Word identification, metacognitive knowledge, motivation and reading comprehension: an Australian study of Grade 3 and 4 pupils.
van Kraayenoord, Christina E. ; Beinicke, Andrea ; Schlagmuller, Matthias 等
Introduction
Reading comprehension is an important literacy competency developed
in the early years of schooling. From a self-regulated learning
perspective, reading comprehension involves the interaction of
cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational variables (e.g., Dignath
& Buttner, 2008). Research has suggested that good reading
comprehension is the result of the use of a range of these variables
including word identification and decoding abilities (Chapman, Tunmer
& Prochnow, 2000), knowledge of cognitive and metacognitive
strategies, such as planning and self-monitoring (Pressley & Harris,
2006), and motivational aspects of learning, such as self-concept and
interest (Miller & Faircloth, 2009). There is also evidence that
pupils who apply cognitive and metacognitive strategies are better
comprehenders (Paris, Lipson & Wixson, 1994), and that the training
of such variables can lead to improved reading comprehension (Pressley,
2006). Gender may also lead to differences in reading comprehension
(Logan & Johnston, 2010) The aim of this article is to explore the
role that these variables play in the reading comprehension of
Australian male and female pupils in Grades 3 and 4.
In the following section, we briefly summarise the major research
findings concerning the state-of-the art with respect to the acquisition
of reading comprehension by making reference to the role of word
identification, metacognitive knowledge and motivation in reading
comprehension. With respect to motivation we focus on the intrinsic
motivation variables of reading self-concept and interest. We also
highlight the research that discusses issues of gender difference in
reading comprehension. Thereafter we outline the rationale and main
goals of the present study. But we begin by defining reading
comprehension, the main outcome variable in our study.
Reading comprehension refers to making meaning at the word,
sentence and text level. It involves the dynamic interplay of a range of
knowledge, processes and strategies (Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003).
Successful reading comprehension occurs as a result of the interaction
between both reader and text factors (Sweet & Snow, 2003). Of
interest in this study are some of the reader variables that are brought
to the reading comprehension process. One of the main variables that has
been extensively studied in relation to reading comprehension is that of
word identification.
A pupil's ability to read words accurately influences their
reading comprehension (Jenkins, Fuchs, van den Broek, Espin, & Deno,
2003) and is a strong predictor of reading comprehension (Vellutino,
Scanlon, & Tanzman, 1994). Successful word identification skills
depend upon the effective utilisation of the alphabetic code and
identifying words easily and rapidly. When word identification is fluent
and decoding becomes accurate and automatic, cognitive resources are
freed up so that the meaning of what is being read can be derived. In
this way accurate word identification and decoding at a level of
automaticity allow pupils to place their efforts into comprehension
(Samuels, 2006). Thus successful reading comprehension occurs when
readers are able to accurately and rapidly identify words and to use
non-visual information such as grammatical and semantic knowledge to
work out what the text says. In turn these contribute to establishing
what the text means.
Metacognitive knowledge, an aspect of metacognition, is also
important for successful reading comprehension. Metacognitive knowledge
refers to the declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge
associated with learning, for example learning to read (Pressley, 2002).
Several studies have emphasised the importance of declarative and
procedural metacognitive knowledge for the development of reading
comprehension (see reviews Schneider & Pressley, 1997). Measures of
declarative metacognitive knowledge seem particularly well-suited to
predicting reading comprehension in children. For instance,
Schlagmuller, Vise, and Schneider (2001) showed that metamemory as
assessed by the Wurzburg Metamemory Test (Vise, Schlagmuller, &
Schneider, 1998) was closely associated with memory and was a good
predictor of reading comprehension in primary school children. The
Wurzburg Metamemory Test (Vise, Schlagmuller, & Schneider, 1998) was
used in this study.
While cognitive and metacognitive knowledge and strategies are
important to reading comprehension, a student's affect also
influences their comprehension. Indeed, Guthrie and Wigfield (2000) have
argued that 'motivational processes are the foundation for
coordinating cognitive goals and strategies in reading' (p. 408),
and Wang and Guthrie (2004) found that intrinsic motivation was
positively related to reading comprehension, after controlling for other
variables such as extrinsic motivation and amount of reading.
Motivation in reading is comprised of a number of intrinsic and
extrinsic variables (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 2001). As indicated
earlier, in this study we focus on two variables identified with
intrinsic motivation, namely self-concept and interest. With respect to
the relationship with self-concept and academic achievement in general,
a meta-analysis carried out by Hattie (2009) found a positive
correlation of.43 between self-concept and achievement. Many researchers
have examined self-concept in relationship to specific academic domains,
such as reading. Reading self-concept refers to the ability to make a
cognitive appraisal of one's comprehension and learning (Marsh,
1990). Chapman and Tunmer (1995) investigated the relationship between
reading self-concept, letter and word identification and comprehension
strategies amongst primary school pupils between 5 to 7 years of age.
They found that reading self-concept correlated positively with
comprehension strategies for the older pupils, while perceptions of task
difficulty correlated significantly with reading strategies for the
younger pupils. More recent findings suggest that pupils with lower
reading self-concepts performed more poorly on reading tasks and had
lower overall academic self-concept scores (Chapman, Tunmer, &
Prochnow, 2000). Rider and Colmar (2006) also found a strong positive
relationship between reading achievement, namely accuracy and
comprehension, and reading self-concept scores for children in Grade 3.
