Five-year-olds doing science and technology: how teachers shape the conversation.
Thwaite, Anne ; McKay, Graham
Introduction
Discourse analysis is a valuable tool for analysing and reflecting
on classroom practice. To this end, we observed and audio-recorded a
series of science and technology lessons over nine weeks. Afterwards we
applied a functional discourse analysis to the transcripts in order to
identify the strategies the teachers used to engage the children in the
learning.
Science and technology are vital learning areas in primary school
education and are increasingly being emphasised in the early years.
Classroom discourse in these curriculum areas is a ripe location for
examining children's literacy, how they are initiated into
scientific and technological discourse and the role of teachers in
developing this discourse. Teachers' roles involve, among other
things:
* engaging the learners in the discourses of and about science,
* helping them to question claims about scientific matters, and
* assisting them to investigate, question and draw conclusions
about science topics (Rennie, Goodrum & Hackling, 2001). According
to Rennie (2005) engaging in scientific discourse means:
being able to read about and talk about science in a sensible and
comfortable way ... being able to participate in the communication
of science ... not being frightened of science but being willing to
engage with it. (p. 12)
The Australian Curriculum Science (v.3.0) describes
'scientific literacy' as follows:
Students can experience the joy of scientific discovery and nurture
their natural curiosity about the world around them. In doing this, they
develop critical and creative thinking skills and challenge themselves
to identify questions and draw evidence-based conclusions using
scientific methods. The wider benefits of this 'scientific
literacy' are well established ... (ACARA, 2012)
Constructivist and sociocultural theories of education suggest that
in order to learn most effectively, children need hands-on activities,
observations and social interactions with peers and adults; and the
learning of science lends itself to such an active learning approach
(Shepardson & Britsch, 2001, p. 43). Indeed, scientific inquiry
necessitates the type of environment where children can observe, ask
questions, seek answers, make discoveries and justify their decisions
(DuVall, 2001). This is at the heart of what it means to 'do'
science. This study will investigate to what extent teachers (and
children) were able to create and participate in situations conducive to
science learning.
To involve children with science, teachers first need to engage
them and get them to respond, as Rennie (2005) notes. The extent to
which their responses go beyond the minimal and simplistic will be an
indication of their involvement, and this is illustrated in our
discussion of turn length below, particularly in the case of some
children. Student initiation of talk, occasionally found in our data, is
another indicator of engagement.
One of the roles of the teacher is to model appropriate discourse,
in this case scientific discourse. There may be scientific processes,
such as drawing conclusions, which are beyond the capabilities of very
young children; modelling of these processes by the teacher is one way
of introducing children to the learning of science. Other scientific
processes such as observation are well within the capacity of children
of this age, and the teacher's job is to set up structured
situations where they will be able to practise these processes. Mastery
of a discourse is shown by the use of appropriate register, including
the scientific terminology focussed upon by teachers in our data.
Use of appropriate feedback is another way of setting up a
situation conducive to learning, in this case science learning.
Teachers' targeted feedback can draw attention to terminology and
classification systems, for example by use of antonyms such as
'rough' and 'smooth', or can encourage the children
to think about the reasons behind processes and phenomena. Various types
of questioning are involved in this feedback and questions are a very
important means of encouraging students to think scientifically.
As the children in our data set are very young, their current
scientific knowledge may be rather limited, but their skills of thinking
and interacting, as supported and developed by the teachers in our data,
will set them up for further learning in this area.
Below, we discuss how the teachers shaped activities and spoken
discourse with the aim of developing the children's scientific and
technical knowledge and literacies. Our intention was to analyse the
classroom talk and to make our findings available for pre-service
teachers and others who could learn from these experienced teachers. The
analysis we use is based on functional linguistics, which provides a
holistic model of language in society, linking language variables to
their social context. (Halliday, 1993, 1994; Halliday & Matthiessen,
2004; Martin & Rose, 2007; for functional work relevant to young
children and to the classroom, see Painter, 1998; Christie, 2002; Droga
& Humphrey, 2003; Derewianka, 2011.)
