Whose psychology? Which rationality? Christian psychology within an ideological surround after postmodernism.
Watson, P.J.
Christians who are psychologists must answer a centrally important
question. Are Christianity and contemporary psychology incommensurable?
Notice that this question in no way implies that the two might be
incompatible (MacIntyre, 1988, 1990). The excellent work of so many
Christian psychologists already makes it clear that the two are quite
compatible to some important degree. To be incommensurable simply means
instead that Christianity and contemporary psychology might fail to
share the same ultimate standard. Without a shared ultimate standard,
the central claims of each cannot be judged definitively along a common
metric of evaluation. Incommensurable perspectives, therefore, operate
from essentially different systems of rationality. Assumptions in one
are warranted if they can be appropriately related to an ultimate
standard of evaluation that is unavailable in the other. So, the
question about incommensurability leads to a perhaps more fundamental
question. Do Christianity and contemporary psychology rest upon
ultimately different systems of rationality?
As interpreted within an ideological surround model of the
relationship between religion and science (Watson, 1993), Christian
Psychology will argue that any adequate response to this question must
have at least four elements. First, an adequate response must explain
why incommensurability is in fact an unavoidable and critical problem
for all Christians working in psychology. Second, it will need to
explain why the problem of incommensurability may be so difficult for
some Christians to see or, if seen, to admit. Third, if it is to be
fully effective, any adequate response to this question must make sense
within the current cultural context. That context is sometimes described
as "postmodern." The argument of Christian Psychology is that
postmodernism cannot be ignored, but that Christian psychologists will
need to move beyond postmodernism toward more productive cultural
possibilities. Finally, Christian Psychology will argue that any fully
adequate response to this question must include a tentative model of how
we might all work together to meet the challenges of incommensurability
in the future.
Necessary Incommensurability
First, Christian Psychology assumes that Christianity and
psychology are necessarily incommensurable. For a psychology to be truly
Christian, the final standard of evaluation must presumably be Christ.
That standard is obviously unavailable within a secularized social
science like psychology. For the secularized scientific disciplines of
today, the usually implicit and sometimes explicit ultimate standard of
evaluation is nature (see e.g., Cunningham, Riches, Lehman, &
Hampton, in press).
Empirical observations of nature are evaluated by holding them up
to contemporary understandings of what nature represents. Nothing stands
outside of nature to judge nature. Progress is made as scientific
readings of the world dynamically clarify and are clarified by current
readings of the "theology" of nature. For Christian
psychologists, Christ stands outside of nature to judge nature. Progress
is made as scientific readings of the world dynamically clarify and are
clarified by current readings of the theology of Christ. Christian and
secular psychologists, therefore, might agree. Incommensurability is a
rather obvious and relatively noncontroversial empirical reality.
But, perhaps it is unfair to presume how others might view this
issue. Arguments might exist for placing both Christ and the
naturalistic assumptions of secular psychology under the evaluation of a
shared higher standard. That standard could then make it possible to
judge both Christ and psychology along a common metric of evaluation.
But such arguments will also need to explain how this approach can
support the development of a truly Christian psychology, rather than a
psychology of the proposed higher standard. If such arguments exist,
they should be listened to carefully and evaluated fairly. Christian
Psychology, nevertheless, remains skeptical. The likelihood seems remote
that a psychology without Christ as its ultimate standard could still
operate as a truly Christian psychology.
In short, for Christian Psychology, the most likely conclusion
seems clear. Christianity and contemporary psychology are founded upon
different ultimate standards. They, therefore, operate as
incommensurable, though not necessarily as wholly incompatible systems
of rationality.
Difficulties Seeing Incommensurability
Second, Christian Psychology suspects that other Christian
approaches to psychology insufficiently address the challenge of
incommensurability. Conflicts about the role of psychology in
Christianity often seem to focus on whether or not the two can be
compatible. But again, that is the wrong question. Incommensurability,
not incompatibility, is the problem. It also may be a problem that is be
difficult for some Christians to see.
MacIntyre (1988, 1990) has made the issue of incommensurability a
central concern in his philosophy. Most of us, he suggests, live within
the conceptual framework of a single perspective. When this is so,
"the problem of understanding the position of the other will appear
as a problem of translation: how can we render their beliefs, arguments,
and theses into our terms" (MacIntyre, 1990, p. 111). Such
individuals will not see incommensurability as even a potential problem.
Their confident assumption will be that all propositions of another
perspective can be expressed within their own language of understanding.