Interest is another variable related to intrinsic motivation which
was also examined in this study. Interest is defined as either a
characteristic of the person or of the text (Renninger, Hidi, &
Krapp, 1992). A number of researchers have argued that a student's
level of interest can be activated and can vary according to both the
situation and the context (e.g., Schiefele, 1991). Several studies have
found that students' perceptions of interest in reading influence
reading comprehension (Guthrie & Wigfield, 1999). As is the case
with self-concept, these studies have indicated that interest is related
to increased learning, persistence and effort. Specifically, interest in
reading has been found to influence students' engagement with text,
as well the quality of their comprehension (Wigfield, 1997).
Gender differences have consistently been demonstrated in the
relevant literature, showing that girls are better at reading
comprehension than boys. As Logan and Johnston's (2010) recent
review noted this finding is evident in several studies (e.g., Mullis,
Martin, Kennedy, & Foy, 2007), regardless of the type of instruction
the pupils have received (Ming Chui & McBride-Chang, 2006).
Gender differences of other reading-related variables have also
been found in a number of studies. For example, research related to
pupils' reading attitudes and reading motivation and their
association with or prediction of reading comprehension has consistently
demonstrated gender differences (e.g., Wang & Guthrie, 2004), with
girls scoring more highly than boys. A number of authors have argued
that self-concept is determined by one's frame of reference. That
is, pupils compare themselves and their performance with their local
setting, namely the achievement of their peers, and possibly same-gender
peers, and make comparisons related, first and foremost, to peers in
their own classroom and school (Moller & Koller, 2004; Pekrun &
Zirngibl, 2004). Thus this study allowed us to explore issues related to
these motivational variables with respect to gender differences, and
this issue of frame of reference.
Rationale for the study and research questions
The present study was conducted by a group of international
researchers who are interested in the reading comprehension and the
variables that impact on it. The study is situated within
social-cognitive models of self-regulated learning (e.g., Winne, 2005).
While there are a number of models of self-regulated learning (see
Puustinen & Pulkkinen, 2001 for a review), this study fits best
within models of self-regulated learning that consider self-regulated
learning to be an interaction of cognitive, metacognitive, and
motivational processes (e.g., Boekaerts & Corno, 2005).
The study draws on the literature summarised above that indicates
that the variables of word identification, metacognitive knowledge, and
motivation are related to reading comprehension. In terms of
metacognitive knowledge, we specifically chose to examine the role of
declarative metamemory. In the area of motivation, we chose to examine
reading self-concept and interest in reading. The study not only
examined the relationships among those variables associated in the
literature with the development of reading comprehension, but also used
teacher judgment as a measure of reading achievement. Specifically,
reading comprehension was assessed by combining scores from a
standardised measure of reading comprehension and a measure of teacher
judgment of reading achievement. The study addressed the following
research questions:
1. Are there systematic gender differences on the variables of
interest?
2. Do the variables of interest correlate and what is the pattern
of intercorrelations?
3. Do the variables of interest predict reading comprehension?
4. Does the causal structural model apply to the data?
We assumed that the general model (based on the regression
analyses) would apply to the data, with an expectation that word
identification would have an impact on reading comprehension in the
sample.
Method
The participants
The study involved a sample of 139 Australian third (M = 30, F =
31) and fourth (M = 34, F = 44) graders drawn from two small primary
schools in adjoining suburbs of the city of Brisbane, Queensland,
Australia. The population of School A was described as being from a
'middle' socio-economic background. The population of School B
was described as being from a 'mixed' socioeconomic
background. None of the children were identified as having
developmental, emotional or sensory disabilities. The teachers of these
pupils also participated in the study. There were four Grade 3 teachers
(male N = 2, female N = 2) and four Grade 4 teachers (female N = 4). The
Grade 3 teachers had between 12 and 30 years of teaching experience
(Mean = 18.75, Median = 16.5) and the Grade 4 teachers had between 6 and
30 years (Mean = 21, Median = 24) of teaching experience.
Instruments
A number of different instruments were used to assess performance
in word identification, metacognitive knowledge, reading self-concept,
interest in reading, and reading comprehension.
The Word Identification Subtest--Test 3 of the Woodcock Reading
Mastery Tests-Revised (Form H, Woodcock, 1987). The subtest requires the
identification of single words that appear in a list of 100 items
arranged in order of difficulty. The test was administered individually
and testing stopped when the pupil identified six consecutive words
incorrectly. The test requires accurate word reading. The pupils'
raw scores were calculated. The variable was labelled WORDID. The
split-half reliability coefficients for Form H for Grade 3 is [r.sub.sh]
= .97.
