Beyond rubrics: using functional language analysis to evaluate student writing.
Fang, Zhihui ; Wang, Zhijun
Writing is a significant language and literacy skill that is
essential to students' academic success in school. Despite its
importance, many students struggle with academic writing. In the United
States, for example, roughly threequarters of 4th, 8th, and 12th graders
performed below the proficiency level in a recent National Assessment of
Educational Progress (Persky, Daane & Jin, 2003). In New Zealand,
the Ministry of Education (2006) reported that many secondary students
write no better than their primary school counterparts. In Australia,
concern about students' writing performance has also mounted, as is
evident in the recent inclusion of writing as part of the national
assessment of literacy and numeracy (MCEETYA, 2008). In recognition of
this situation, leading scholars and organisations (e.g., Beard, Myhill,
Riley & Nystrand, 2009; Christie & Derewianka, 2008; Graham
& Perin, 2005; National Commission on Writing, 2003) have called for
greater attention to the subject of writing in school and proposed
ambitious agendas and new pedagogical models for improving
students' written communication skills. It is suggested that a
revolution of sort in the teaching and learning of writing is needed in
order to develop more effective and proficient writers.
What's wrong with rubrics?
An area of writing instruction that is in need of reforming is
classroom assessment. The main goal of any classroom assessment should
be to inform instruction (IRA & NCTE, 2009). In the case of writing,
classroom assessment should help teachers not only identify
students' levels of performance but, more importantly, provide
insights into students' strengths and needs for the purpose of
planning instruction and remediation. Popular assessment techniques,
such as rubrics, fall short of these goals, however. Widely used in the
classroom, the rubric is regarded by many as an exact, efficient, and
objective tool for evaluating student writing. In rubrics-based writing
assessment, teachers develop several score levels, such as 1 to 6, with
a description of what is expected at each level in terms of mechanics,
content, organisation, vocabulary, and grammar. A tool for evaluating
student writing highly recommended for and widely used by school
teachers in the United States is the six traits writing rubric. Figure 1
describes what an exemplary piece of writing (Level 6) should look like
under the six-traits writing rubric.
Figure 1: six-traits writing rubric: Level 6
Ideas and content: * Exceptionally clear, focused, engaging
supporting detail with relevant, strong
Organisation: * Effectively organised in logical and
creative manner
* Creative and engaging introduction
and conclusion
Voice: * Expressive, engaging, sincere;
* Strong sense of audience;
* Shows emotion, humour, honesty,
suspense or life.
Word choice: * Precise, carefully chosen;
* Strong, fresh, vivid images
Sentence fluency: * High degree of craftsmanship
* Effective variation in sentence patterns
Conventions: * Exceptionally strong control of
standard conventions of writing
from http://school.discoveryeducation.com/schrockguide/assess.html
(Accessed on 20 February 2010
One problem with this rubric is that it is neither exact nor
objective. It does not, for example, give teachers any hint of what it
is that makes a piece of writing 'exceptionally clear, focused,
engaging' and 'effectively organised'. Nor does it
specify what 'high degree of craftsmanship' entails. Moreover,
the rubric does not elaborate on what it means to be
'creative', 'effective', 'engaging',
'expressive', 'strong', and 'fresh'.
Teachers are left wondering what textual evidence to look for when
evaluating a text's content, organisation, and language use. Often,
they have to rely on their own intuition and discursive knowledge in
making judgment calls. This can be especially troubling, because
teachers are rarely trained to understand the discursive features of
writing (Louden, Rohl, Gore, Greaves, Mcintosh, Wright, Siemon &
House, 2005; Schleppegrell, 2004) and they often feel ill equipped to
evaluate student writing in linguistic terms (Harper & Rennie, 2009;
Hammond & MackenHorarik, 2001). In fact, many teachers usually do no
more than point to errors in the more obvious aspects of writing, such
as spelling, capitalisation, punctuation, subject-verb agreement, tense,
and idiomatic expressions (Christie & Derewianka, 2008; Martin,
1996; Schleppegrell, 2004).
Another problem with the rubric is that it focuses on grammatical
forms with little attention to their functions. For example, the rubric
calls for variation in sentence patterns, but fails to specify which
kind of variation is appropriate for which type of text. While syntactic
variety and complexity for its own sake may enhance a text's
status, it does little to improve its discursive quality and
functionality (Myhill, 2008). Researchers (e.g., Martin, 1989;
Schleppegrell, 2004) have shown that different genres and registers draw
on different constellations of lexicogrammatical features that enable a
text to mean what it does in a particular context. For example, factual
genres in the academic context, such as reports and explanations, tend
to use sentences that are grammatically simpler but lexically dense and
that contain abstract nouns and expanded noun groups, whereas personal
genres such as recounts and narratives often use sentences that are
grammatically more complex, stringing together coordinate and
subordinate clauses. These differences are a reflection of the
fundamental differences in the ways different disciplinary experts read,
write, and think.