Methodology
The children observed in this study were from various cultural and
linguistic backgrounds, including Indigenous Australian, Amharic,
Japanese, Mandarin, Spanish, Swedish and Vietnamese, making for a
multicultural classroom environment. The researchers audio-recorded
classroom 'conversations' of two of the nine groups of
children. There were three teachers working with the children; here they
are given the pseudonyms of Sara (SC), Laura (LA) and Davida (DW). The
teachers were accompanied at various times by aides, parents and the
school chaplain. One of the authors was also involved in observing the
lessons.
Below, we first outline the scope of the unit of work done by the
children, then briefly: describe the analysis we undertook, present
examples of some of the features we observed, and discuss the
teachers' discourse styles and what pre-service teachers could
learn from them.
The Field: topics and activities
According to teacher Laura, the observed lessons were designed to
give the children 'a complete understanding of buildings and the
reasons why we need them'. She went on to say that the teachers
aimed to immerse the children in the topic through hands-on and
problem-solving activities, beginning with the question, 'What
makes a house and what makes a home?' Nine groups of children
rotated through nine activities, one each week. Before and after the
group activities, teachers convened whole-class sessions to preview and
then reflect on the week's topic. Small-group activities included a
photoshoot, a building materials investigation and sketching using scale
and perspective.
One of the authors followed one group of children throughout the
nine weeks of activities. Another group was followed by a research
assistant. All of the discourse was audio-taped and later transcribed.
The researcher and research assistant took field notes for their groups
and interviews were conducted with staff before and after the unit of
work.
The research question: How did the teachers shape the
children's learning?
The following analyses focus on how the teachers used discourse and
actions to shape the children's learning:
Number and length of turns
This describes participation in the discourse by looking at who is
talking and for how long.
Speech Function
This analysis describes types of Initiation, Response and Feedback
structures and their place in the discourse.
Exchange Structure
This involves turn-taking, and predictable and unpredictable
discourse moves. It gives information on the dynamics of the
interactions.
Register
Register describes different aspects of any given situation: the
subject matter and what is going on (Field); relationships among
participants (Tenor); and medium of communication, (Mode). See Martin
& Rose (2007, pp. 296-308) for an outline. In this paper we focus on
the Field and the Tenor of the discourse.
Space does not permit us to give a detailed description of these
analyses here; instead we present a discussion of what each of the tools
enabled us to notice in our data.
Discourse analysis Number and length of turns
This simple analysis can help indicate power relationships in the
discourse and the extent to which children have been supported to
produce lengthier utterances. The most straightforward way to count turn
length is by the number of lines in the transcripts. However, with some
basic knowledge about language, the analyst can calculate the
grammatical complexity of turns, which reflects on children's
development in this area.
Number and length of turns were measured for both the children and
the adults. Teachers were extremely dominant overall. The
children's turns varied considerably, with some children being
prolific talkers. The number and length of turns from particular
children seemed to depend on the situation and on which adult they were
working with. Table 1 illustrates the overall distribution of turns in
our data.
Speech Function
A Speech Function analysis can include a traditional
Initiation/Response/Feedback (IRF) analysis (Sinclair & Coulthard,
1975; Halliday, 1994, p. 69). This IRF analysis is shown in Example 1,
adapted from our data:
Example 1
IRF analysis
1 SC Initiation (I) It is Wednesday. I'm wearing a
shirt that should give you a big
clue. I know lots of people in
my class know. Karen, do you know
the name ...?
2 Ka Response (R) A remembering day for the people
that died in the war?
3 SC Feedback (F) It's Remembrance Day, that's right.
In our data set adults predominantly initiated topics and threads
of conversation, although occasionally children performed Initiations.
An IRF analysis can be combined with an analysis of the Speech
Functions of Statement, Question, Command and Offer in order to look
more closely at the meaning of each element. For example, it is very
common for teachers to initiate with a Question, as illustrated in
Examples 1 and 3, and this indicates their control over the discourse; a
Question, unlike a Statement, strongly requires a response and therefore
this is one way in which the teachers make it necessary for the children
to participate in the discourse.