A scientific worldview may suggest, for example, that all religious
assertions can be translated as either rational or irrational relative
to current understandings of nature as the final standard. A Christian
worldview may instead claim that all scientific assertions can be
translated as either faithful or sinful relative to one or another
current Christian understanding of the Bible. Those who see a high
correspondence between the rational and the faithful and between the
irrational and the sinful will assume that Christianity and psychology
are largely compatible. Those who instead claim that the rational often
corresponds to the sinful and the irrational to the faithful will
describe the two as largely incompatible. Less polarized positions are
of course possible. Most importantly, however, this focus on the degree
of compatibility makes it virtually impossible to see incommensurability
as even a potential problem.
But even when Christians could see the potential difficulty, they
may be tempted to ignore it. To take the problem of incommensurability
seriously might seem to require an unacceptable surrender to relativism.
Incommensurability does, after all, suggest the existence of isolated
rationalities that are incapable of meaningful dialog with and critique
of other rationalities. MacIntyre (1990) argues, however, that
relativism is not a necessary consequence of incommensurability.
Progress in addressing the problems of incommensurability requires
people who are fluent in the rationalities of multiple perspectives.
Such individuals will understand from the outset that one perspective
cannot rationally defeat the other, because the two forms of
understanding are calibrated to incommensurable standards of evaluation.
Such individuals will try instead to use one perspective to out-narrate
all other positions that see translation as the only solution to
conflicts between perspectives. Relativism can be avoided. The
productive response to incommensurability is not to use reason, but
rather narration.
Christian Psychology can, therefore, be conceptualized as an
ongoing project to narrate the problem of incommensurability that
necessarily confronts Christians who work in psychology. Christian
Psychology needs to tell a story that refuses to ignore the challenges
of incommensurability, that avoids the dangers of relativism, and that
suggests how psychology might function with Christ as the ultimate
standard. That story will also need to narrate our current cultural
context and then attempt to narrate our future so that we can all begin
to understand how to collaborate in expanding the reach of Christian
rationality.
Narrating the Cultural Context
A focus on incommensurability obviously presupposes that scientific
forms of rationality must have limits. Again, incommensurability simply
means that the ultimate standards of secular science do not and cannot
be those of Christianity and vice versa. Each, in other words, operates
within the boundary conditions or limits of its own standards. Those
convinced of the full adequacy of the sciences to judge even
Christianity will want to reject this kind of thinking. This rejection
will at least implicitly reflect a modernist faith in the sciences that
is increasingly challenged by the influential arguments of postmodernism
(see e.g., Erickson, 2001; Smith, 2006). The postmodern belief is that
all scientific observations are based upon non-empirical, theory-laden
pre-judgments. Hence, science like religion rests upon normative values
and, therefore, can never be fully objective. Scientific
"faith" in nature as the ultimate standard is, for example, a
normative judgment. This increasingly postmodern cultural context has at
least four implications for efforts to address the problem of
incommensurability.
First, postmodern critiques make it clear that science like
Christianity is therefore ideological (Watson, 1993). As defined by
MacIntyre (1978), ideologies are somewhat non-empirical, normative, and
sociological systems of belief. Christianity assumes that God created
the universe. Science argues that the universe began with the Big Bang.
All kinds of empirical observations can be organized around each
assumption, but neither God nor the Big Bang can be definitively
confirmed nor falsified scientifically. These somewhat non-empirical
assumptions will then have normative consequences. An ideology
"does not merely tell us how the world is and how we are to act,
but is concerned with the bearing of the one upon the other"
(MacIntyre, 1978, p. 6). Christian and scientific pursuits of the
"truth," for example, will necessarily reflect methods that
are consistent with their very different non-empirical assumptions. All
of this has sociological implications as well. As MacIntyre (1978)
points out, "There is a Christian account of why Christians are
Christians and the heathens are not" (p. 6). Science too will have
accounts of who is a "believer" within the scientific
community and who is a "heathen." In short, any adequate
approach to the problem of incommensurability will begin with an
understanding that both Christianity and secular psychology necessarily
operate within different ideological surrounds. Formal attention to the
influence of these differing ideological surrounds will, therefore, be
essential.
Second, many Christians psychologists will want to move beyond
postmodernism. Postmodern critiques can seem to trap Christians in an
untenable situation. A historically naive pre-modern reading of
Christianity cannot easily address the realities of an increasingly
pluralistic and scientific world. A full embrace of modernist
rationality seems ill suited to solve the unavoidable problems of
incommensurability. Postmodernism seems to accurately describe the
contemporary cultural context, but also points toward a disturbing
relativism. If everything is ideological, then presumably nothing and
everything can be "true" at the same time. None of this seems
to be acceptable.
In response to this situation, Christians have begun to call for
the development of a "post-postmodernism." Erickson (2001)
writes, for example, "We must work toward a post-postmodernism, not
simply ignoring the phenomenon of postmodernism, and reverting back to a
pre-postmodernism, but also not halting with postmodernism" (p.
