首页    期刊浏览 2024年09月18日 星期三
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Investigating the summer learning effect in low SES schools.
  • 作者:Jesson, Rebecca ; McNaughton, Stuart ; Kolose, Tone
  • 期刊名称:Australian Journal of Language and Literacy
  • 印刷版ISSN:1038-1562
  • 出版年度:2014
  • 期号:February
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:Australian Literacy Educators' Association
  • 摘要:Many school systems strive for more equitable outcomes and our knowledge of how to design more equitable schools has advanced in recent years (McNaughton, 2011). But a major barrier to generalised success continues to be the 'summer learning effect' (SLE) where students' achievement declines over summer. The effect differentially affects students from low socioeconomic status (SES) communities and from some cultural groups and creates a barrier to equity that gets larger over time.
  • 关键词:Academic achievement;School administration;School management and organization

Investigating the summer learning effect in low SES schools.


Jesson, Rebecca ; McNaughton, Stuart ; Kolose, Tone 等


Variability in the Summer Learning Effect in Years 4-6

Many school systems strive for more equitable outcomes and our knowledge of how to design more equitable schools has advanced in recent years (McNaughton, 2011). But a major barrier to generalised success continues to be the 'summer learning effect' (SLE) where students' achievement declines over summer. The effect differentially affects students from low socioeconomic status (SES) communities and from some cultural groups and creates a barrier to equity that gets larger over time.

The effect is known internationally to contribute to more than half of the overall differences in literacy achievement between low and high SES youth: after 5 years at school the cumulative effect of summer differences is greater than the differences in school literacy scores apparent at school entry (Alexander, Entwisle and Olson, 2007). Resulting achievement gaps contribute to on-going inequity in high school placements, dropout rates and college attendance. In New Zealand similar obstacles are apparent. Lai, McNaughton, Amituanai-Toloa, Turner and Hsiao (2009) identify the 'staircase shape' of achievement levels, which show reading gains within school years tempered by a plateau over summer, a pattern similar to those identified in the United States (Borman, 2000).

In this study we conceive of literacy development as a co-construction between learners and more expert persons that occurs through engagement in activities that define the practices of families and schools. These activities are dependent on resources in these settings. This means that forms of literacy develop at home through valued practices, but that families may differ in their focus on, their access to, and their provision of support for those literacy activities that relate to development at school. Optimally, guidance at school in how to read enables the learner to become increasingly self-regulated and engaged in valued activities such as independent reading, and this generalises to out-of-school reading. But because development is partly dependent on the relationships between practices across the settings, increasing the complementarity of the activities between settings such as access to, guidance for, and engagement in high-interest texts appropriate for literacy development at school is predicted to contribute to the developmental potential of both settings (Bronfenbrenner, 1979).

Three general sources of influence on literacy development over summer can be predicted from this view of development. These are, the child's engagement in literacy activities which relate to school learning; the school's contributions to that engagement through direct guidance to students (e.g., in developing strategies for 'independent reading') and indirect influences with the student and significant family members (e.g., providing access to engaging books or advice on how to access and use the local library); and home and community contributions, such as providing activities which contribute to engagement. This study sought to identify the contribution of each of these sources to students' learning over summer.

There is research evidence that substantiates the role of the aforementioned sources on summer learning differences. In terms of students' practices, Heyns (1978) demonstrated that volume of summer reading is an indicator of summer loss or gain. According to her calculations, for every four books read over summer one month's achievement gain occurred. Importantly, volume of reading was associated with income level and access to a library.

A necessary condition for volume of summer reading is access to engaging texts. Disparity of access has been described in communities and homes (Allington & McGill-Franzen, 2009, Neuman & Celano, 2001), and programmes have been implemented to address the disparity, often, providing books for disadvantaged children (e.g., Duffy Books in Homes, n.d.). In experimental demonstrations, access coupled with student selection of books that optimise engagement is associated with increased achievement (Allington et al., 2010).