Exchange Structure
An Exchange Structure analysis (Martin & Rose, 2007; Ventola,
1987; Berry 1981a,b,c) goes beyond the concept of Speech Function,
coding the discourse for whether it involves talking or action (or both)
and the role of the speaker in terms of their level of expertise, for
example the 'primary knower' role. For a very brief
introduction to Exchange Structure see Thwaite 1993, pp. 163-165. For a
recent application of Exchange Structure to classroom discourse, see
Jones, Kervin & McIntosh (2011).
Discourse dynamics
Discourse structure
As mentioned above, teachers, rather than children, are
overwhelmingly the initiators of exchanges in our data, as is common in
classroom discourse. Example 2, however, shows children vying with the
teacher (DW) for control of the exchange.
Example 2
53 Cr ... when they built my house,
they used the cement and I
was only a little baby.
54 DW Oh goodness me.
55 Am Guess what?
56 Cr And I watched the
video of my ...
57 DW Did you now? Okay.
58 Cr ... when I was a baby.
59 Am Guess what?
60 DW We're going to stop now ...
61 Am It's about bricks.
62 DW ... and we're going to go
over because we're got some
activities to do and we need
to do it quickly.
Prior to the extract above, Amanda had succeeded in making
considerable input, and other children had also joined in. Line 61 is
particularly interesting in that it shows Amanda's awareness of
discourse conventions. She has been bidding for another turn and, in the
face of Davida's closing remark (line 60), here she attempts to
justify her bid by stating that her utterance is on topic. However, for
management reasons, Davida exercises her right to close Amanda down;
but, mindful of the fruitful discussion she is interrupting, she
explains her reason for doing so.
When teachers initiate an exchange, most often with a question,
there is usually more than one child who responds, some non-verbally.
The teacher then selects one or more children to take the floor.
Typically, the teacher then acknowledges what each child has said,
although in large groups it is not always possible to respond to
everyone. The acknowledgement often takes the form of repeating,
paraphrasing or 'recasting' the child's discourse.
Teachers rarely directly contradict what children have said. After their
feedback, teachers may round off the discussion or introduce a new (sub)
topic. In this way, they are opening up the discussion for as many
children as possible to contribute. They are also helping to shape the
children's ideas, modelling generalisations and conclusions and, in
some cases, showing how different sub topics in the fields are related.
The IRF analysis in Example 3 illustrates the typical discourse pattern,
showing some different types of feedback moves and what they do.
Example 3
IRF
26 DW I Well, we're talking about bricks
today and bricks that we've used,
and yes, bricks can be made for
the floor like you said, Cara, or
for paving. And are they all the
same colour, bricks?
27 Ss R No.
28 DW F (repeating) No ...
29 Cr R Some are brown and some are white.
30 DW F (repeating) Some are white.
I Any other colours that bricks
can be?
31 Je R Are some ...?
32 M R Black?
33 DW F (elaborating) Yeah, there's some black
I bricks over there, look, isn't
there? Any other colours?
34 Am R Orange bricks.
35 DW F Orange bricks, mm, bricks
(summarising) can be all different kinds
of colours.
Above, several children add to the discourse, including Mario (line
32), who is usually quiet. Thus Davida's questions appear to be
successful in encouraging contributions from the children and engaging
them in the discourse. This is a step towards them thinking and talking
scientifically, although responses are often very short.
Elaboration
In order to develop the conversation, teachers add information to
children's utterances, often with just a single adjective, for
example, 'beautiful' in line 343 below:
Example 4
341 LA In the bathroom, uh huh, that's a tile.
342 Ok Yeah, they go in bathrooms; they go in
bathrooms.
343 LA A beautiful tile in your bathroom.
Above, Laura's response to Okko acts as a confirmation of the
value of his utterance, a variation on the obvious feedback such as,
'Yes, that's right.' This arguably helps the interaction
seem more conversational, although outside the classroom this would be
an odd discourse move.