Index of Reading Awareness (Jacobs & Paris, 1987). This is a
twenty-item test that assesses pupils' metacognitive knowledge
about strategies that can be used in reading. It is a multiple-choice
questionnaire that measures evaluation, planning, regulation and
conditional knowledge. Five questions measure each aspect of
metacognition. Pupils select one of three responses that they believe to
be the best strategy in a described reading scenario. The test was
administered to the class as a whole and took 10 min. Each response is
scored as 0 for an inappropriate response, 1 for a partially adequate
answer, or 2 for a strategic response, with a range from 0-40. A
student's total score (AWARE) was calculated. Cronbach's a for
the 20 items was.56. A study by McLain, Gridley and McIntosh (1991)
obtained a Cronbach's alpha of.61. These authors suggested that the
Scale is acceptable if used as a total score. This was done in the
current study.
Wurzburg Metamemory Test (Wurzburger Metagedachtnistest) (Vise,
Schlagmuller, & Schneider, 1998, rev. ed, transl). This is a test of
declarative metamemory. It comprises 3 subscales. The first subscale
('Dolphin') assesses general metamemory related to person,
task and strategy variables and consists of 15 items. Possible scores
range from 0 to 30. The second subtest ('Seal') comprises 18
items which assesses strategies related to text processing, specifically
task-related knowledge of clustering strategies for recall. Possible
scores range from 0 to 36. 'Elephant' comprises 17 items and
assesses knowledge of semantic categorisation strategies. Possible
scores on this third subtest range from 0 to 34. (Contact the first
author for a copy of the items in the subscales). The test was
administered in a whole class setting and took 40 min. The range for the
total score on this test (METAMEM) is 0 to 100. Cronbach's a
was.74. The test-retest reliability after four months on the original
version of this test was.70 (Vise, Schlagmuller, & Schneider, 1998).
Reading Self-Concept Scale (Chapman & Tunmer, 1993). This is a
30-item measure that assesses reading self-concept. It comprises three
scales, namely Difficulty, Attitude and Competence that Chapman and
Tunmer argue constitute three separate, but related aspects of reading
self-concept. The Difficulty scale assesses perceptions of difficulty
with reading such as pupils' beliefs that reading activities are
hard or problematic. The Attitude scale assesses attitudes towards
reading, specifically pupils' feelings towards and affinity for
reading, while the Competence scale assesses perceptions of competence
such as beliefs regarding ability and proficiency in reading tasks. The
test took 20 minutes to administer as a whole class test. The total
score (Reading Self-Concept Total: SELFCON) is expressed on a 5-point
scale. Cronbach's a was .76. Other studies have also revealed the
test's strong psychometric properties (Chapman & Tunmer, 1995;
Rider & Colmar, 2006).
Interest in Reading Scale (Lesen Interessen Skala) (van
Kraayenoord, 1996, transl) The scale comprises 10 items. Four items
relate to children's attitudes towards reading such as 'I like
to read books containing stories' and to texts of different types,
and the other six items relate to children's habits and behaviours
associated with reading, such as 'I get books for my birthday and
at Christmas'. The test was administered to the whole class group
and took 5 min. The children responded on a 3-point Likert-type scale (1
= Not true, 2 = Sometimes true, 3 = Always true). The range of possible
scores is 10 (low) to 21 (high) (INTEREST). Cronbach's a was.59,
indicating moderate but still sufficient consistency.
Tests of Reading Comprehension (TORCH) (Mossenson, Hill &
Masters, 1995). The passage 'Lizards Love Eggs' (A3) was used
to collect the data for 87 of the pupils in the sample. The
Kuder-Richardson Reliability Coefficient, KR 20 for Test Booklet A.
Passage A3. Lizards Love Eggs was [r.sub.tt] =.91. The two classes of
Grade 4 pupils in School A had undertaken the Tests of Reading
Comprehension (TORCH) using the passage A2. 'The Bear who Liked
Hugging Trees' close to the data collection period. According to
the TORCH Manual passage A2 overlaps with that of A3 and so the TORCH
scores from the 52 pupils who had been tested using passage A2 were used
in the study rather than retesting them. The TORCH involves the silent
reading of the passage and then the completion of a cloze retelling of
the passage by providing the correct answers in writing. The test items
measure literal and inferential comprehension and pupils' abilities
to synthesise information presented. The test was administered in whole
class groups. The TORCH score was calculated for each student according
to the instructions in the Manual. The variable was identified as TORCH.
The Kuder-Richardson Reliability Coefficient KR 20 for Test Booklet A,
Passage A2, The Bear Who liked Hugging People was [r.sub.tt] =.93
(Mossenson, Hill, & Masters, 1995). A pilot study by Burgon (1988)
examining the relationship between the TORCH and the Progressive
Achievement Test-Comprehension (PAT-RC; Elley & Reid, 1969) and the
TORCH and teacher ratings of their pupils' reading comprehension on
a 1 to 5 scale on 350 New Zealand students revealed that pupils'
scores on the TORCH correlated well with the PAT-RC and with teacher
ratings. The author also commented that the given that teacher ratings
were based on standard classroom practice, the results point to the
validity of TORCH for use in New Zealand classrooms. While it is not
possible to draw inferences between practices in New Zealand and
Australia, the PAT-RC and teacher judgments are also common assessment
practices in Australia.
Teacher Judgment of Reading Achievement (Lehrerfragebogen zur
Einschatzung der Lesefahigkeit) (Adapted from Marx, 1998, transl).