293). What would a Christian post-postmodernism look like? Greer (2003)
emphasizes that "in the post-postmodern paradigm, absolute truth
has a name: Jesus Christ" (p. 217). In other words, Christian
psychologists can embrace the problem of incommensurability as an
opportunity. They can begin with the assumption that Christ supplies the
absolute ultimate standard for a psychology and then develop the
potentials of a different rationality based upon that standard.
Post-postmodern Christian psychologists will, therefore, understand
that a relativity of perspectives is an undeniable empirical reality of
the contemporary cultural context. They will also know, however, that
relativity as an empirical reality does not dictate relativism as a
normative standard. Relativism as a norm must be overcome. This cannot
occur through use of a scientifically objective rationality. A wholly
"objective" scientific rationality simply cannot exist. The
task instead will be to challenge relativism with an increasingly
compelling Christian narration of psychology and rationality.
Third, a post-postmodern response to incommensurability will
require the social construction of what might be called a
meta-perspective. In other words, Christians in psychology will need to
create a perspective of understanding that is developed at a higher
level of abstraction. The need for a meta-perspective was in fact
suggested by the philosopher who stood at the origins of postmodernism,
Friedrich Nietzsche (1887/1967). While passionately criticizing
Christianity, Nietzsche also rejected the claim that science could ever
be fully "objective." Science, he argued instead, invariably
reflects perspectives motivated by diverse "subjective"
interests. Hence, a scientific view on any particular issue is
invariably limited by its interests. For Nietzsche, the solution to this
problem is what he called a "future objectivity." In the
future, "objectivity" will require openness to diverse
"interests" and the use of "a variety of perspectives and
affective interpretations in the service of knowledge" (Nietzsche,
1887/1967, p. 119). In the service of Christian knowledge, a
post-postmodern approach to psychology will need to socially construct a
meta-perspective that uses "future objectivity" to bring all
relevant perspectives into conformity with a rationality that takes
Christ as the ultimate standard.
Finally, therefore, a Christian Psychology will want to collaborate
with all Christians in psychology to narrate a future in which the
problem of incommensurability is embraced as an opportunity. That
narration will attempt to construct a future objectivity that includes
three most important elements: Christ as the ultimate standard, a
meta-perspective that evaluates the potential contributions of all
Christian psychologists relative to that ultimate standard, and the
various perspectives that psychologists use in the service of Christian
and other knowledge.
Narrating the Future: A Tentative Model
An ideal narration of the future must, therefore include the
important contributions of all perspectives that Christians use in their
work as psychologists. More specifically, further development of
Christian rationality as it relates to psychology will require the
social construction of a meta-perspective that is capable of evaluating
Christian perspectives that represent three very different forms of
intellectual endeavor: the etic, the emic, and the dialogic.
In anthropology, etic research perspectives involve a study of
human communities using the "outside," relatively more
objective methods of science (e.g., Headland, Pike, & Harris, 1990).
Postmodern critiques make it clear that etic research can never be
wholly objective. Etic research programs, nevertheless, maintain a
useful distance from issues that can make it possible to see things more
broadly than is sometimes possible within the too-close perspectives of
persons living within the community itself. Individuals within a
Christian community, in other words, may become so enmeshed within one
or another approach to a question that they are unable to see the full
possibilities of their own Christian rationality.
Etic perspectives, therefore, have the obvious advantage of using
powerful scientific methodologies to empirically clarify Christian
concerns. To some degree, the Levels of Explanation approach to
Christianity and psychology may illustrate the etic approach (e.g.,
Myers, 2010). As described by Johnson (2010),
proponents of this approach maintain
that all levels of reality are important
(the physical, chemical, biological,
psychological, social and theological),
that each dimension or level of
reality is accessible to study by methods
that are appropriate to it that
have been developed by the corresponding
discipline, and that the
boundaries of each discipline, therefore,
should not be blurred. (p. 33)
Levels of Explanation research thus makes it possible to use the
vast array of etic, scientific "methods that are appropriate"
to clarify the concerns of Christian Psychology. At the same time,
however, an etic perspective could promote a colonization of Christian
communities by scientific value systems. This is so because somewhat
non-empirical assumptions based upon nature as the ultimate standard can
lead to very different normative conclusions than those reflecting
somewhat non-empirical assumptions based upon Christ as the ultimate
standard. In short, the etic perspective has clarification as its
advantage and colonization as its potential disadvantage.
In anthropology, emic research perspectives involve the study of
communities using the "inside," less "objective"
perspectives of the community itself. For Christians working in the
service of knowledge, emic research programs seek to express forms of
psychological understanding that are possible within the faith itself.