However, provision of access does not guarantee most effective learning from texts. Continued engagement in school-like literacy practices supported by family and neighbourhood contexts is also predictive (Anderson, Wilson & Fielding, 1988; Heyns, 1978). While students might become increasingly independent as they mature, it is likely that independence is supported by practices and resources at school and home. The research evidence shows that schools and families vary in both access and the forms that guidance takes (Anderson, et al., 1988). Neuman and Celano (2006), for example, observed differences in quality of library usage. These authors report widespread usage of libraries by both middle and low income communities, with similar numbers of children using the library for similar amounts of time, selecting similar numbers of books. However, differences were apparent in the types of books selected.

Children from the middle income communities selected appropriately levelled or challenging books, while children from low income communities selected more below age-level books. There were also differences in support and guidance. Children from middle income communities had adult mentors, who assisted making choices, and issuing books and using computers productively; children from low income communities more often came alone, or with a peer or sibling who provided little specific help. Hence, even given equitable access, guidance and support seems a necessary ingredient to overcoming the SLE drop (Kim, 2006).

Several conclusions can be drawn. One is that access to texts outside of school is a determinant of literacy learning at school. But the texts need to afford engagement, by being well matched to interest. Students need productive engagement with these texts, so motivation and metacognition also contribute, as do forms of teacher guidance for students and support for their families likely enhance the effectiveness of this engagement.

How these sources of influence function in the New Zealand context is unclear. It is not known what access different family groups have to texts and how that access is associated with variations in learning over summer, or what the uses of those texts might be. It is also not known what variations in guidance for summer reading students may receive. We designed a mapping exercise to identify existing teacher practices, family practices and student characteristics associated with continued engagement in school-related literacy activities over summer. The context was a group of schools with students from low SES families who were from cultural groups traditionally not well served, but these schools were demonstrably effective in promoting literacy achievement while the students were at school.

Methods

The study sought evidence about the nature of an ongoing problem in these schools with the aim of designing an appropriate programme with the schools (McNaughton, 2011). An ongoing collaboration with seven multicultural low SES urban primary schools identified the SLE as a significant barrier. Reading achievement data were analysed for all students Years 4-8 (n = 1,495). Of these students, 77% were from Pacific nationalities; 18% were Maori. Typically, within-year gains had occurred in reading comprehension but these gains were lost between years. The SLE averaged -0.55 stanine (SD = 1.23, t(1494) = -14.94, p < .001, d = -0.45) as measured by Supplementary Tests of Achievement in Reading (Elley, 2001).

However, within this overall trend, rates of SLE were variable. In fact, many students showed little or no SLE. The methodological approach, therefore, was designed to understand this variability within the context. Within our ecological framing, differences in achievement outcomes within the community would likely arise from differences in students' experiences and participation in both home and school contexts. Thus we sought to understand the differences associated with rates of SLE at two levels: between high and low SLE classes, and between high and low SLE students within those classes.

Selection of outliers

Analysis of outliers was employed to understand this variation in SLE. Outlier classes were identified through analysis of the repeated measures of reading achievement either side of summer. These analyses identified eight classes associated with low SLE (n = 4) and high SLE (n = 4). The classes consisted of four matched pairs, similar in year level and average end of year achievement profile; differing in average SLE gives details of the classes selected.

Within each class, two students with end of year scores at or near average (stanine 5) were selected as high or low SLE. Thus four categories of students could be identified based on the class SLE and the student SLE (see Table 2). This categorisation was possible in all classes except one high SLE class where all students dropped over summer. In that class two high SLE students (one high, and one higher) were selected. Of the 16 students, 11 were female and five male. Six students were Samoan, two Maori, two Cook Island Maori, two Niuean, one Fijian, one Tokelauan and one was Chinese.

Data sources and analyses

Retrospective interviews after the summer were conducted with the teachers (n = 8), the students (n = 16), and an adult family member (n=16). We asked teachers about preparation and follow-up for summer in four areas identified within the literature as likely to contribute to differential rates of SLE: student reading skills, metacognition, engagement and parental guidance. The interviews used a combination of pre-coded and open response items. Pre-coded items asked whether participants had employed a particular practice; open-response items followed up on these, and asked participants to explain how they had done so. For example, teachers were asked whether they had prepared their students in any way (yes/no), whether they had provided ideas about reading or writing (yes/ no), and if so what those ideas were (open response). Similarly, teachers were asked whether they had introduced, described or recommended specific programmes (yes/ no), and if so, what those programmes were (open response). At the end of each item, a catch-all question was included: 'did you do anything else'? In this way, the interviews collected both quantitative data about how many practices were employed and short descriptions of the nature of those practices.