Teachers were asked to rate each child on 6 subscales (TEACH 1 to TEACH
6). TEACH 1 asked teachers to make a judgement about the child's
reading ability in general. For example for TEACH 1 that item stated,
'I judge the overall reading ability of the child as -, and the
teacher responded with a rating from 1 = 'Far above average'
to 7 = 'Far below average'. TEACH 2 asked teachers to make a
judgement about the children's ability to read a new, unknown,
age-appropriate text. TEACH 3 asked teachers to make a judgement about
the children's ability to read words of new, unknown texts. TEACH 4
asked teachers to make a judgement about the children's ability to
comprehend new, unknown texts. TEACH 5 asked teachers to make a
judgement the children's ability to write words of unknown texts
and TEACH 6 asked teachers to make a judgement about the children's
ability to ability to comprehend following listening to new, unknown
texts. Each Likert-type scale was used to assign a rating from 1 (low)
to 7 (high). The mean of TEACH 1 to TEACH 6 was used to create a total
score (TEACHJUDGE). Cronbach's a was. 97.
A word about the translation
In this study, the German language tests, specifically the Teacher
Judgment of Reading Achievement, Wurzburg Metamemory Test, and the
Interest in Reading Scale were translated into English by the first
author. In some instances, alterations were also made to words or
phrases to make them more culturally and contextually appropriate (e.g.,
names of rivers in the Wurzburg Metamemory Test were changed to the
names of Australian rivers). The third and fourth authors whose first
language is German and who are both very competent English speakers
checked the German-English translation for accuracy and appropriateness
of word selection.
Procedure
Ethical clearance for the study was obtained from The University of
Queensland and gatekeeper approval was provided by Education Queensland.
Data was only collected from pupils for whom consent had been obtained
from their parents. The first author and four research assistants
collected the data. Two persons from the research team were in each
classroom during test administration and the classroom teachers were not
present. At the start of the test administration the purpose of the
study was outlined to the class and the pupils were assured of the
confidentiality of their data.
The order of presentation of the tests were as follows: Visit 1:
Wurzburg Metamemory Test; Visit 2: Interest in Reading Scale, Reading
Self-concept Scale, and Index of Reading Awareness; and Visit 3: TORCH.
The Woodcock Word Identification Subtest was administered individually
to each student in an office or empty classroom by the research
assistants on a fourth visit. The first author approached the teachers
individually and the purpose of the Teacher Judgment of Reading
Achievement was explained. Written instructions were provided to the
teachers so that the form could be completed in the teachers' own
time. The completed forms were given to the school secretary from whom
they were collected by the research assistants or the first author.
Data analyses
Prior to undertaking the analyses, for each construct used in the
study (except gender), the raw scores of each subtest were aggregated
and normed (separately) for each grade level in order to avoid different
levels of performance. Next, these scores were converted into z-scores
in order to compare the scores and across the variables.
The correlation between the two metacognitive knowledge measures
(declarative metamemory: METAMEM and metacognitive knowledge about
reading strategies: AWARE) in the sample was r =.35 (p <.01). These
metacognitive knowledge measures were combined to create a latent
variable (META) for the causal modelling. We also created a variable
named total reading comprehension (READCOM) by combining the relevant
z-scores on the pupils' reading comprehension tests (TORCH, r =.69,
p <.01)) and the teacher judgments on the 6 subscales (TEACHJUDGE).
Specifically, the z-scores of each construct (TORCH & TEACHJUDGE)
were aggregated (separately for each grade level to avoid different
levels of performance) and then again converted to z-scores in order to
create a latent variable for the causal modelling that represented the
dependent variable (READCOM).
As indicated earlier, the participants comprised pupils from Grade
3 and 4. Given that the differences between the two grades for Total
reading were not significant the grade levels were combined. Statistical
analyses were performed using SPSS 16.0 for Windows. Comparisons of the
dependent (continuous) variables (test instrument scores) for gender
were undertaken by the use of independent-samples t-tests. A value of p
<.05 was considered significant. Using the effect size, Cohen's
d provides an indication of the magnitude of the mean differences
between the groups.
Results
We first checked the age and gender distribution in the sample.
There were no significant differences in mean ages for males and females
with reference to Grade (3 and 4) (see Table 1), all ps >.05.
Gender differences
There was a significant difference in test scores for males (N =
64) and females (N = 75) with respect to total reading comprehension
(READCOM), reading comprehension (TORCH), and reading self-concept, all
ps <.04 (see Table 2). Specifically, girls outperformed the boys on
all these measures.
Predictors of reading comprehension and their intercorrelations
With respect to the second and third research question we report
the findings of the intercorrelations and stepwise multiple regression
analyses for the sample.
A stepwise multiple regression was performed with the total reading
comprehension score (READCOM) as the dependent variable and the other
variables as independent variables (WORDID, META, SELFCON, INTEREST,
GENDER). Preliminary analyses were conducted to ensure no assumptions
were violated. With respect to multicollinearity, all the independent
variables showed some relationship with the dependent variable.
