition to addressing the broad questions about strengths and weaknesses, noted above, we hope additional studies will add to the psychometric properties of the SF and improvements of future versions of the instrument.

Additional research is needed to identify effective components of strengths-based advising and strengths-based teaching that may be linked to outcomes in education and industry. Further, strengths-based programs need to be compared to specific components of programs that address both strengths and weaknesses, especially in the context of academic advising and career counseling. Moreover, we can conceive of a role for strengths-based psychotherapy, which has the potential to move beyond the remediation of impairments in mental status and relationships (based on traditional models of insight or skill development) to a focus on enhancing strengths.

References

Aiken, L. R., & Groth-Marnat, G. (2006). Psychological testing and assessment (12th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 82, 191-215.

Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-147.

Bandura, A. & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 87-99.

Brashears, M.T., & Baker, M. (n.d.). A comparison of the influence of traditional predictors and individual student talents upon collegiate success: A longitudinal study. Proceedings of the Southern Agricultural Education Research Conference, February 1-3, Mobile, AL.

Buckingham, M. & Clifton, D. O. (2001). Now, discover your strengths. New York, NY: Free Press.

Cantwell, L. D. (2005). A comparative analysis of strengths-based versus traditional teaching methods in a freshman public speaking course: Impacts on student learning and academic engagement. Dissertation Abstracts International, 67(02A), 478-700. (UMI No. 3207574).

Chemers, M. M., Hu, L., & Garcia, B. F. (2001). Academic self-efficacy and first-year college student performance and adjustment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 55-64.

Clifton, D. O., Anderson, E. C., & Schreiner, L. A. (2006). StrengthsQuest: Discover and develop your strengths in academics, career, and beyond (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: The Gallup Organization.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Garavalia, L. S., & Gredler, M. E. (2002). Prior achievement, aptitude, and use of learning strategies as predictors of college student achievement. College Student Journal, 36, 616-625.

George, D. & Mallery, P. (2006). SPSS for Windows step by step (6th ed.). New York, NY: Allyn & Bacon.

Hackett, G., Betz, N. E., Casas, J. M. & Rocha-Singh, I. A. (1992). Gender, ethnicity, and social cognitive factors predicting the academic achievement of students in engineering. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39, 527-538.

Harter, J. K., & Hodges, T. D. (2003). Construct validity study: StrengthsFinder and the five factor model (technical report). Omaha, NE: The Gallup Organization.

Hayes, T.L. (2001). Appendix: A technical report on StrengthsFinder. In M. Buckingham & D. O. Clifton, Now, discover your strengths. New York, NY: Free Press.

Leach, C. W., Queirolo, S. S., DeVoe, S. E., & Chemers, M. (2003). Choosing letter grade evaluations: The interaction of students' achievement goals and self-efficacy in the choice to receive letter grades. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 495-509.

Lehnert, A. B. (2009). The influences of strengths-based development of leadership practices among undergraduate college students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 70(10), 148. (UMI No. AAT3377758).

Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1984). Relation of self-efficacy expectations to academic achievement and persistence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 31, 356-362.

Lopez, S. J., Hodges, T., & Harter, J. (2005). The Clifton StrengthsFinder technical report: Development and validation. Washington, DC: The Gallup Organization.

Louis, M. C. (2008). A comparative analysis of the effectiveness of strengths-based curricula in promoting first-year college student success. Dissertation Absracts International, 69(06A). (UMI No. 3321378).

Maslow, A. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370-396.

Robbins, S.B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 261-288.

Schneider, D., & Dorans, N. J. (1999). Concordance between SAT I and ACT scores for individual students. New York, NY: College Board.

Schreiner, L. (2006). A technical report on the Clifton StrengthsFinderTM with college students. Omaha, NE: The Gallup Organization.

Schunk, D. H. (1983). Ability versus effort attributional feedback: Differential effects on self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(6), 848-856.

Winseman, A. L., Clifton, D. O., & Liesveld, C. (2003). Living your strengths: Discover your God-given talents, and inspire your congregation and community. Washington, DC: The Gallup Organization.

Geoffrey W. Sutton

Sheri Phillips

Alina B. Lehnert

Bradley W. Bartle

Priscilla Yokomizo

Evangel University

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Geoffrey W. Sutton, Ph.D., Evangel University, 1111 N. Glenstone Ave., Springfield, MO 65802 Suttong@evangel.edu

Note

(1.) StrengthsFinder[R] and Clifton StrengthsFinder[TM] and each of the 34 themes are trademarks of the Gallup Organization and refer to the measure used in this study. The measure is available from the Gallup Organization, www.gallup.com.

Authors

Geoffrey W. Sutton, Ph.D. is a professor of psychology at Evangel University. He is a licensed psychologist who works as a medical consultant in psychology for the Social Security Administration, Disability Determinations Division. His research interests focus on positive psychology and Christian spirituality.