The interviews with students and families mirrored these and sought personal and family practices over summer. They also asked about the resources students and families had access to, including church, community, libraries, summer programmes and extended family resources.

Analyses of interviews were conducted using a mixed-methods data analysis approach (Onwuegbuzie & Teddlie, 2003) in two phases, using the participant as the unit of analysis. The first approach to analysis was purely quantitative: questions were categorised according to whether participants' answers (yes/no) to the pre-coded interview items. The second phase of analysis employed content analysis of the open response items, based on the a-priori categories which framed the interviews: (student competence, student metacognition, student engagement and parental guidance). Within each of these categories, the degree to which the reported practices might affect the degree of summer learning was considered. Thus, participants' responses were summarised and sorted into two types within each category: practices that were specific to the summer, and practices employed throughout the year, which participants indicated might generalise to summer. The interview analyses were then subject to a verification phase. Through discussion and debate, the data and subsequent analyses were reviewed by the whole research team in a series of weekly meetings to ascertain whether disconfirming data were accounted for and whether additional themes emerged which had not been captured by existing analyses. Disagreements in coding were resolved through discussion in this stage.

The final analysis frame comprised eight categories: general or specific practices within four areas likely to contribute to reading achievement gains or losses over summer. Examples included the following: for a focus on competence reference was made to developing students' independent reading for summer through the classroom programme (competence: general) or deliberate teaching of how to use the neighbourhood library over summer (competence: specific); in metacognition talking about setting goals for reading (metacognition: general) or preparing students to develop reading logs over summer (metacognition: specific); in engagement, employing a classroom programme that built habits of reading (engagement: general) or providing individualised selections of high-interest texts for student use in school and during summer (engagement: specific); in parental guidance reminders through newsletters to use the library (parental guidance: general) or end of year meetings with parents at which goals such as visiting the library were set (parental guidance: specific).

Results

Classroom Contributions

It quickly became apparent that differences between teachers of high and low SLE classes could not be discerned by counting of number of reported strategies for preparing students over summer. In terms of the total number of 'yes' responses to pre-coded items, there were few clear differences. In terms of preparing students to read over summer and building students' independent learning strategies, although teachers of low SLE classes gave the highest number of positive responses, one teacher of a low SLE class reported the lowest number of these practices.

Some minor differences in numbers were seen between particular question items however. Three of the four teachers of low SLE classes encouraged their students to use the library over the summer break and three teachers of low SLE classes provided guidance for individual students. A contrast emerged between three teachers of low SLE classes who promoted independent reading by helping their students choose suitable books, and three teachers of high SLE classes reported providing specific books for parents. Table 3 provides a summary of the numbers of quantitative responses.

Content analysis of the open ended and the catchall questions provided some clues as to difference between the teachers' practices, namely in the degree to which the practices were targeted to summer. All the teachers reported general strategies for developing reading competence. All teachers of low SLE classes also reported practices specific to summer. One teacher reported that she specifically prompted students to use the Duffy Books (Books in Homes, n.d.) during the summer break. Another reported reminding students to visit the public library and to follow up holiday activities with related reading, which she described as 'looking for opportunities to learn, to use their reading to inquire'. Similarly, another teacher prompted students to 'marry physical artefact with texts', by looking for objects at the museum, that they had learned about in a unit of work. One teacher of a high SLE class also reported specific practices, telling students it is 'important to at least read one book or magazine or newspaper article, even reading labels off breakfast cereal ... or anything that interests [them] in reading.'

Students' metacognition was developed in a general way by two of the teachers of high SLE classes and by all of the teachers of low SLE classes. All teachers of low SLE classes also reported specific reference to summer. Examples included mileage recording sheets, reading logs, and 'word consciousness' prompts or reminders. One teacher of a low SLE class explicitly discussed summer reading loss with her class, through 'ongoing discussion of results, maintaining levels over holidays. We talked about drops over holidays'. Another prompted children to use 'Bloom's taxonomy to link it back to our thinking', and 'mileage sheets to encourage them to look at the various genres that they're reading -so that they don't read 'baby' books'. Those sheets also served to 'encourage and challenge them to select appropriate books'. One high SLE teacher also prompted awareness (possibly not in a positive way) by reminding students about writing in the following year, 'They know it's [what I did over the summer holidays story] coming.'