The Bible and the available texts and traditions of interpretation will
be at the center of methodologies designed to enhance the articulation
and actualization of Christian community. At the same time, however,
Christian emic research programs could be so deeply embedded within
theologically conditioned and overly narrow perspectives on the faith
that they could encourage retreat into increasingly isolated and
besieged ghettos of interpretation. To some degree, the Biblical
Counseling perspective, with its emphasis on relying upon the Bible and
its skepticism about the role of scientific research, may illustrate the
emic perspective (e.g., Powlison, 2010). In short, the emic perspective
has actualization as its advantage and ghettoization as its potential
disadvantage.
For Christians working in psychology, etic and emic research
programs will be necessary, but not sufficient. Etic scientific and emic
Christian perspectives must be brought into conversation. Dialogic
research programs pursue this essential task. Such research programs
explicitly explore the possibilities of translating scientific
psychological insights into the language of Christianity, and vice
versa. Dialogic research could, nevertheless, produce what might be
described as a "dysinterpretation" of the conversational
possibilities. Dysinterpretations would presumably have at least some
conceptual and empirical foundations of support, and so could not be
called simple misinterpretations. Dysinterpretations would instead
reflect deviations from normatively ideal translations, in other words
deviations from the ultimate standard defined by Christ. Two
possibilities seem most obvious. One deviation could tend toward an etic
colonization of the faith, whereas the other could encourage an emic
ghettoization. To some degree, the Integration approach to psychology
and Christianity may illustrate the dialogic perspective (e.g., Jones,
2010). In short, dialogic research programs have translation as their
advantage and dysinterpretation as their potential disadvantage.
Questions for the Narration of Christian Psychology
This conceptual framework now makes it possible for Christian
Psychology to begin narrating the future by answering three important
questions. Those questions will address concerns about how Christians in
psychology can embrace the problem of incommensurability by socially
constructing the rationality of their own meta-perspective.
First, what must Christians in psychology do in order to construct
their Christian meta-perspective? The answer should now be clear in very
general terms. With Christ as their ultimate standard of evaluation,
Christians in psychology will need to develop concepts and methodologies
that are useful in promoting clarification, actualization, and
translation while simultaneously avoiding colonization, ghettoization,
and dysinterpretation.
Second, which Christians in psychology will need to construct this
meta-perspective? The answer is that all Christians in psychology will
need to contribute. The Levels of Explanation, Biblical Counseling,
Integration, Transformational, and all other views will each need to
create knowledge that is useful in fulfilling the positive potentials of
each perspective. In addition, however, advocates of these views will
also need to express arguments that rise above their own perspective and
make points about their discoveries at the higher level of a Christian
meta-perspective.
Finally, what is the role of Christian Psychology in all this?
Christian Psychology will embrace the task of developing the Christian
meta-perspective as a formal responsibility. Progress in this task will
be obvious in the creation of an increasingly sophisticated and uniquely
Christian rationality about psychology. That rationality will need to
work within two contexts. Within the Christian community itself,
Christian Psychology will want to develop conceptual and methodological
innovations that are useful in promoting clarification, actualization,
and translation and in avoiding colonization, ghettoization, and
dysinterpretation. However, Christian Psychology will also understand
the importance of working outside the Christian community. To limit
Christian rationality within the boundaries of its own community would
promote an unacceptable cultural ghettoization of Christian
perspectives. In opposition to that possibility, Christian Psychology
would hope to articulate an ever more compelling Christian rationality
that could enter into increasingly productive conversations with all
other rationalities that influence the discipline of psychology and the
wider world.
Conceptual and Methodological Innovations
As it narrates the problem of incommensurability, Christian
Psychology will be interested not only in the insights that are
available from etic, emic, and dialogic research programs. A perhaps
more basic goal will be to develop concepts and methodologies that are
useful in building up the communal meta-perspective of all Christians
who work in psychology. With Christ as the ultimate standard, Christian
Psychology will use the Bible and Christian texts and traditions of
interpretation to define an increasingly dynamic and sophisticated
Christian rationality. A "theology of clarification," for
example, may be essential in helping the Christian community know how to
obtain the full and faithful benefits of the Levels of Explanation
perspective. Theological evaluations of the potential problems of
ghettoization may be essential in actualizing the promise of, for
example, Biblical Counseling. Dysinterpretation as a possible danger of
the Integration perspective is likely to be a complex phenomenon that
will require the careful analysis of a sophisticated Christian
rationality.
Empirical research will be essential as well. The Christian
Psychology research program will assume that science, like religion,
operates within an ideological surround. This means that science will
not have access to an unambiguous "objectivity" that is
unavailable to religion. Both science and religion will instead have
incommensurable, though not necessarily incompatible, forms of
objectivity. Each form of objectivity will operate within its own
normatively defined ideological surround. Awareness of that fact will
have two most important implications for Christian Psychology.
Operationalizing the Tradition
First, Christian Psychology will seek to encourage the development
of measures and procedures that directly express the rationality of a
Christian ideological surround. In other words, Christian Psychology
will identify operationalization of the Christian tradition as a
centrally important research objective.