Elaborations used by teachers may be in the form of a question or
series of questions, often including an alternating question using the
word 'or', for example, 'Was it easy or hard?'
The use of this type of 'suggesting' question is typical
of the classroom discourse of Sara (SC) and is also used by the other
teachers. For example, in each of two lessons, Cristiano, a child from
South America, has two of these questions directed to him, even when the
teacher seems not to have quite understood what he is saying. Perhaps
questions like this are helpful to a child who is struggling for an
appropriate answer, or having difficulties finding vocabulary, because
the question clearly suggests possible responses.
Enabling and extending children's answers
Teachers give prompts or hints to assist children to respond to
their initiations, by referring to previous discourse. For example, Sara
says, 'We just said it; we just said it right now', clearly
indicating where the answer to her question can be found. In Example 5
Davida helps Jessica to respond.
Example 5
37 DW What do you think brick's made from?
38 Je Are they really ... some of those
hard things?
39 DW Mm, well they are hard, aren't they?
I think you told us before, Jessica.
You said they were made of something
and then they dry out.
Can you remember?
40 Je Clay.
41 DW Clay. That's exactly right.
Above, Davida refers to Jessica's previous discourse, and
substitutes the general word 'something' in place of the
answer, which she is successful in obtaining.
Types of Questions
'Teacher knows the answer' questions This commonly-used
type of question, usually a closed question, is known as a
'dk1' ('delayed primary knower') move in Exchange
Structure. Table 2 shows the extent to which these moves are used by the
adults in our data. Overall, the adults use more closed questions than
open ones. However, given this pattern, as a whole the proportion of
open questions used by the teachers is greater than that of the other
adults.
An illustration of the use of dk1 moves by teacher Laura is shown
in Example 6.
Example 6
402 LA Up. Where does it go? Does anyone know
where it goes? Cristiano's right, it
goes on the top of your house. Do you
know what it is? No? I really haven't
seen one this close before either.
It's a roof tile. Yeah? And lots of
those together form the roof when
they're joined together. And roofs
are very useful. What are roofs very
useful for?
403 S They give you shelter.
404 LA Oh, absolutely.
Here Laura is using the questions in line 402 to teach vocabulary,
among other things. While dk1 moves sometimes receive bad publicity,
this is an example of a case where they are useful: Laura has some terms
and concepts that she wants the children to learn, and the closed
questions above are a quick way of achieving this. Some other dk1 moves
have already been shown above: 'And are they all the same colour,
bricks?' (Example 3) and 'What do you think brick's made
from?' (Example 5), illustrating how this discourse strategy is
successfully used by these teachers.
Extending children's answers
Experienced teachers ask a variety of questions. In our data the
adults ask various types of questions, sometimes 'how' and
'why' questions, but often questions which only require a
one-word answer (cf Example 3): of the 1326 teacher questions in our
data 935 (71%) are closed questions. Open questions are used to extend
the conversation, such as in Example 7, where Sara is looking at
Okko's construction and asking him to explain aspects that she is
not aware of:
Example 7
79 SC Why is the door going to
open out like that?
80 Ok Because it's ... because I
want it to, because it
can't open by itself.
81 SC Oh, it can't open by
itself. Is it going to
be, like, an electric door?
82 Ok Yes, it's an electric door.
83 SC So it goes up or does it
go out? Does it go up to
let people in, Okko?
84 Ok Yes.
85 SC It does, fantastic.
In this segment, Sara follows her open 'why' question
with three yes/no questions, in order to encourage Okko to talk more
specifically about the door, using his name to get his attention.
Redirecting questions
If a child does not give a preferred response or does not respond,
the adults will rarely criticise. In accordance with early childhood
pedagogy, they use various feedback strategies to elicit a response, or
in some cases may redirect the question, either by asking a different
child, or by framing the question in another way, such as by using
Modality.
Modalised questions
A negative response can sometimes be avoided by use of Modal verbs
(markers of probability or uncertainty), as in Example 8.