The rubric's indifference to genre- and register-specific
requirements is also found in its undue emphasis on personal involvement
with the topic (e.g., expressive, emotion, humour, honesty, suspense,
vivid images, creative, and engaging introduction and conclusion). Some
researchers (see, for example, Biber, Johansson, Leech, Conrad &
Finegan, 1994; Schleppegrell, 2004) have pointed out that unlike
narrative genres where personal involvement and voice are valued,
academic texts, particularly those of factual genres, often feature a
more objective, abstract, and authoritative style of writing that
encourages personal detachment. Martin (1996, 2002) further demonstrated
that academic texts are typically organised in 'waves of
abstraction' that make the introduction and conclusion paragraphs
as well as the beginning and ending sentences of each paragraph highly
nominalised and abstract. This pattern of text organisation, Martin
suggested, facilitates information flow and development of argument.
Therefore, blind adherence to certain grammatical forms or styles
without regard for their functions can result in texts that have little
rhetorical power and unexpected communicative effects.
It is clear that the rubrics-based assessment has its limitations
and problems (see also Wilson, 2006 for a different critique of
rubrics). On one hand, rubrics encourage the teacher to pigeonhole
individual students into a certain proficiency level; on the other hand,
they fail to provide specific, concrete, genre/register-sensitive
criteria that will enable the teacher to render a more objective and
valid judgment. As such, rubrics give the teacher little insights into
what exactly makes a text more or less effective/valued and are
marginally useful for informing writing instruction.
In this paper, we propose an alternative approach to rubrics-based
classroom assessment. The approach, called functional language analysis
(Fang & Schleppegrell, 2008, 2010), offers a set of analytical tools
that enables teachers to focus systematically on the language choices
students have made in their writing and evaluate whether these choices
are appropriate for the particular task at hand and effective for
presenting information, creating discursive flow, and infusing
perspectives. The approach recognises that language is the primary
medium through which student writing is communicated and assessed in
school. It foregrounds the important role of teachers in understanding
the discursive features of writing and in using that knowledge to inform
writing assessment and instruction. In the remainder of the paper, we
discuss the theoretical basis of functional language analysis and
illustrate the power of the approach in evaluating student writing and
in guiding writing instruction.
Functional language analysis: a description
Functional language analysis is grounded in systemic functional
linguistics, or SFL (Halliday & Matthiessen, 2004). According to
SFL, language is a semiotic resource for making meaning, and the kinds
of meaning made are influenced by the social and cultural context in
which they are exchanged. As an interlocking system of grammatical
choices, language enables speakers and writers to make different kinds
of meaning for different purposes and contexts. SFL also provides a
metalanguage, a language for talking about language, that makes visible
the varied ways language constructs texts in different genres and
registers. It offers an array of analytical tools for evaluating texts
and their effectiveness in meaning making.
From a SFL perspective, every use of language, whether spoken or
written, involves saying something about the world (the experiential
meaning), connecting what is said by some kind of logic (the logical
meaning), enacting a social relationship of some kind (the interpersonal
meaning), and presenting a message in a coherent way (the textual
meaning). The experiential meaning refers to meaning about what human
experience is represented in language; it is realised through the
grammatical system of transitivity. The transitivity system construes
the world of human experience into a manageable set of process types,
typically realised by verbal groups of various kinds, such as doing
(e.g., jump, grow), sensing (e.g., think, believe), relating (e.g., be,
have), and saying (e.g., talk, say). These processes are often
accompanied by participants, typically realised in noun groups, and
circumstances, typically realised in adverbial groups or prepositional
phrases.
The logical meaning refers to meaning about logical links and
dependency relationships among clauses; it is typically realised through
logical connectives of various types (e.g., moreover, for example,
because, if, however) and projecting verbs (e.g., say, state, know,
believe). SFL recognises four major clause types--main clause,
hypotactic clause, paratactic clause, and embedded clause (Schleppegrell
& Colombi, 1997). The main clause is the only clause in a simple
sentence, the dominant clause in a hypotactic clause complex, or the
initiating clause in a paratactic sequence. Hypotactic clauses include
adverbial clauses (e.g., those introduced by conjunctions such as if,
when, because, and however), clauses projected through verbs of saying
or thinking (e.g., think, know, say), and non-restrictive relative
clauses (e.g., He found the stolen bag, which was later returned to its
owner.). They are dependent on but not a part of another clause.