Traditional Christianity assumes, for example, that achievement of
better psychological functioning requires awareness of personal
sinfulness. Beliefs about Sin Scales have attempted to express four
dimensions of this awareness (Watson, Morris, Loy, Hamrick, &
Grizzle, 2007). The Self-Improvement Scale is reflected in the claim,
"My beliefs about sin have helped me work on my weaknesses."
Illustrating Perfectionism Avoidance is the statement, "Knowledge
of my personal sinfulness has lifted the burden from my shoulders of
trying to be perfect." An example of Healthy Humility is the
belief, "My awareness of sin helps me maintain an appropriate
humility." Self-Reflective Functioning includes such statements as,
"My beliefs about sin have made it possible for me to be more
objective about myself."
Use of these scales made it possible to demonstrate that Beliefs
about Sin in fact measure psychological health in largely Christian
samples (Watson et al., 2007). Specifically, these four scales
correlated with greater self-esteem and lower levels of narcissism,
depression, and anxiety. Simultaneous use of these measures to predict
mental health also produced unexpected results. Self-Improvement proved
to be centrally important in defining the mental health benefits of
Beliefs about Sin. Indeed, Healthy Humility turned out to be unhealthy
when statistical procedures simultaneously accounted for the influence
of Self-Improvement. This latter result seemed especially important in
spotlighting the essential contribution of empirical research in
understanding how Christian rationality operates today. When working
with clients, Christian therapists and Biblical Counselors presumably
would need to be aware of the possible importance of Self-Improvement
and of the potential vulnerabilities of an unmitigated Healthy Humility.
Such knowledge would not be easily available except through the use of
social scientific procedures.
And of course, the Christian assumption is that the ultimate
solution for the problem of sin lies in God's grace.
Operationalizing the tradition, therefore, clearly requires the creation
of another scale for assessing personal beliefs in and experience of
God's grace. Such an instrument was recently developed (Sisemore et
al., 2010), and its use along with the Beliefs in Sin Scales made it
possible to empirically explore theologically sophisticated questions
about relationships among sin, grace, and psychological well-being
(Watson, Chen, & Sisemore, 2010).
Operationalizing the tradition would be only one aspect of a
Christian Psychology research program. Again, the ideological nature of
research has a second important implication for Christian Psychology.
New methodologies need to examine the incommensurable rationalities of
Christianity and contemporary psychology. Five procedures have been
developed for that purpose thus far: direct rational analysis, empirical
translation schemes, correlational marker procedures, comparative
rationality analysis, and statistical controls for ideology.
Direct Rational Analysis
Direct rational analysis may be especially useful in documenting a
potential secular psychological bias against Christian rationality
(Watson, Hood, & Morris, 1988). A scale reflecting a nonreligious
existential ideology measures whether someone tends to maintain an
unhealthy existential avoidance of the harsher realities of life. Those
realities include meaninglessness, suffering, and death. Correlational
evidence in fact demonstrates that this scale can predict both sincere
Christian commitments and anxiety. Such data could, therefore, seem to
point toward a linkage of Christian commitments with an anxious refusal
to confront harsh existential realities.
However, this scale associates "existential avoidance"
with an affirmation that "God exists" and with the belief that
it is "quite certain what happens after death." Instead of
merely measuring existential avoidance, these questions within a
Christian ideological surround may instead reveal the somewhat
non-empirical and anti-religious assumptions of an existentialist
ideological surround. This is so because most orthodox Christians
presumably would regard belief in God and certainty about what happens
after death as reflecting a healthy confrontation with the harsh
realities of life.
A direct rational analysis of all statements within this scale,
therefore, suggested that some items contradicted Christian faith and
essentially defined an "anti-Christian" subscale. Other items
appeared to be unobjectionable within a Christian ideological surround
and hence could be described as a "Christian-neutral"
subscale. A reanalysis of the data using these subscales revealed that
the association of sincere Christian faith with "existential
avoidance" was limited solely to the anti-Christian items. The
Christian neutral subscale explained the linkage of existential
avoidance with anxiety. As a consequence, the initial finding of a
correlation between existential avoidance and sincere Christian
commitments merely demonstrated that the original scale was biased
against Christian beliefs in God and the afterlife. In other words, the
original scale had the potential to colonize Christian commitments in
terms of an existentialist ideological surround.
Empirical Translation Schemes
Christians over recent decades have appropriately emphasized
contrasts between Christian and secular humanistic understandings of
psychological well-being. Such arguments move easily and plausibly
toward the suggestion that humanistic self-actualization is wholly
incompatible with Christian forms of mental health. But is humanistic
self-actualization only more incommensurable than incompatible with
Christianity? Empirical translation schemes helped answer that question
(Watson, Milliron, Morris, & Hood, 1995; also see Watson 2008a, b).