In terms of preparation for engagement, general practices anticipated to transfer to summer included homework and rewards for reading mileage. All the teachers of low SLE classes and one teacher of a high SLE class reported reading to students, and modelling a love of reading. Practices for student engagement specific to summer included provision of resources, support for summer internet use, and recommendations of specific websites. All the low SLE teachers reported specific practices. Moreover, these teachers' specific practices seemed to focus on student engagement over summer, by promoting literacy for leisure or personal challenge. For example, one teacher of a low SLE class said that she focussed on 'reading for inquiry thinking'. Another prompted students to 'choose challenging books that make them think'. The contrast was a focus on skills or schooling success. For example, one teacher of a high SLE class told students it was 'important to read to develop skills'. Another teacher of a high SLE class reported that 'students need to read for all subjects, and parents should also read [the text] and follow up with comprehension [questions]'. Another teacher of a high SLE class had sent home a homework packet of reading and maths, 'not specifically tied to curriculum'.

In terms of parental support, three of the teachers of low SLE classes reported general practices such as advice in newsletter; three also reported specific practices such as goal setting for library use. As one teacher described her practice, 'the end of year report talked about what needed to happen at home for coming year ... [there was a] major emphasis on goal setting at parent interviews'. No teachers of high SLE classes reported advice to parents specifically targeted to summer. The quantified results of this content analysis are presented in Table 4.

In summary, there appeared to be little difference between high and low SLE teachers in the quantity and variety of practices reported. However, based on content analysis of the interviews, teachers in low SLE classrooms more often reported preparing students specifically for summer rather than just generally in the four areas. Thus the difference in practices between teachers of low and high SLE classes may have related to the specificity and focus of the preparation, not simply its presence.

Students' practices

Quantitative analyses did not reveal marked differences between high and low SLE students. Most students reported liking books, with fewer liking other types of reading. All children also reported general reading at home, and most reported reading at home over summer. The children all gave similar responses in terms of their motivation for summer reading: enjoyment.

Comparing students' general responses with those specific to summer, some slight indications of difference in student practices did emerge. Over summer, reading variety altered for both high and low SLE students, with fewer students reading comics, magazines, church materials or on the internet over summer. While no students reported reading recipes generally, three said that they read these over summer. Both high and low SLE children reported reading at family and friends' houses more over summer. Furthermore, fewer children reported reading at the library over summer.

Additionally, there were some indications of practices specific to low SLE students. Slightly higher numbers of low SLE students enjoyed reading through games, with half of the respondents from low SLE classes reporting that they enjoyed board games (e.g., Monopoly) and Internet games (e.g., Moshi Monsters). However, few students reported reading through games over summer. Conversely, while few students reported reading 'other' genres in general, over the summer period more than half of the low SLE students reported reading materials other than those we listed (including picture books, Christmas stories, instructions for crafts or drawing). For all of these children, these other genre included the newspaper, and in particular the local free newspaper, The Courier. Responses to reading enjoyment did not distinguish between high or low SLE students, and all except two students reported regular reading (perhaps the anticipated desirable response). These responses are presented in Table 5.

Students were also asked about the conditions that supported and surrounded their reading. Approximately half of the children read to parents or family. All students reported usually getting books from the school and local library to read. However, only five said that they had been to the library over summer. Students from low SLE classes reported that their teachers had given them ideas about reading over summer. All low SLE students, and all those from low SLE classes, reported that someone at home helped them read. Table 6 summarises their responses to these questions.

Parents' reports

Parents, like their children, identified that the local newspapers were commonly read. They also commonly reported that their children read from the bible, although the children hadn't reported this. When asked why their children read, the parents of low SLE students reported either prompting or encouragement. Parents of high SLE children either did not know or reported that it was because the children were 'told to'. This was true in all cases except one, who did report encouraging her child to read, and having intended to take them to the library; 'taking her to the library so she could choose her own books ... we did that once before ... I allow her to choose [at home] but I sometimes try to push my one [my daughter]'.