Example 8
381 LA What part of the building
do you think that [a wooden
block] would be used; where
would you use ...?
382 Yl A bridge.
383 LA On a bridge? Okay. You
might use it on a bridge
outside.
Above, Laura uses the Modal verbs 'would' and
'might' and the Mental process 'think' to encourage
Yolanda to suggest an answer even if she is not certain.
As can be seen from the analysis samples for Speech Function and
Exchange Structure above, the adults shape the discourse by determining
who speaks, for how long, and on which general topic, and the children
are encouraged to contribute to the content of the exchanges. Questions
are a very important part of the adults' repertoire, having the
functions of allocating turns, directing the discourse, and helping the
children to extend their thinking and their contributions.
Register
We now turn to the concept of Register, and here focus firstly on
Field and secondly on the Tenor of the discourse. Field involves:
* the subject matter and how it is organised,
* topics of conversation, and
* vocabulary;
Tenor concerns the construction of relationships among the
participants. Our discussion indicates what these analyses can teach us
about teacher dialogue strategies.
Field
Introducing vocabulary
In many of the observed lessons there was a strong focus on
vocabulary, a key part of inducting the students into the discourse of
science and technology. For instance, the teachers directly named some
items, as with the tile in Example 4, above. We have already mentioned
vocabulary being introduced in 'suggesting' questions such as,
'Was it easy or hard?', where the child simply needs to choose
one item from two provided by the teacher. Laura explicitly refers to
vocabulary in requests such as, 'I want some good words to write up
on our brainstorm.' She is persistent in eliciting the descriptive
vocabulary, as can be seen in Example 9.
Example 9
52 LA Can you give me some more words about
those bricks? Mario, what do you think? Have
you had a good feel? What do you think about
bricks, Mario? Are they smooth? They're very
hard.
Above is another example of a 'suggesting' question,
where Laura provides the word 'smooth' as a possible answer.
However, she is open to other suggestions from Mario.
Sometimes the vocabulary is jointly constructed between teacher and
children, as in Example 10.
Example 10
30 SC I wonder how it's staying up there.
31 J Because sticks are holding it up.
32 O Yeah, because poles are here.
33 SC You can touch it. Touch it and
see if they are sticks.
Does that feel like sticks?
34 O They're poles.
35 SC What are poles made from?
In this example Jessica and Otto provide names for the roof
supports, but it is Otto's suggestion of 'poles' that is
endorsed by the teacher.
Jointly-constructed spoken tasks such as those in which these
children were involved over the nine weeks have been described as
'Activity-based Sharing' (EDWA, 1997, p. 3). Children
gradually develop the types of language they need to complete these
tasks: the least challenging language involves labelling, followed by
describing, with classifying being the most difficult (EDWA, 1997, p.
3). Vocabulary in the present data set was used in these three different
ways.
Overall, the vocabulary was mostly at the Labelling end; however
the teachers also did some Describing and Classifying with the children
who they knew were capable of using these functions. With children who
needed greater input there was a lot of repetition to reinforce the
vocabulary. All three teachers also used some quite advanced vocabulary
with the five-yearolds, for example alfresco, pergola, ventilation, heft
and palaeontologist, and the children showed evidence of understanding
these words in context.
Teacher generalisations
To help children organise their knowledge of the Field, Sara
provided generalisations, as in Example 11.
Example 11
109 Hn Some houses are made
out of leaves.
110 SC Are made out of ...?
111 Hn Leaves.
112 SC Leaves, yes. So they're
made out of different materials.
Here she connects 'leaves' and 'materials',
relating the discussion to the exploration of building materials over
the nine weeks, and simultaneously drawing a conclusion, thus modelling
an aspect of scientific literacy.
Focus on scientific processes
As well as developing the children's Field-related vocabulary,
the teachers also focussed on scientific processes. For example, Sara
concentrated on the key process of observation (cf. DuVall, 2001) by
firstly asking the children to feel a bike rack, and secondly getting
them to look at what shape it was and what material it could be made
from.