Additionally, all the bivariate correlations between each of the
independent variables were below the cut-off point of r =.70 (r
<.41). Supported by the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) values (that
measure redundancy between the explanatory variables in the model),
multicollinearity was not violated, that is, < 1.31 and well below
the cut-off of 10. The correlations amongst the 'plain'
z-standardised variables of the sample appear in Table 3.
The word identification variable was the best predictor and
explained 58% of the variance in reading comprehension. In step 2, the
metacognitive knowledge variable was selected to go into the equation
and explained an additional 6% of the variance. Reading self-concept was
identified as the third predictor and was included in the model.
Self-concept contributed to the prediction of reading comprehension and
so the whole model explained 66% of the variance, F(3, 135) = 87.15, p
<.0005. This pattern of results suggests that two-thirds of the
variability in total reading comprehension was predicted by word
identification, metacognitive knowledge and reading self-concept. In the
final model, the three measures were statistically significant, with
word identification recording the highest [beta] value ([beta] =.61, p
<.001), followed by metacognitive knowledge ([beta] =.24, p <.001)
and reading self-concept ([beta] =.17, p =.002) (see Table 4).
In sum, word identification explained 58% out of a total of 66% of
the variance in total reading comprehension. Interestingly, reading
self-concept added little to the prediction of reading comprehension.
Causal modelling
In order to assess the interrelationships among predictor variables
and determine the construct validity of the model, a latent variable
causal modelling approach, using AMOS 16.0 was carried out. This allowed
us to assess the causal influences of the various variables on reading
comprehension. Preliminary analyses revealed that gender and grade did
not have a reliable impact when included as exogenous variables. In
addition, the relationship coherence between gender and total reading
comprehension was r =.17 (p <.01) for the sample. Therefore these
variables were not considered in our subsequent causal modelling. The
motivational variables (reading self-concept and interest) formed a
single latent construct. As reported earlier, metacognitive knowledge
comprised the variables of declarative metamemory and metacognitive
knowledge about reading strategies. The total reading comprehension
variable comprised two measures--reading comprehension (TORCH) and
teachers' judgment of reading. The correlation among these
variables was r =.69 (p <.01). The results of the best-fitting causal
model for the sample are shown in Figure 1.
In this sample both, word identification and metacognitive
knowledge influenced reading comprehension directly, although there was
a stronger path between word identification and reading comprehension
than between metacognitive knowledge and reading comprehension. In
addition, motivation had a direct effect on reading comprehension, as
well as via metacognitive knowledge and word identification. The
efficiency of the model was evaluated by several goodness-of-fit indices
that summarise the discrepancy between the observed values and the
expected values for the model. Overall, the various measures showed
acceptable data fit for the model. The Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index
(AGFI) was.87, and the Root Mean Residual (RMR) score was. 07. The Root
Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) was
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Finally, we examined the patterns of interrelationships among
predictor variables. Word identification and metacognitive knowledge
influenced reading comprehension directly. There was a strong path
between word identification and reading comprehension, r =.65, with a
less strong path between metacognitive knowledge and reading
comprehension, r =.41. Motivation had a direct effect on reading
comprehension as well as via metacognitive knowledge and word
identification. Thus, word identification substantially influenced
reading comprehension in the sample.
Discussion
This article has reported on the results of an investigation of
reading comprehension involving 139 Australian male and female pupils in
Grades 3 and 4. Specifically, the pupils' word identification,
metacognitive knowledge and motivation in relationship to their reading
comprehension were examined.
The study found no significant differences in mean ages for males
and females in this sample. Gender differences were found for total
reading comprehension, reading comprehension, and reading self-concept,
with girls performing better than boys on these variables.
These findings of gender differences in reading comprehension align
with the results from the majority of studies that have found
differences between boys and girls in reading comprehension (Logan &
Johnston, 2010). With respect to gender differences in reading
self-concept, this finding provides some support for the findings of the
studies by Moller and Koller (2004) and Pekrun and Zirngibl (2004) who
have pointed to the importance of one's frame of reference in
determining one's reading self-concept.
The findings relating to the stepwise regression analyses indicated
that reading comprehension was predicted strongly by word
identification. In addition, the causal modelling found that the model
created basically fit the data set. In the model there was a direct link
between word identification and reading comprehension and between
motivation and reading comprehension. Both metacognitive knowledge and
word identification were moderator variables between motivation and
reading comprehension. Thus motivation influenced reading comprehension,
but was also mediated by both metacognitive knowledge and word
identification. These results partially mirror the results of causal
modelling undertaken in an earlier study of German pupils by van
Kraayenoord and Schneider (1999) and the follow-up study by
Roeschl-Heils, Schneider, and van Kraayenoord (2003). Specifically, in
these studies the models indicated that metacognition had a direct
effect on reading comprehension, and motivation had an indirect effect
via word identification and metacognition. The role of word
identification was more salient in this study that involved reading
comprehension in the English language than was found in the German
language studies.
The findings of the causal modelling revealed that word
identification ability was strongly related to reading comprehension.
Goswami (2008) has indicated that children have difficulty acquiring
reading because phonology is complex with varying levels of consistency.