Parents also agreed that interest motivated reading. Asked whether access affected what children read, differences were striking. While no parents of low SLE students considered that access affected their child's reading, all the five parents of high SLE students who responded to this question agreed that access impacted reading, highlighting The Courier and the Bible as accessible texts.

In terms of guidance for reading, seven parents reported receiving guidance for general reading: three via school notices and three directly from the teacher. In addition one parent also reported receiving guidance 'at the library'. Four parents reported that their child had received advice about summer reading; three were parents of children in low SLE classes, and one parent of a low SLE student, in a high SLE class. There appeared to be a qualitative difference between those parents who had received guidance. Parents of high SLE students described this guidance in general terms, for example, one parent reported that they were told to 'continue on reading at home and on holiday ... and they watch TV programmes, continue with some activities'. Parents of low SLE students responded with more elaboration, which included specific strategies. For example, this parent of a low SLE child explained, 'spend time with [child] reading. Get the child to read to you. Correct any words they don't understand or how to say them. Ask what it's about'. Another described the advice they received as, 'making time, providing a quiet place for them to do their homework and assisting where we can'. Another question asked of parents was whether they knew of or used programmes that encouraged summer reading. Five parents were aware of summer programmes; three parents had children who took part.

In summary, students and their parents reported a wide range of reading. Over summer, reading variety altered to include reading of newspapers and recipes. Low SLE students were more likely to have parental support for access to texts, and parental support that responded to and provided encouragement for reading. They were also more likely to report guidance from a teacher about summer reading. Additionally, parents of high SLE students were more likely to report that access to texts influenced their children's reading practices. Parents of low SLE children were more likely to report the guidance that they had received in elaborated or specific terms. Summer programmes were seldom used.

Discussion

The SLE is a barrier to equitable outcomes that might be considered outside the domain of the school. Our approach has been to see the problem as one which schools face to be as effective as possible by being able to build each year on gains made in previous years. Based on an ecological approach, it was likely that sources of variation would be found in four areas of influence over students' development in reading over summer, each of which would in turn be influenced by interactions between students, their teachers and their families. Analysis of outliers allowed us to map the variation at each of these levels in four areas: access to resources, engagement in reading, metacognition and monitoring of reading; and support and guidance available to children and their families.

Access to resources

Theoretically, overcoming barriers to accessing texts would allow students to engage with literacy over summer (Allington et al., 2010; Heyns, 1978), and differential access might therefore account for differences in the amount of summer reading. In this community, while the students generally reported regular reading over the summer period, they also reported changes in what they read. However this did not distinguish high from low SLE students. Books clearly dominated holiday reading and there was some evidence that students' ability to access various types of resources was reduced over summer. Teachers all identified that school libraries were not available to students over summer, but it did not follow that low SLE teachers provided books for children. Community newspapers were read more over summer and these were identified as accessible for students.

What may have distinguished high from low SLE students is the extent to which access influenced children's choice of what to read, and therefore the 'match' between students and texts. Although the students didn't report that access influenced their choice, parents of high SLE students reported that their child's choices were influenced by what was accessible. Thus, although texts were accessible, there was perhaps a differential constraint operating on students' ability to choose from a wide range of texts. Two alternative implications are likely. The first is that there might be a need to talk with students about their ability to access texts, and to develop strategies that widen their selection, such as seeking out and choosing engaging texts at libraries or online. But like Neuman and Celano's (2006) findings this might be more about how where and with whom students gained access, which suggests more specific guidance for teachers and parents, not only to provide access to texts, but to work with students on specific strategies for accessing and selecting texts. Our data suggests that teachers of low SLE classes did this, not by providing texts for students, but by working with them on how to choose.

Motivation and engagement

Perhaps unsurprisingly, engagement in reading over summer seems key to students' ability to maintain their literacy levels (Heyns, 1978, Anderson, et al., 1988), which, over summer, is more likely if students are motivated to read. Students were unanimous that their motivation for reading over summer was purely enjoyment. Their parents concurred. However their teachers were less united. Whereas low SLE teachers' responses offered support for developing students' interests and strategies for choosing texts, a focus on reading skills was apparent in many high SLE teachers' responses, highlighting the contrast between a framing of reading as homework and what has traditionally been conceived as 'recreation' (Anderson, et al., 1988). Arguably, a homework framing for reading is less likely to transfer to summer leisure activities than a recreational framing. It is conceivable that the homework stance may even provide a disincentive to summer reading, by highlighting boundaries between school and holidays.