In this procedure, each statement of a humanistic
self-actualization scale was restated in a number of hypothetically
comparable expressions of Christian self-actualization. In largely
Christian samples, an empirically defensible translation became evident
if a potential Christian translation correlated positively with the
original humanistic statement of self-actualization. One humanistic
expression of self-actualization said, for example, "I can like
people without having to approve of them." A successful Christian
translation asserted, instead, "Christ's love for sinners has
taught me to love people regardless of their background and
lifestyle." A Christian Self-Actualization Scale made up of
successful translations predicted higher levels of Christian commitment
and turned out to be more useful than the humanistic scale in assessing
Christian psychological adjustment.
Self-actualization when calibrated to Christ as the ultimate
standard, therefore, proved to be an even more predictive and thus valid
measure of Christian psychological well-being. In other words,
humanistic articulations of self-actualization were not wholly
incompatible, but rather only incommensurable with Christian
commitments. Such data demonstrate that an extreme emic rejection of
self-actualization could promote a ghettoization of Christian
perspectives. Also worth emphasizing is the fact that this humanistic
scale was at least somewhat successful in predicting Christian mental
health. Researchers could, therefore, legitimately use this particular
secular humanistic scale to measure the self-actualization of
Christians. The empirical translation scheme results, nevertheless,
demonstrated that to do so could lead to a dysinterpretation of
Christian self-functioning. Data based upon this secular scale could
encourage a dysinterpretation, not because they were wholly wrong in
suggesting the possibility of Christian self-actualization, but rather
because they were insufficiently calibrated to Christ as the ultimate
standard and would thus yield less robust empirical findings.
Correlational Marker Procedures
A further analysis of the self-actualization issue used a different
secular humanistic scale along with a correlational marker procedure
(Watson, Morris, & Hood, 1989). This research strategy rests upon
use of a psychological scale that has well established credentials as a
valid measure of Christian commitment. In other words, such a scale
would display what might be called "tradition validity"
because it successfully assesses whether research participants sincerely
try to follow the assumptions of their own Christian rationality. This
instrument, therefore, could serve as a "marker" of
traditional Christian commitments.
In a largely Christian sample, one study correlated a tradition
valid measure of religious commitment with each of 150 separate items
from a different humanistic self-actualization scale. Some humanistic
items correlated positively with the tradition valid scale, and thus
could be "marked" as "pro-Christian." One
"pro-Christian" statement said, "I do not have feelings
of resentment about things that are past." Christian and humanistic
ideological surrounds, therefore, seemed to agree that "feelings of
resentment" interfere with self-actualization. Other humanistic
items correlated negatively with the religious commitment scale, and
thus served as markers of "anti-Christian" belief.
Illustrating these "anti-Christian" assertions was the claim
that "people need not repent their wrongdoings." Hence,
"repentance" was a feature of Christian, but not humanistic
self-actualization. In correlations with other measures,
"pro-Christian" statements more likely predicted adjustment,
whereas "anti-Christian" items pointed toward poorer
psychological functioning. Most notable, however, was the finding that
"pro-Christian" and "anti-Christian" items
correlated negatively with each other. This outcome violated basic
psychometric standards of scale development, which require a strong
positive relationship among all items within a scale.
So was this 150-item humanistic self-actualization scale compatible
or incompatible with Christian commitments? The paradoxical answer was
yes. This scale was both compatible and incompatible, as might be
expected of a measure that was incommensurable with Christ as the
ultimate standard. Inclusion of both "pro-Christian" and
"anti-Christian" items within a single instrument produced a
scale that was largely irrelevant to Christian understandings of
self-actualization. An ideologically naive use of this measure could,
therefore, lead to the misleading conclusion that Christians fail to
achieve the mental health benefits of self-actualization. Such a
conclusion could promote a colonization of Christian beliefs by a
humanistic ideological surround. Within the objectives of an Integration
research program, such data could also support a dystinterpretation of
Christian self-functioning. Advocates of the emic perspective could also
applaud such results as proof that self-actualization has no real place
in Christianity, thereby encouraging a ghettoization of Christian
beliefs. Correlational marker procedures made it possible to empirically
evaluate all of these possibilities within a Christian ideological
surround (also see, Watson & Morris, 2008).
Comparative Rationality Analysis
Comparative rationality analysis is a procedure that asks
Christians to respond to a psychological scale twice (Watson, 2010).
First, they react to questionnaire items under standard conditions,
responding to all items as intended by the creators of the scale. Then,
they respond to these very same items once again, but this time by
evaluating whether each is consistent or inconsistent with personal
religious commitments. Based on these assessments, items can be defined
empirically as "pro-Christian" or as
"anti-Christian." This means that the scale taken under
standard instructions can then be rescored in a different way expressing
the assumptions of a Christian rather than a non-Christian ideological
surround.