This finding suggests that third and fourth Grade pupils still may need
instruction (and intervention) in word identification in these years of
primary school (Juel, 1988; Torgesen, 2000). Indeed with respect to
implications of this finding for practice in the Australian school
system there is a need for teachers to emphasise the development of the
word identification abilities of their pupils in the early years of
reading. Intervention programs that promote the development of word
identification, reading comprehension and knowledge of strategies are
especially necessary for pupils at-risk for reading failure (Tunmer
& Chapman, 2002; Woolley, 2007).
Of interest is the finding that the motivational variables added
little to the pupils' reading comprehension. One possible reason
for the lack of impact of the motivation construct is that only two
relevant indicators (i.e., reading self-concept and interest) were
considered in this study. We know from other studies (e.g., Paris &
Paris, 2001) that other concepts such as perceived self-efficacy might
also play an important role. Another reason could be that individual
differences in learning motivation are not that pronounced in third or
fourth grade, but prove more important for the prediction of learning
outcomes as pupils become older (Pintrich & Schrauben, 1992).
Including additional motivational indicators could have improved the
predictive quality of this construct and future studies could
investigate the influence of other intrinsic motivation variables on
reading comprehension.
There are several limitations of our study that are important to
note. First, the sample is not representative of all Australian children
and was limited to pupils from two schools in Brisbane. Second, the
translated instruments were not piloted. Third, we caution that these
results cannot be generalised beyond the particular variables related to
reading comprehension that we investigated. Additional studies exploring
these issues with other samples at other grade levels are required.
A strength of this study was the use of different data analysis
techniques, that is both regression analyses and causal modelling. This
meant that the interplay among the constructs of interest in this study
could be carefully investigated.
There are several implications for research and practice. There is
a clear need for effective initial teaching of reading and reading
comprehension. Teachers in the early years of schooling need to ensure
that all the elements of early reading acquisition are taught
systematically and thoroughly. Pupils' progress should be monitored
and early identification of difficulties should be followed up with
early intervention (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Education systems
and schools should identify programs that will promote reading
comprehension. Such programs must pay attention to the development of
word-level skills as well as the development of reading comprehension
(National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000). In
addition, such programs should be delivered in a balanced manner that
integrates the teaching of word-level skills and reading comprehension
strategies within meaningful opportunities for reading (Pressley, 2006;
Taylor, Pearson, Garcia, Stahl, & Bauer, 2006). Other authors (e.g.,
Guthrie & Humenick, 2004; Taboada, Tonks, Wigfield, & Guthrie,
2009) have argued that programs in which motivation, metacognitive
knowledge and strategy use are emphasised can enhance reading
achievement. Such programs provide the use of interesting texts, offer
pupils choice and opportunities for personal control during learning,
set knowledge goals and use activities that employ cooperative learning.
To conclude, this study showed that individual differences in
primary school pupils' reading comprehension can be explained by
word identification, and to a lesser extent metacognitive and
motivational variables. The creation and delivery of effective programs
that promote successful reading comprehension for these students is
essential.
References
Boekaerts, M.M., & Corno, L. (2005). Self-regulation in the
classroom: A perspective on assessment and intervention. Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 54(2), 199-231.
Burgon, J.R. (1988). Tests of Reading Comprehension (TORCH) pilot
study. Wellington, New Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational
Research. Retrieved from ERIC database. (ED326855).
Chapman, J.W., & Tunmer, W.E. (1993). Reading Self-Concept
Scale. Unpublished scale, Massey University, Educational Research and
Development Centre, Palmerston North, New Zealand.
Chapman, J.W., & Tunmer, W.E. (1995). Development of young
children's reading self concepts: An examination of emerging
subcomponents and their relationship with reading achievement. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 87, 154-167.
Chapman, J.W., Tunmer, W.E., & Prochnow, J.E. (2000). Early
reading-related skills and performance, reading self-concept, and the
development of academic self-concept: A longitudinal study. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 92(4), 703-708.
Deci, E.L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R.M. (2001). Extrinsic
rewards and intrinsic motivation in education: Reconsidered once again.
Review of Educational Research, 71, 1-27.
Dignath, C., & Buttner, G. (2008). Components of fostering
self-regulated learning among students: A meta-analysis on interventions
studies at primary and secondary school level. Metacognition &
Learning, 3, 231-264.
Elley, W.B., & Reid, N. (1969). Progressive Achievement Tests:
Reading comprehension. Wellington, New Zealand: New Zealand Council for
Educational Research.
Goswami, U. (2008). Reading, complexity and the brain. Literacy,
42(2), 67-74. Guthrie, J.T., & Humenick, N.M. (2004). Motivating
students to read: Evidence for classroom practices that increase reading
motivation and achievement. In P.
McCardle & V. Chhabra (Eds.), The voice of evidence in reading
research (pp. 329-354). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Guthrie, J.T., & Wigfield, A. (1999). How motivation fits into
the science of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 199-205.
Guthrie, J.T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and motivation
in reading. In M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, P.D. Pearson, & R. Barr
(Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 111, pp. 403-422). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800
meta-analyses relating to achievement. Abingdon, United Kingdom:
Routledge.
Jacobs, J.E., & Paris, S.G. (1987). Children's
metacognition about reading: Issues in definition, measurement and
instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22, 225-278.