If engagement in reading over summer relies on selecting and enjoying texts as part of leisure activities, this implies that students may need support to build metacognition focused on their reading enjoyment. The low SLE teachers in our study provided metacognitive prompts for 'thinking', 'learning' and 'inquiring', rather than reading skills. This was unexpected, and runs somewhat counter to prevailing understandings about the need for goal setting and self-regulation of learning (Paris & Paris, 2001). This needs further investigation. It may be, for example, that while metacognitive prompts and goal setting focussed on learning which may achieve higher outcomes within a year, teaching that is explicit for reading over summer may be best to reinforce monitoring and self-regulation, not of achievement, but of enjoyment: What is it in texts that I enjoy? How can I identify and then find texts with these features?

Support and guidance

Previous studies have found that the nature of support and guidance for students varies between low and middle income families (Neuman & Celano, 2006). In our study, students were all part of the same community, and so might be considered similar in terms of their general socio-economic status, although we never explicitly asked about parental income or education. However, like Newman and Celano, we also found differences in the nature of support and guidance that students received. Students in low SLE classes were more likely to have received specific support from their teachers for summer reading. Additionally, low SLE students also reported support from family to read at home. But we also found differences in the way that parents talked about the guidance they received. Parents seemed to differ in their ability to offer specific or elaborated responses in terms of what guidance for reading had been offered. Thus, guidance and support from schools seems to be a powerful ingredient, both at the level of the students, and also the family. Support for students seems most powerful when specific to summer and this has been reflected in international interventions. Kim's (2006) intervention, for example, had very specific directions for students. Support for parents may also benefit from specificity; perhaps requiring concrete examples of strategies for accessing and selecting motivating and well matched texts, rather than general advice to encourage children to carry on reading over summer.

This finding of specificity is consistent with literature on parent involvement and parent tutoring which point to the need to have specific messages and guidance (Robinson, Hohepa & Lloyd, 2009). However, it is inconsistent with the focus. Whereas parent involvement and parent tutoring are predicated on a schoolwork framing, we found that in low SLE contexts students' motivation was interest based, and their teachers and parents supported this motivation. The resolution of his might lie in recognising the need to have specific guidance for parents related to supporting recreational reading over summer which do not have a remedial or skills building focus.

Conclusion

This study provides initial evidence of differences between high and low SLE students and classes in a group of schools in a low SES community in Auckland. Findings suggest a number of practices of the low SLE students, their teachers and their parents that are likely to be associated with the maintenance of reading achievement over the summer. The students considered that they did read over summer, and when they did so, they read for enjoyment. Their teachers supported this, by preparing students in ways that had a summer-specific focus. They worked with students to develop strategies for choosing texts and for seeking out opportunities to read over summer. Their parents more often reported strategies for supporting students' reading.

Our study, based as it was on interviews and self-report, is limited by the responses given, and the threats of socially desirable responding and misreport. The issue of mileage, for example, crucial though it is, was not well addressed by the self-report of children thinking back to the summer break and responding in a school setting. The apparent solution to this would seem to be reading logs or similar, which might give a more accurate representation of what children actually read over summer. However, the evidence that we present here suggests that additional 'work' for students may not support the recreational nature of summer reading. Instead, it would seem that overcoming the SLE requires instead specific focus, support and guidance for recreational reading.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by the Teaching Learning Research Initiative Funding (New Zealand Council for Educational Research). We warmly acknowledge the substantial contribution of the participating schools, teachers, students and parents.

References

Alexander, K.L., Entwisle, D.R. & Olson, L.S. (2007). Lasting consequences of the summer learning gap. American Sociological Review, 72, 167-180.

Allington, R.L. & McGill-Franzen, A. (2009, August 24). Why summers matter in the rich/poor achievement gap. Teachers College Record. Retrieved from http://www. tcrecord. org/Content.asp?C ontentID=15757

Allington, R.L., McGill-Franzen, A., Camilli, G., Williams, L., Graff, J., Zeig, J. & Nowak, R. (2010). Addressing summer reading setback among economically disadvantaged elementary students. Reading Psychology, 31 (5), 411-427. doi:10.1080/02702711.2010.505165.