Paratactic clauses are connected to the main clause through either mere
juxtaposition (as in direct quotations) or the use of coordinating
conjunctions (e.g., and, or). An embedded clause is both dependent on
and part of another clause in which it is embedded (e.g., He found the
bag that had been reportedly stolen.). These clauses can be combined in
many different ways, allowing language users to construe different
logical links (e.g., elaborating, enhancing, extending) and dependency
relationships (e.g., coordinate, subordinate) between meanings.
The textual meaning refers to meaning about how language users
organise their intended messages so that these messages are
'cohesive, coherent and well-crafted' (Christie &
Derewianka, 2008, p. 24); it is realised through the Theme/Rheme system
of language as well as patterns of cohesion. The Theme/Rheme system
describes the structural configurations by which the clause is organised
as a message. Clauses in English text typically begin with something
that is familiar or already known to the reader and then moves on to
present something new. The part of the clause that is the point of
departure for the message is called Theme and the rest of the clause is
called Rheme. Linguistic devices such as reference (e.g., pronouns,
demonstratives), synonyms and antonyms, and conjunctions also enable
language users to create texts that are internally cohesive and make
sense.
The interpersonal meaning refers to meaning about people's
relationship with and attitudes toward each other; it is realised
through mood, modality, and other appraisal resources of language (e.g.,
attitudinal lexis). The mood system allows language users to make
statements (normally expressed in declarative clauses), ask questions
(normally expressed in interrogative clauses), and issue commands
(normally expressed in imperative clauses). Modality and other appraisal
resources, on the other hand, enable language users to (a) talk about
possibility, certainty, usuality, normality, seriousness, necessity,
obligation, etc.; (b) express and amplify their attitudes and feelings
towards people, ideas, or things; and (c) enact a particular kind of
relationship with the audience (Martin & Rose, 2003; Martin &
White, 2005).
Every text (and each clause in the text) simultaneously encodes
these four strands of meaning, and it is the grammatical systems of
language that enable the text (and clause) to mean what it does. Given
the systematic relationship between meaning and grammar, a functional
analysis of the language patterns in a text can reveal how meaning is
constructed in the text. For example, if teachers want to find out about
the content of a text, which concerns the experiential meaning, they can
analyse the transitivity patterns in the text. If teachers are
interested in evaluating the way a text is organised, which involves
both textual and logical meanings, they can examine the Theme/Rheme
structure and cohesion patterns, as well as clause types and clause
combining strategies, in the text. If teachers are concerned with the
style of writing (e.g., how the author interacts with the reader or the
author's perspective in the text), which is part of the
interpersonal meaning, they can analyse mood, modality, word choices,
and other appraisal resources. Table 1 shows the kinds of functional
language analysis strategies that are appropriate for evaluating the
three key areas of writing--content, organisation, and style--that are
often the focus of classroom writing assessment and instruction.
Applying functional language analysis in writing assessment
In this section, we illustrate the power of functional language
analysis in evaluating student writing. Presented in Table 2 are two
texts, both belonging to the report genre, or more specifically,
descriptive report. Text 1 (134 words) is composed by a ninth grader in
response to an explicit request by his language arts teacher to assume
the role of a scientist author and write a formal report about one of
his most familiar and favorite animals for an educational audience. Text
2 (129 words), excerpted from a U.S. middle school science textbook
(Science Voyages, 2000), is a report about fish and presumably written
by a science education expert.
Descriptive report is one of the major genres of schooling (Martin,
1989; Schleppegrell, 2004; Veel, 1997). It describes attributes,
properties, behaviors, etc. of a single class or entity in a system of
things. While the exact form (or textual realisation) of the genre can
vary from one instance to another and change over time, it has
nonetheless evolved some general language patterns that remain fairly
stable across instances and time, making it distinct from other genres.