Jenkins, J.R., Fuchs, L.S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., &
Deno, S.L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading
comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology,
95(4), 719-729.
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study
of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 80(4), 437-447.
Logan, S., & Johnston, R. (2010). Investigating gender
differences in reading. Educational Review, 62, 175-187.
Marsh, H.W. (1990). Influences of internal and external frames of
reference on the formation of maths and English self-concepts. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 82, 107-116.
Marx, H. (1998). Knuspels Leseaufgaben, Forms A & B. Gottingen,
Germany: Hogrefe.
McLain, K.V.M., Gridley, B.E., & McIntosh, D. (1991). Value of
a scale used to measure metacognitive reading processes. Journal of
Educational Research, 85, 81-87.
Miller, S.D., & Faircloth, B.S. (2009). Motivation and reading
comprehension. In S.E. Israel & G.G. Duffy (Eds.), Handbook of
research on reading comprehension (pp. 307-322).
Ming Chui, M., & McBride-Chang, C. (2006). Gender, context and
reading: A comparison of students in 43 countries. Scientific Studies of
Reading, 10(4), 331-362.
Moller, J., & Koller, O. (2004). Die Genese akademischer
Selbstkonzepte: Effekte dimensionaler und sozialer Vergleiche [On the
development of academic self-concepts: The impact of social and
dimensional comparisons.] Psychologische Rundschau, 55, 19-27.
Mossenson, L., Hill, P., & Masters, G. (1995). Tests of Reading
Comprehension (TORCH). Melbourne, VIC: Australian Council for
Educational Research.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000).
Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An
evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on
reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication
No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P.E (2003). The dissociation
of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills.
Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443-468.
Paris, S.G., Lipson, M.Y., & Wixson, K.K. (1994). Becoming a
strategic reader. In R.B. Ruddell, M.R. Ruddell, & H. Singer (Eds.),
Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 788-811). Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.
Paris, S.G., & Paris, A.H. (2001). Classroom applications of
research on self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 36(2),
89-101.
Pekrun, R. & Zirngibl, A.C. (2004). Schulermerkmale in
Mathematik. [Student characteristics in mathematics]. In PISA-Konsortium
Deutschland (Hrsg.): PISA 2003. Der Bildungsstand der Jugendlichen in
Deutschland--Ergebnisse des zweiten internationalen Vergleichs (Seiten
191-210) [PISA 2003. The level of education of young people in
Germany--Results of the second international comparison (pp. 191-210).
Munster: Waxmann.
Pintrich, P.R., & Schrauben, B. (1992). Students'
motivational beliefs and their cognitive engagement in academic tasks.
In D. Schunk & J. Meece (Eds.), Student-perceptions: Causes and
consequences (pp. 149-183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pressley, M. (2002). Comprehension strategies instruction: A
turn-of-the-century status report. In C.C. Block, & M. Pressley
(Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research based best practices (pp.
11-27). New York: Guilford.
Pressley, M. (2006). Reading instruction that works (3rd ed.). New
York: Guilford.
Pressley, M., & Harris, K.H. (2006). Cognitive strategy
instruction: From basic research to classroom instruction. In P.A.
Alexander, & P.H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology
(2nd ed., pp. 265-286). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Puustinen, M., & Pulkkinen, L. (2001). Models of self-regulated
learning: A review. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 45(3),
269-286.
Renninger, K.A., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (1992). The role of
interest in learning and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rider, N., & Colmar, S. (2006). Reading achievement and reading
self-concept in Year 3 students. In P.L. Jeffery (Ed.), AARE Education
Research. Creative Dissent: Constructive Solutions (pp. 1-Paper No).
Parramatta, 27 Nov-1 Dec 2005.
Roeschl-Heils, A., Schneider, W., & van Kraayenoord, C.E.
(2003). Reading metacognition and motivation: A follow-up study of
German students in Grades 7 and 8. European Journal of Psychology of
Education, 18(1), 75-86.
Samuels, S.J. (2006). Reading fluency: Its past, present, and
future. In T. Rasinski, C. Blachowicz, & K. Lems (Eds.), Fluency
instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 7-20). New York:
Guilford.
Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning and motivation.
Educational Psychologist, 26, 299-323.
Schlagmuller, M., Vise, M., & Schneider, W. (2001). Zur
Erfassung des Gedachtniswissens bei Grundschulkindern:
Konstruktionsprinzipien und empirische Bewahrung der Wurzburger
Testbatterie zum deklarativen Metagedachtnis. [The acquisition of
primary school children's knowledge about memory: Design principles
and empirical verification of the Wurzburg Test Battery of Declarative
Metamemory]. Zeitschrift fur Entwicklungspsychologie und Padagogische
Psychologie, 33(1), 91-102.
Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development
between two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Snow, C.E., Burns, M.S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998).
Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.
Sweet, A.P., & Snow, C.E. (2003). Reading for comprehension. In
A.P. Snow C.E. Snow (Eds.), Rethinking reading comprehension (pp. 1-11).
New York: Guilford Press.
Taboada, A., Tonks, S.M., Wigfield, A., & Guthrie, J.T. (2009).
Effects of motivational and cognitive variables on reading
comprehension. Reading and Writing, 22, 85-106.