Anderson, R.C., Wilson, P.T. & Fielding, L.G. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 285-303.

Borman, G. (2000). The effects of summer school: Questions answered, questions raised. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65(1, Serial No. 260), 119-127.

Duffy Books in Homes (n.d.) Duffy Books in Homes. Retrieved from http://www.booksinhomes.org.nz

Elley, W. (2001). STAR supplementary test of achievement in reading: Years 4-6. Wellington, New Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational Research.

Heyns, B. (1978). Summer learning and the effects of schooling. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Kim, J.S. (2006). Effects of a voluntary summer reading intervention on reading achievement: Results from a randomized field trial. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 28 (4), 335-355. doi:10.3102/01623737028004335

Lai, M.K., McNaughton, S., Amituanai-Toloa, M., Turner, R. & Hsiao, S. (2009). Sustained acceleration of achievement in reading comprehension: The New Zealand experience. Reading Research Quarterly, 44 (1), 30-56. doi:10.1598/ RRQ.44.1.2

McNaughton, S. (2011). Designing better schools for culturally and linguistically diverse children: A science of performance model for research. New York, NY: Routledge.

Neuman, S.B. & Celano, D. (2001). Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities: An ecological study of four neighborhoods. Reading Research Quarterly, 36 (1), 8-26. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.36.1.1

Neuman, S.B. & Celano, D. (2006). The knowledge gap: Implications of leveling the playing field for low-income and middle-income children. Reading Research Quarterly, 41 (2), 176-201. doi:10.1598/RRQ.41.2.2

Onwuegbuzie, A.J. & Teddlie, C. (2003). A framework for analyzing data in mixed methods research. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social and behavioral research (pp. 351-383). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Paris, S.G. & Paris, A.H. (2001). Classroom applications of research on self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 36(2), 89-101. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3602_4

Robinson, V., Hohepa, M. & Lloyd, C. (2009). School leadership and student outcomes: Identifying what works and why. Best evidence synthesis iteration [BES]. Wellington, New Zealand: Ministry of Education.

Rebecca Jesson and Stuart McNaughton

University of Auckland

Tone Kolose

Manurewa South School

Rebecca Jesson is a Senior Lecturer in the Faculty of Education at the University of Auckland. Her research focuses on raising achievement in literacy in diverse communities in New Zealand. She works closely with schools to analyse their profile of teaching and learning to design instruction that is responsive to context.

Stuart McNaughton is a Professor and Director of the Woolf Fisher Research Centre at the University of Auckland. His research focuses on literacy and language development, including the design of instruction and educational programmes for culturally and linguistically diverse populations. His current research focuses on the properties of effective teaching of literacy and language in research-based interventions with clusters of schools.

Tone Kolose is Principal of a large urban school in South Auckland, New Zealand. He has been instrumental in schooling improvement initiatives where schools collaborate to solve achievement issues. Tone worked closely with researchers on this project to build understanding of the Summer Learning Effect in the schools' community as a basis for a summer reading programme.
Table 1. Features of Selected Classes

                                Class End      Class Mean
           Year     Teacher    of Year Mean    Gain Over    Selected
Pair      Level     number    Stanine (STAR)     Summer        as

Pair 1    Year 4       1           3.95          -0.70      High SLE
                       2           3.96           0.13      Low SLE

Pair 2    Year 5       3           4.57          -0.71      High SLE
                       4           4.19          -0.04      Low SLE

Pair 3   Year 5-6      5           4.86          -1.19      High SLE
                       6           4.69           0.00      Low SLE

Pair 4    Year 7       7           4.36          -0.73      High SLE
                       8           4.77           0.23      Low SLE

Table 2.
Summer Stanine Gains and Categorisation of
Participating Students

Student      Summer      Class   Student   Category
number    Stanine Gain    SLE      SLE      Totals

1              0          low      low
2              0          low      low
3              0          low      low
4              1          low      low      n = 4
5              -1         low     high
6              -2         low     high
7              -1         low     high
8              -2         low     high      n = 4
9              0         high      low
10             0         high      low
11             0         high      low      n = 3
12             -2        high     high
13             -2        high     high
14             -2        high     high
15             -1        high     high
16             -2        high     high      n = 5