For example, one measure of "irrational beliefs" based
upon a secular system of psychotherapy assumes that mental health
requires full independence and that any belief in the necessity of
depending upon others promotes psychopathology (Watson, Morris, Hood,
& Folbrecht, 1990). According to this scale, for example, it is
irrational to claim that "people need a source of strength outside
themselves." Another supposed irrationality says, "I try to
consult an authority on important decisions." Unsurprisingly,
Christians evaluated these beliefs as being rational rather than
irrational, in conformity with their commitment to depend upon God. At
the same time, however, Christian and psychotherapeutic assumptions
agreed that it was rational to believe that "I like to stand on my
own two feet." Once again, a scale based upon an ideological
surround of contemporary psychology proved to be both incompatible and
compatible with Christian beliefs, and hence was incommensurable.
Analysis of the Christian evaluations of dependency and other
so-called irrationalities made it possible to define an overall
Christian system of rationality (Watson, 2010). Most therapeutic-based
assessments of irrationality were ideologically compatible with
Christian beliefs, but some were not. Quantitative comparisons between
the Christian and psychotherapeutic scorings of irrationality once again
revealed that Christian rationality was relatively more valid than the
therapeutic rationality when used with Christians. Overall, these data
revealed that the psychotherapeutic evaluation of dependency had a
potential to colonize Christian commitments and that a naive use of
these irrationality scales could at best encourage a dysinterpretation
of Christian rationality.
Statistical Control for Ideology
Finally, statistical control procedures can examine the
relationship between Christian commitments and psychological functioning
after statistically accounting for possible conflicts between
ideological surrounds. In one project, numerous measures of Christian
commitment were correlated with an array of scales assessing
self-esteem, self-acceptance, and self-actualization (Watson, Morris,
& Hood, 1987). Christian commitments were more likely to predict
unhealthy than healthy self-functioning, although most relationships
proved to be nonsignificant.
In this study, efforts were also made to assess self-actualization
in humanistic terms that were clearly anti-Christian and to assess
Christian beliefs about sin and guilt that were clearly anti-humanistic.
Statistical procedures then made it possible to subtract out the
influences of anti-Christian and anti-humanistic language on the
observed relationships. The result was that Christian commitments became
overwhelmingly linked with healthier self-functioning. Definitive
interpretation of these results is complex and will likely require
additional research. Nevertheless, this outcome demonstrated the
importance of understanding how ideological surrounds can condition
supposedly "objective" empirical assessments of Christian
psychological functioning. Depending upon perspective, a failure to
remain sensitive to the problem of incommensurability could promote
conclusions about the relationship between Christianity and
self-functioning that could encourage colonization, dysinterpretation,
or ghettoization.
Conclusion
In summary, Christian Psychology can be conceptualized as an
attempt to narrate the answer to a centrally important question. Are
Christianity and contemporary psychology incommensurable? When
understood within the ideological surround model of the relationship
between religion and science, Christian Psychology argues that the
empirically obvious and presumably noncontroversial answer must be yes.
The ultimate standard of Christianity is Christ. The ultimate standard
of secular psychological science is not. In the absence of a shared
common ultimate standard of evaluation, the two must be incommensurable
by definition.
To say that Christianity and psychology are incommensurable in no
way means that they will be fully incompatible. Relative to the
incommensurable rationality of contemporary psychology, Christian
rationality will include a vast array of compatible, incompatible, and
unrelated ideological assumptions. Incommensurability, therefore,
presents us not only with a challenge, but also with an opportunity. We
should all work together, embracing both science and our faith to
clarify, actualize, and translate the implications of our Christian
commitments.
We should also collaborate in the social construction of a
meta-perspective. This meta-perspective should help us avoid the
problems of colonization, ghettoization, and dysinterpretation. It will
also encourage us to develop the conceptual and methodological
innovations of a "future objectivity" that we can use in the
service of Christian knowledge. All of us have essential contributions
to make. Our goal should be to nurture the dynamic growth of a Christian
rationality that we can pass on to future generations of Christian
psychologists.
References
Cunningham, C., Riches, A., Lehmann, Z., & Hampson, P. (In
press). Radical Orthodoxy and Christian Psychology: 2 Ontological
naturalism and Christology. Interview with Conor Cunningham and Aaron
Riches. Edification: The Transdisciplinary Journal of Psychology.
Erickson, M. J. (2001). Truth or consequences. Downers Grove, IL:
InterVarsity Press.
Greer, R. C. (2003). Mapping postmodernism. Downers Grove, IL:
InterVarsity Press.
Headland, T. N., Pike, K. L., & Harris, M. (Eds.). (1990).
Emics and etics. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Johnson, E. L. (2010). A brief history of Christians in psychology.