Tenor
Relationship constructed with children
Sara, in particular, constructed quite an egalitarian relationship
with the children, given the fact that they were five years old. She
assumed that the children may have diverse experiences of the world; for
example, she questioned Hannah about the location of a park she visited,
implying that it might be overseas. She also made a joke about her own
position as a teacher, thus decreasing the distance between herself and
the children:
Example 12
601 Aide they just discussed what
would be in a community.
602 SC Oh. And what was one thing
that a community needs?
A school? ... You didn't
have a school?
603 Aide No.
604 SC Maybe next week they'll
put a school in the
community; I hope so.
605 Aide Maybe.
606 SC Otherwise we won't
have a job.
Sara's discourse could be described as 'high-level'
in some respects, and to an extent Laura's discourse was also at an
advanced level in relation to the age of the children. Sara was very
inclusive and responded positively to the varied circumstances of the
children in the class. She demonstrated an international outlook and
often mentioned things about other countries, connecting with the
children's cultural, linguistic and family backgrounds. For
example, in talking to Cristiano she used the Spanish word for
'house'. Speaking with Yolanda, she included discussion of the
family's religious practices, and this elicited a long stretch of
discourse from Yolanda, who was normally rather quiet. Sara talked about
different places of worship, including temples, synagogues and mosques,
in contrast to teacher Laura, who emphasised Christian churches in her
presentation to the class. Space precludes us giving examples of
Sara's inclusive discourse here but it would certainly be a useful
model for beginning teachers who are striving to incorporate such
discourse into their practice. This is, of course, relevant to all
learning areas, not just science and technology.
Discussion
The analyses selected have illustrated some aspects of the
teachers' conversational strategies that helped their students to
learn about science and technology in the contexts we observed.
In this series of learning experiences the adults facilitated the
children's learning by:
* Controlling the structure of the discourse as it unfolded.
* Controlling turn-taking, attempting to include as many children
as possible in the interactions.
* Asking various types of questions, in some cases 'how'
and 'why' questions, but often questions that only required a
one-word answer, sometimes as part of encouraging children to
participate.
* Eliciting and introducing vocabulary. Teachers Sara and Laura
used some advanced vocabulary, which the children demonstrated that they
understood.
* Facilitating use of vocabulary for labelling, describing and
sometimes classifying, to help develop children's knowledge of the
Field.
* Constructing inclusive relationships with the children.
While we have dealt with each analytic device separately, clearly
all of the strategies used by the adults were being employed
simultaneously, which serves to underline the adults' linguistic
dexterity in juggling all these discourse behaviours while at the same
time managing groups of up to fifty five-year-olds.
Having access to a toolbox of analyses such as these can help
teachers reflect on their practice. While some aspects of classroom
discourse may appear self evident and can be accessed through simply
listening to an audio-recording, it is not until one actually delves
deeper by, for example, analysing the number and type of turns from each
participant, that one gains further insight into 'what is going
on' and, perhaps more importantly, how it is going on. Some of the
linguistic patterns of these teachers are quite subtle and could be
easily overlooked, especially by pre-service teachers.
Having some analytical concepts at their fingertips can also help
teachers talk about their practice more easily. A case in point is the
dk1, a much less cumbersome term than 'teacher knows the answer
question'. In our work with teachers we have found that they
benefit from looking at transcripts (and if possible, videos) of their
own classrooms and appreciate being given some questions and analytical
tools to aid their self-reflection. (See Thwaite & Rivalland, 2009.)
However, the analysis is more than the sum of its parts. A holistic
analysis necessitates a good foundational knowledge about language and
the ability to draw on various tools to describe interactions. It takes
some time to learn how to do this and our aim in this paper has not been
to instruct readers in how to do the analyses. Instead, we have
attempted to share with readers how these analyses have helped us to
notice some characteristics of the excellent work of teachers such as
Sara, Laura and Davida. In a paper of this length, there has not been
space to discuss in detail the context of each of the strategies used by
the teachers and how they planned, set up and carried out their
interactions. This is a limitation of our discussion and is an area that
it would be useful to develop in the future.