According to Martin (1989), for example, a report usually starts with
general classification, followed by successive elements contributing to
a description, such as types, parts and their functions, qualities,
uses, habits, and so on. Some of the grammatical features of the genre
include generic participants, clauses with linking verbs (e.g., be,
have), verbs in present tense (are, stabilise), technical vocabulary
(e.g., ectotherms, reptiles), nominalisations (e.g., eruption,
diversity), and expanded noun groups with embedded clauses (e.g., fleshy
filaments that are filled with tiny blood vessels) and other modifiers
(e.g., prepositional phrases). These features are functional for science
reporting, as they enable the author to talk about a class rather than a
specific individual (via generic participants); to identify a class of
things that is being reported on and attribute various characteristics
to it (via linking clauses); to situate a report as objective and
universal rather than particularistic (via verbs in present tense); to
construct specialised knowledge (via technical vocabulary); to coin
technical terms, summarise data, distill information, and create text
flow (via nominalisations); and to pack information (via expanded noun
groups) (Schleppegrell, 1998). Understanding these features and their
functions in meaning making can help teachers better evaluate the
quality of students' reports and design instruction that increases
students' awareness and use of these features in their report
writing.
Content
Let us first examine the content of the two sample reports using
functional language analysis. This can be done at the clause level by
analysing the transitivity patterns in these texts. Within the
transitivity system, analysis can be done on the processes and
accompanying participants and circumstances. We will focus on processes
and participants in our analysis, as a substantial amount of content in
reports is conveyed through these two grammatical elements.
Each process type in the transitivity system construes a distinct
kind of human experience, and in science reports the kind of experience
construed is typically that of classifying, categorising, and
attributing, which calls for the use of relating processes, typically
realised in linking verbs such as be and have. Other process types, such
as doing processes, are used to augment the generalisation and
classification statements made in the relating processes. Table 3 shows
the number and percentage (in parenthesis) of each process type across
the two texts. In Text 1, the doing processes figure prominently, with
18 out of 25 clauses (72%) used to describe when crocodiles live, where
they live, what they eat, where they swim and play, where they are born,
how they watch their babies, and so on. These doing processes are
interjected with 4 (16%) relating processes, which describe the
attributes of crocodiles (i.e., have good vision, have big mouth, have
rough skin, are reptiles), 1 (4%) sensing process that describes what
crocodiles like (i.e., like alley water), and 2 (8%) process complexes
that combine sensing with doing (i.e., like to eat in the sun, like to
eat outside of water). However, it is not clear how these processes
complement and reinforce each other in the presentation of content.
Unlike Text 1, Text 2 contains mostly relating processes (realised
in linking verbs be and have), with 56% (9 out of 16) of the clauses
used to classify, define, and characterise fish (e.g., fish are
ectotherms; gills are fleshy filaments ...; fins are fanlike structures
...; scales are hard, thin overlapping plates...). The doing process
clauses (e.g., pumps, passes, picks up, is released, steer and move),
which constitute 44% of the total clauses in the text, complement the
relating process clauses by providing further information about how the
different body parts of fish (e.g., gills, fins, scales) work.
In science reporting, students are expected to present specialised
content that is technical, dense, and abstract. A major carrier of this
content is the grammatical participant, typically realised in noun
groups of varying complexities. Science reports typically draw on
technical nouns (mitosis, arthropod) to create scientific taxonomies,
nominalisations (e.g., deforestation, frequency) to make generalisations
and distill information, and expanded noun groups (e.g., well-defined
intracellular bodies that perform specific functions for the cell) to
pack a large amount of information. An examination of the noun groups in
the two sample reports reveals the following patterns (see Table 4).
Text 1 uses 1 technical noun (reptiles), 1 nominalisation (vision), 22
pronouns (e.g., I, they), and 0 expanded noun groups. Text 2, on the
other hand, has 9 different technical nouns (e.g., ectotherms, gills,
filaments, oxygen, vessels, carbon dioxide, fins, scales, plates), 1
nominalisation (those protective plates in clause #16 is used to
summarise hard, thin, overlapping plates that cover the skin in clause
#15), 2 pronouns (it, they), and 9 expanded noun groups (e.g.,
ectotherms that live in water and use gills to get oxygen; fleshy
filaments that are filled with tiny blood vessels; fanlike structures
used for steering, balancing, and moving; those on the top and bottom;
hard, thin, overlapping plates that cover the skin). These data suggest
that the content presented in Text 2 is more technical and dense than
that conveyed in Text 1. Although the two texts are roughly equal in
length (134 words vs. 129 words), Text 2 uses considerably more
technical nouns and expanded noun groups, which carry substantial
ideational content, and sharply fewer pronouns, which carry little
ideational content, than does Text 1.
The analysis of processes and participants shows a pronounced
difference in the nature of content presented in the two sample reports.
Text 1 construes commonsense knowledge through the use of doing verbs
and noun groups with simple structures (e.g., pronouns; nouns without
modifiers; nouns with just an article, determiner, demonstrative, and/or
adjective). The report has the flavor of a story, where the emphasis is
on presenting a sequence of actions (e.g., swim, play, watch, carry).