Sheri Philips, Ph.D. is an assistant professor and director of career development at Evangel University in Springfield MO. Her Ph.D. is in higher education leadership from Azusa Pacific University. She holds a B.S. from Evangel College and an M.A. in counseling from Wheaton College.

Alina B. Lehnert, Ph.D. is an assistant professor of organizational leadership and associate director of leadership and strength development at Evangel University. Her Ph.D. is in organizational leadership from Regent University. She holds an M.S. in counseling from Missouri State University and a bachelor's degree from Evangel University.

Bradley W. Bartle, B.S. was a psychology senior fellow at Evangel University when the study was completed. He now lives and works in Illinois.

Priscilla Yokomizo, B.S. was a psychology senior fellow at Evangel University when the study was completed. She now lives and works in Colorado and is pursuing additional education.
Table 1

StrengthsFinder Themes Associated with Dimensions

Relating            Impacting     Thinking        Striving

Communication       Command       Analytical      Achiever
Empathy             Competition   Arranger        Activation
Harmony             Developer     Connectedness   Adaptability
Includer            Maximizer     Consistency     Belief
Individualization   Positivity    Context         Discipline
Relator             Woo *         Deliberative    Focus
Responsibility                    Futuristic      Restorative
                                  Ideation        Self-Assurance
                                  Input           Significance
                                  Intellection
                                  Learner
                                  Strategic

Note. Four dimensions suggested by Winseman, Clifton, & Liesveld
(2003).

* Woo = Winning Others Over.

Table 2

Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations for Study 1

Variable          1         2         3

1. Relating      --
2. Impacting   -.13 **     --
3. Thinking    -.48 **   -.46 **     --
4. Striving    -.36 **   -.30 **   -.28 **
5. ACT         -.17 **   .14 **    .21 **
6. GPA          -.04     -.16 **    .10**
N                521       521       521
M               1.32      1.00      1.30
SD               .90       .85      1.04

Variable         4       5       6

1. Relating
2. Impacting
3. Thinking
4. Striving     --
5. ACT          .07      --
6. GPA         .08 *   .50 **    --
N               521     450     518
M              1.37    22.54    3.05
SD              .87     4.07    .75

Note. ACT = Converted American College Test Score,
GPA = Undergraduate Grade Point Average.
* p < .05, ** p < .01 (one-tailed)

Table 3

Multiple Regression Analysis of Variables Explaining GPA in Study 1

Variable          B      SE    Beta      t        p

Converted ACT   .095    .008   .503    12.008   < .001
Impacting       -.129   .047   -.146   -2.759    .006
Thinking        -.06    .040   -.081   -1.521    .129
Striving        -.024   .043   -.028   -.568     .570

Note. The criterion variable was undergraduate GPA and n = 450.

Table 4

Table of Top Ten Strengths

Frequency   2005 Strength    Rank   Frequency   2006 Strength

182         Belief            1        112          Belief
159         Adaptability      2        101       Adaptability
150         Developer         3        93        Restorative
135         Positivity        4        90         Developer
134         Empathy           5        85          Achiever
121         Includer          6        84          Empathy
109         Input             7        80           Input
109         Responsibility    8        76       Responsibility
109         Restorative       9        75         Positivity
102         Achiever          10       74          Includer

Note. Frequency and ranking of the top 10 strengths
from 2005 (n = 521) and 2006 (n = 344).

Table 5

Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations for Study 2

Variable          1         2        3       4

1. Relating      --
2. Impacting   -.16 **     --
3. Thinking    -.43 **   -.42 **     --
4. Striving    -.33 **   -.32 **   -.33 *    --
5. ACT          -.08       .07     .21 **   .09
6. ASE         -.09 *    -.13 **    .12     .07
7. GPA          -.04     -.13 **    .07     .06
N                344       344      344     344
M               1.28       .94      1.28    1.50
SD               .91       .90      1.08    .94

Variable          5         6        7

1. Relating
2. Impacting
3. Thinking
4. Striving
5. ACT           --
6. ASE         .24 **      --
7. GPA         .49 **    .39 **      --
N                284       344      340
M               22.60     41.01     3.22
SD              4.32      7.16      .67

Note. ACT = Converted American College Test Score,
GPA = Undergraduate Grade Point Average.

* p< .05, ** p< .01 (one-tailed)

Table 6

Multiple Regression Analysis of Variables Explaining GPA in Study 2

Variable          B      SE    Beta      t        p

Converted ACT   .065    .009   .398     7.45    < .001
ASE             .023    .005   .241    4.494    <.001
Impacting       -.065   .039   -.082   -1.637    .103

Note. Converted ACT = converted American College Test scores.
ASE = Academic Self-Efficacy. N = 284 for this analysis.
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有