Table 3.
Teachers' Preparation for Summer
('Yes' responses to yes/no items)

      Question                                     Low SLE   High SLE

1.    Did you prepare your students in any way        3         4
      to read over summer?

2.    If yes, did you provide general ideas           3         3
      about reading / writing?

3.    If yes, did you introduce / describe /          3         1
      recommend specific programmes?

4.    If yes, did you provide specific books?         1         1

5.    Is there any other programme or guidance        4         4
      at your school that might provide guidance
      for your students' reading over summer?

6     Did you prepare or provide guidance for         3         1
      any single individual student?

7.    Did you provide information / ideas /           3         4
      resources to any parents?

8.    If yes, did you provide general ideas           3         2
      about reading / writing?

9     If yes, did you introduce / describe /          1         1
      recommend specific programmes?

10.   If yes, did you provide specific books?         0         3

11.   Do your children like reading by                4         3
      themselves?

12.   How do you build / promote independent          2         1
      reading? By telling them to have lots of
      practice?

13.   How do you build / promote independent          4         4
      reading? By providing time in class to
      read (e.g. SSR)?

14.   How do you build / promote independent          3         1
      reading? By helping to choose a suitable
      book?

Table 4. Teachers' General and
Summer Specific Practices

                                                  Specific
                        General Preparation     Preparation

                       Low SLE    High SLE   Low SLE    High
                       teachers   teachers   teachers   SLE

Competence                4          4          4        1
Metacognition             4          2          4        1
Engagement                1          3          4        1
Parental information      3          1          3        0

Table 5.
Students' Personal Reading ('Yes' responses)

                                  Student              Class

                           Low SLE   High SLE   Low SLE   High SLE
                             N=7       N=9        N=8       N=8

What things do you like to read?

Books                         6         9          7         8
Comics                        3         3          4         2
Magazines                     2         2          3         1
Internet                      2         3          3         2
Games                         3         1          4         0
Church materials              3         1          2         2
Recipes                       0         0          0         0
Lyrics                        1         2          2         1
Other                         1         1          1         1

Where do you read?

School                        5         9          6         8
Home                          7         9          8         8
Church                        1         1          0         2
Library                       2         3          4         1
Family or friend's house      1         0          0         1
Other                         0         2          0         2

Where did you read over the holidays?

Home                          5         8          5         8
Church                        0         0          0         0
Library                       1         0          0         1
Family or friend's house      3         6          6         3
Other                         0         1          0         1

What things did you read in the holidays?

Books                         5         9          6         8
Comics                        1         1          0         2
Magazines                     0         1          1         0
Internet                      1         0          1         0
Games                         0         1          1         0
Church materials              2         0          2         0
Recipes                       0         2          1         1
Lyrics                        0         0          0         0
Other                         4         1          4         1
Why did you read these?
Someone encouraged me         0         1          1         0
Availability                  1         2          1         2
Enjoyment                     6         7          6         7
School                        0         0          0         0

Note. Students are categorised twice: by individual SLE and
by class SLE.

Table 6.
Conditions Supporting Students' Reading ('Yes'
Responses)

                                 Student               Class

                           Low SLE   High SLE   Low SLE   High SLE
                             N=7       N=9        N=8       N=8

Who did you read with over the holidays?

Parent                        4         2          3         3
Other family                  3         5          4         4
Friends                       0         1          1         0
Other                         0         0          0         0
Did anyone read to you?       5         4          4         5
Did you read to anyone?       6         7          7         6

Were there any special trips or activities over summer?

Library                       3         2          3         2
School holiday programme      0         1          0         1
Bible study                   1         0          1         0
Museum visits                 1         0          1         0
Other                         2         2          3         1

Did your teacher or someone   6         7          8         5

Did you do what they          5         4          5         4
said / read what they
gave you?

Did anyone at home help       7         6          8         5
you read?

What did they do to
help?

Take to library               2         2          4         0
Helped with reading           2         2          3         1
Read to you                   2         0          0         2
Other                         3         2          2         3
Did anyone at home help       3         3          3         3
  you choose what to
  read?
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有