Taylor, B.M., Pearson, P.D., Garcia, G.E., Stahl, K., & Bauer,
E. (2006). Improving students' reading comprehension. In K. Stahl
(Ed.), Seeking understanding in how to teach reading: Selected works by
Steven Stahl (pp. 303-315). New York: Guilford.
Torgesen, J.K. (2002). The prevention of reading difficulties.
Journal of School Psychology, 40, 7-26.
Tunmer, W.E., & Chapman, J.W. (2002). The relation of beginning
readers' reported word identification strategies to reading
achievement, reading-related skills, and academic self-perceptions.
Reading and Writing: In Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 341-358.
van Kraayenoord, C.E. (1996). The Interest in Reading Scale.
Unpublished scale, The University of Queensland, Fred and Eleanor
Schonell Special Education Research Centre, Brisbane, Australia.
van Kraayenoord, C.E., & Schneider, W. (1999). Reading
achievement, metacognition, reading self-concept and interest: A study
of German students in Grades 3 and 4. European Journal of Psychology of
Education, 14, 305-324.
Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M., & Tanzman, M.S. (1994).
Component of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing
word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In
G.R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning
disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 279-332).
Philadelphia, PA: Brookes Publishing Co.
Vise, M., Schlagmuller, M., & Schneider, W. (1998). Wurzburg
Metamemory Test (rev. ed.). Wurzburg, Germany: Universitat Wurzburg,
Institut fur Psychologie.
Wang, J.H.Y., & Guthrie, J.T. (2004). Modeling the effects of
intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, amount of reading, and past
reading achievement on text comprehension between U.S. and Chinese
students. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 162-186.
Wigfield, A. (1997). Reading motivation: A domain-specific approach
to motivation. Educational Psychologist, 32, 59-68.
Winne, P.H. (2005). A perspective on state-of-the-art research on
self-regulated learning. Instructional Science, 33(5/6), 559-565.
Woodcock, R.W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests--Revised,
Form H. Examiner's Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Service.
Woolley, G.E. (2007). A comprehension intervention for children
with reading comprehension difficulties. Australian Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 12(1), 43-50.
Christina E. van Kraayenoord, (1) Andrea Beinicke, Matthias
Schlagmuller & Wolfgang Schneider (2)
(1) The University of Queensland, (2) University of Wurzburg,
Germany
Table 1. Mean ages (and standard deviations) of sample
in years and months by gender
Gender Grade 3 Grade 4
Males 9.14 (0.41) 10.08 (0.36)
N = 30 N = 34
Females 9.16 (0.45) 10.07 (0.40)
N = 31 N = 44
Table 2. Results of t-tests
[M.sub.male] [SD.sub.male] [M.sub.female]
READCOM -0.34 1.95 0.29
TEACHJUDGE -0.14 1.08 0.12
TORCH -0.20 1.05 0.17
WORDID -0.13 1.15 0.11
META -0.17 1.70 0.14
AWARE -0.03 1.00 0.02
METAMEM -0.14 1.03 0.12
SELFCON -0.23 1.05 0.19
INTEREST -0.03 1.11 0.02
[SD.sub.female] t p d
READCOM 1.67 -2.06 .04 0.35
TEACHJUDGE 0.91 -1.57 .12 0.27
TORCH 0.92 -2.21 .03 0.38
WORDID 0.83 -1.35 .18 0.23
META 1.58 -1.10 .27 0.19
AWARE 1.00 -0.31 .76 0.05
METAMEM 0.96 -1.51 .13 0.26
SELFCON 0.91 -2.53 .01 0.43
INTEREST 0.89 -0.31 .76 0.05
Table 3. Correlations
1 2 3 4
1 READCOM 1
2 TEACHJUDGE 92 ** 1
3 TORCH 92 ** .69 ** 1
4 WORDID .76 ** .73 ** .66 ** 1
5 METAMEM .46 ** .46 ** .39 ** .30 **
6 AWARE .34 ** .34 ** .28 ** 27 **
7 SELFCON .48 ** .42 ** .47 ** .41 **
8 INTEREST 0.12 0.08 0.14 .20 *
5 6 7 8
1 READCOM
2 TEACHJUDGE
3 TORCH
4 WORDID
5 METAMEM 1
6 AWARE .35 ** 1
7 SELFCON .21 * .19 * 1
8 INTEREST -0.03 .19 * .36 ** 1
* p < .05; ** p < .01
Note. These correlations refer to the means of the z-scores.
Table 4. Stepwise multiple regression
Variable B SE B P [sr.sup.2]
(incremental)
Step 1
Word identification 0.70 0.05 .76 *** .58 ***
Step 2
Word identification 0.62 0.05 .67 *** .06 ***
Metacognitive knowledge 0.14 0.03 .26 ***
Step 3
Word identification 0.56 0.05 .61 ***
Metacognitive knowledge 0.13 0.03 .24 *** .02 ***
Reading self-concept 0.16 0.05 .17 ***
[R.sup.2] = .66 ***, R = .81 ***.
** p < .01, *** p < .001.
Note.
R = Multiple correlation
[R.sup.2] = Multiple correlation squared