Finally, while our discussion has been limited to our sample data,
which focuses on the science and technology learning area, the
techniques illustrated could be readily applied to other subject areas.
This too could be a topic for further research and discussion.
Conclusions
We contend that trainee teachers can learn about classroom
discourse from observing teachers like these. Anlaysing talk is not an
easy task, as some of its features are very subtle. For example, we
sometimes advise preservice teachers to, 'Make sure everyone has a
turn'. However, an overemphasis on equitable turn-taking could be
detrimental to the discourse in other respects, such as a detailed
development of concepts. Classroom discourse is a highly complex area
and we have barely touched on its complexity here. One area which we
have not gone into here but which we experience in our daily work is
pre-service teachers' experiences with the concepts discussed here;
we would welcome the opportunity to undertake further research on this.
Our analysis has focussed on linguistic variables. Obviously there
was much going on in the learning environment in terms of non-verbal
behaviour, but this aspect of the conversation would require another
paper.
Acknowledgements
We would like to remember the late Dr Ann Galloway for her initial
leadership of the research team and for all her work in linguistics and
diversity.
We thank the following people for their assistance: Sandra Parsons
for her help with recording the data and the literature review.
Debra Webb from Dictatum Transcription Services for her excellent
transcripts.
Dr John Hall for his comments on earlier versions of this paper.
The AJLL reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of
this paper.
This study formed part of the project, 'Early Science Literacy
in Indigenous and Culturally Diverse Communities', funded by a
university small grant 2009-2010.
References
Australian Curriculum, Assessment and Reporting Authority (ACARA)
(2012). The Australian Curriculum v.3.0 Science. Rationale. Retrieved
from, http://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/Science/Rationale
Berry, M. (1981a). Systemic linguistics and discourse analysis: A
multi-layered approach to Exchange Structure. In M. Coulthard & M.
Montgomery (Eds.) Studies in Discourse Analysis (pp. 120-145). London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Berry, M. (1981b). Polarity, ellipticity, elicitation and
propositional development, their relevance to the well-formedness of an
exchange. Nottingham Linguistic Circular, 10(1), 36-63.
Berry, M. (1981c). Towards layers of Exchange Structure for
directive exchanges. Network, 2, 23-32.
Christie, F. (2002). Classroom Discourse Analysis: A Functional
Perspective. London: Continuum.
Derewianka, B. (2011). A New Grammar Companion for Primary
Teachers. Sydney: PETA.
Droga, L. & Humphrey, S. (2003). Grammar and the Organisation
of Meaning. Berry, N.S.W.: Target Texts.
DuVall, Rick (2001). Inquiry in science: From curiosity to
understanding. Primary Voices K-6, 10(1), 3-9.
Education Department of Western Australia (EDWA). (1997). First
Steps Oral Language Resource Book. Melbourne: Rigby Heinemann.
Halliday, M.A.K. (1993). Towards a language-based theory of
learning. Linguistics and Education, 5, 93-116.
Halliday, M.A.K. (1994). An Introduction to Functional Grammar (2nd
ed.). London: Edward Arnold.
Halliday, M.A.K. & Matthiessen, C.M.M. (2004). An
Introduction to Functional Grammar (3rd ed.). London: Edward
Arnold.
Jones, P., Kervin, L. & McIntosh, S. (2011). The interactive
whiteboard: Tool and/or agent of semiotic mediation. Australian Journal
of Language and Literacy, 34(1), 38-60.
Martin, J.R. & Rose, D. (2007). Working with Discourse: Meaning
Beyond the Clause. London: Continuum.
Painter, C. (1998). Learning Through Language in Early Childhood.
London: Cassell.
Rennie, L.J. (2005). Science awareness and scientific literacy.
Teaching Science, 51(1), 10-14.
Rennie, L.J., Goodrum, D. & Hackling, M. (2001). Science
teaching and learning in Australian schools: Results of a national
study. Research in Science Education, 31, 455-498.
Shepardson, D.P. & Britsch, S.J. (2001). The role of
children's journals in elementary school science activities.
Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(1), 43-69.
Sinclair, J. & Coulthard, M. (1975). Towards an Analysis of
Discourse: The English Used by Teachers and Pupils. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Thwaite, A. (1993). 'Gender Differences in Spoken Interaction
in Same Sex Dyadic Conversations in Australian English'. Australian
Review of Applied Linguistics, Series S, Issue 10 'Language and
Gender in the Australian Context' (Ed. Wigglesworth & Winter),
149-179.
Thwaite, A. & Rivalland, J. (2009). How can analysis of
classroom talk help teachers reflect on their practices? Australian
Journal of Language and Literacy, 32(1), 38-54.
Ventola, E. (1987). The Structure of Social Interaction: A Systemic
Approach to the Semiotics of Service Encounters. London: Pinter.
Anne Thwaite
Edith Cowan University
Grtaham Mckay
St. John's of Tanzania
Anne Thwaite is a lecturer in Language and Literacy Education at
Edith Cowan University in Western Australia, where she works training
primary teachers. She has previously taught at the University of Western
Sydney, the University of Sydney and in China. Her research interests
include classroom discourse analysis and functional grammar.
Graham McKay is an Associate Professor and Dean of the Faculty of
Humanities and Education, St John's University of Tanzania. After
completing his PhD in linguistics he worked for ten years in bilingual
education and in training literacy workers in Indigenous languages at
Maningrida and Batchelor in the Northern Territory. He has also taught
applied linguistics for 25 years at Edith Cowan University where he
continues to hold an honorary appointment.
Table 1. Number and length of turns
Non-verbal turns are not included. Turn length is given in lines.
Speaker Number of Maximum
turns length
Teachers
Sara 19
Davida 385 22
Laura 376 16
Other adults
Mrs Edgar (teacher aide) 226 15
Mrs Naxos (teacher aide) 95 10
Mary (chaplain) 88 11
Father (parent helper) 3 1
Tt/Ts = adults together 7 1
T = unidentified adult 62 6
Students
Albert 3 1
Alex 43 2
Alison 6 1
Amanda 30 2
Amy 14 1
Anna 13 2
Astika 1 1
Betty 7 1
Bruce 79 3
Cara 20 4
Chad 12 2
Cristiano 311 8
Dennis 6 1
Eddie 1 1
Elena B 1 1
Elena C 1 1
Elena X 19 1
Enda 1 1
Hannah 115 2
Jackson 4 1
Jacob 7 1
Jessica 19 1
Joe 20 2
Juan 11 1
Karen 36 4
Karina 5 1
Katrina 98 2
Kim 10 1
Kylie 6 1
Lorenzo 36 4
May 7 1
Mike 8 1
O (not clear if Okko or 5 1
Otto)
Okko 221 3
Otto 33 4
Patricia 10 1
Pearl 2 1
Roberto 7 1
Sasha 5 2
Susan 6 1
Yolanda 44 2
Zerlina 50 1
Ss = students together 96 3
S = unknown student 727 3
Researcher 1 35 2
Researcher 2 2 2
Table 2. Adults' Questions
dk1 = turn contains one or more dk1s ('teacher knows
the answer' moves)
Speaker dk1 Question type
open closed
Sara (teacher)
total 80 157 (23%) 525 (77%)
TOTAL Qs 682
Davida (teacher)
total 90 101 (30%) 235 (70%)
TOTAL Qs 336
Laura (teacher)
total 39 133 (43%) 175 (57%)
TOTAL Qs 308
Mrs Edgar (teacher aide)
total 119 64 (21%) 237 (79%)
TOTAL Qs 301
Mary (chaplain)
19 10 (14%) 59 (86%)
TOTAL Qs 69
Mrs Naxos (teacher aide)
1 7 (12%) 52 (88%)
TOTAL Qs 59
Mrs Stoppard (teacher aide)
0 0 2 (100%)
TOTAL Qs 2
Leonie (parent helper)
0 1 (100%)
TOTAL Qs 1