In E. L. Johnson (Ed.) Psychology and Christianity: Five Views (pp.
9-47). Downers Grove, IL: InterVarsity Press.
Jones, S. (2010). An integration view. In E. L. Johnson (Ed.)
Psychology and Christianity: Five Views (pp. 101-128). Downers Grove,
IL: InterVarsity Press.
MacIntyre, A. (1978). Against the self-images of the age. Notre
Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
MacIntyre, A. (1988). Whose justice? Which rationality? Notre Dame,
IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
MacIntyre, A. (1990). Three rival versions of moral enquiry. Notre
Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
Myers, D. G. (2010). A levels of explanation view. In E. L. Johnson
(Ed.) Psychology and Christianity: Five Views (pp. 49-78). Downers
Grove, IL: InterVarsity Press.
Nietzsche, F. (1967). On the genealogy of morals. In W. Kaufmann
(Ed.) On the genealogy of moral and ecce homo (pp. 13-163). New York:
Random House. (Original work published in 1887)
Powlison, D. (2010). A biblical counseling view. In E. L. Johnson
(Ed.) Psychology and Christianity: Five Views (pp. 245-273). Downers
Grove, IL: InterVarsity Press.
Sisemore, T. A., Arbuckle, M., Killian, M., Mortellaro, E.,
Swanson, M., Fisher, R. et al. (2010). Grace and Christian
Psychology--Part 1: Preliminary Measurement, Relationships, and
Implications for Practice. Edification: The Transdisciplinary Journal of
Psychology, 4(2), 57-63.
Smith, J. K. A. (2006). Whose afraid of postmodernism? Grand
Rapids, MI: Baker Academic.
Watson, P. J. (1993). Apologetics and ethnocentrism: Psychology and
religion within an ideological surround. The International Journal for
the Psychology of Religion, 3, 1-20.
Watson, P. J., (2008a). Faithful translation and postmodernism:
Norms and linguistic relativity within a Christian ideological surround.
Edification: Journal of the Society for Christian Psychology, 2(1),
5-18.
Watson, P. J., (2008b). Faithful translation, ideological
perspectives, and dimensions of Christian psychology beyond
postmodernism. Edification: Journal of the Society for Christian
Psychology, 2(1), 51-62.
Watson, P. J. (2010). Christian rationality and the postmodern
context: The example of Rational-Emotive Therapy within a Christian
ideological surround. Edification: The Transdisciplinary Journal of
Christian Psychology, 4(1), 64-74.
Watson, P. J., Chen, Z., & Sisemore, T. A. (2010). Grace and
Christian Psychology--Part 2: Psychometric refinements and relationships
with self-compassion, depression, beliefs about sin, and religious
orientation. Edification: The Transdisciplinary Journal of Christian
Psychology, 4(2), 64-72.
Watson, P. J., Hood, R. W., Jr., & Morris, R. J. (1988).
Existential confrontation and religiosity. Counseling and Values, 33,
47-54.
Watson, P. J., Milliron, J. T., Morris, R. J., & Hood, R. W.,
Jr. (1995). Religion and the self as text: Toward a Christian
translation of self-actualization. Journal of Psychology and Theology,
23, 180-189.
Watson, P. J. & Morris, R. M. (2008). Self-control within a
Christian ideological surround. Edification: Journal of the Society for
Christian Psychology, 2(2), 62-72. Watson, P. J., Morris, R. J., &
Hood, R. W., Jr. (1987). Antireligious humanistic values, guilt, and
self-esteem. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 26, 535-546.
Watson, P. J., Morris, R. J., & Hood, R. W., Jr. (1989). Sin
and self-functioning, Part 5: Antireligious humanistic values,
individualism, and the community. Journal of Psychology and Theology,
17, 157-172.
Watson, P. J., Morris, R. J., Hood, R. W., Jr., & Folbrecht, J.
(1990). Dependency, "irrationality," and community. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 18, 334-347.
Watson, P. J., Morris, R. J., Loy, T., Hamrick, M. B., &
Grizzle, S. (2007). Beliefs about sin: Adaptive implications in
relationships with religious orientation, self-esteem, and measures of
the narcissistic, depressed and anxious self. Edification: Journal of
the Society for Christian Psychology, 1(1), 57-67.
P. J. Watson
University of Tennessee at Chattanooga
P.J. Watson is U.C. Foundation Professor of Psychology at the
University of Tennessee at Chattanooga. His research interests include
the psychology of religion and personality.
A version of this article was presented at the Bryan Institute for
Critical Thought and Practice on March 20, 2010. Address all
correspondence to P. J. Watson, Ph.D., Psychology/Dept. #2803. 350 Holt
Hall-615 McCallie Avenue, University of Tennessee at Chattanooga,
Chattanooga, TN 37403; paul-watson@utc.edu.