Text 2, on the other hand, construes technical knowledge through the use
of technical nouns and expanded noun groups with embedded clauses and
prepositional phrases, as well as linking verbs that connect these noun
groups. It is more characteristic of a science report, where the
emphasis is on classifying, defining, and characterising a class of
organisms or objects in the natural world.
Organisation
Let's now turn to the organisation of the two reports. There
are many elements that contribute to a text's organisation. These
include Theme/Rheme structuring, cohesion (e.g., co-reference,
co-classification, conjunction), and clause combining strategies. We
will focus on the patterns of Thematic progression in these texts. To
this end, we will first identify the kinds of Themes used and then track
how these Themes develop through text. Consistent with Halliday and
Matthiessen (2004, p. 64), we identify the part of a clause that serves
as the point of departure (or the orientation) of the message as Theme
and the remainder of the message (i.e., the part in which the Theme is
developed) as Rheme. Different kinds of Themes indicate different
approaches to text organisation (Schleppegrell, 2004). Academic,
particularly scientific, texts typically thematise noun groups that are
lexicalised, abstract, and dense; whereas everyday texts typically
thematise items that are pronominalised. Table 5 lists the clause Themes
used in the two texts.
As the table shows, the two reports differ noticeably in their
choice of Themes. Text 1 thematises mostly 'crocodiles' and
its pronominal reference 'they'. The Themes in Text 2 are much
more varied. It thematises 'fish' and its various body parts
(e.g., gills, heart, fins, scales). In terms of Thematic progression, it
is clear from Table 5 that Text 1 features a reiterating pattern because
the Theme 'crocodiles' and its reference 'they' are
repeated in successive clauses. This reiterating pattern of Thematic
progression, while enabling the author to sustain a focus on crocodiles,
results in a random listing of statements without any clear indication
of how one clause relates to another. This can make the text not only
confusing but also monotonous to read. Text 2, on the other hand, adopts
both zig-zagging and reiterating patterns of Thematic progression (see
Figure 2), which is typical of the organisation patterns found in
science reports (Fang & Schleppegrell, 2008) and is an effective way
for the author to accumulate information and at the same time create
discursive flow. Specifically, the Rhemes in clauses #1 (gills), # 2
(tiny blood vessels), # 3 (blood), # 5 (oxygen), # 7 (fins), # 9
(paired), # 14 (scales), and # 15 (hard, thin, overlapping plates that
cover the skin) are picked up to become, respectively, the Themes in
clauses # 2 (gills), # 3 (the heart), # 4 (blood, which is repeated in
clause #5), # 6 (carbon dioxide), # 8 (fins, which is repeated in clause
#9), # 10 (those on the top and bottom) and # 11 (those on the side,
which is repeated in clause #12), # 15 (scales), and # 16 (these
protective plates). This way of structuring clause Themes and Rhemes
allows the topic to be logically developed and makes the text more
cohesive and interesting to read.
The analysis demonstrates that differences in Theme choices and
Thematic patternings result in qualitative differences in the
organisation of the two sample reports. Text 1 aggregates a random list
of statements about the topic (crocodile) without a clear focus,
resulting in what Bereiter and Scardamalia (1987) called 'knowledge
telling' or what Pea and Kurland (1987) dubbed as 'a memory
dump'. The author, who is seemingly knowledgeable about crocodiles,
simply translated his knowledge into words without making serious
efforts to craft the presentation of this knowledge. Text 2, on the
other hand, uses zig-zagging and reiterating patterns of Thematic
progression to facilitate presentation of content and development of
information flow. It shows evidence of 'knowledge
transforming' (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987) and rhetorical
crafting.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Style
Many factors contribute to the shaping of textual style. These
include the use of mood and modality, as well as word choices. Because
we are interested in examining how the author establishes the
'authorial self' (Christie & Derewianka, 2008, p. 15) in
his/her interaction with the reader in the formal, academic context of
schooling, we will focus on word choices in our analysis. Specifically,
we will look at the choice of noun groups and other vocabulary items in
the sample reports. Research has suggested that nouns are a sensitive
indicator of textual style (Biber, Johansson, Leech, Conrad &
Finegan, 1994; Fang, Schleppegrell & Cox, 2006; Quirk, Greenbaum,
Leech & Svartvik, 1985). Texts that are written for different
purposes and contexts often use nouns in distinct ways. In informal
registers, for example, simple nouns are often used to name things and
pronouns to establish endophoric (within-text) or exophoric
(outside-text) references; whereas in more formal registers, nouns of
varying complexities--particularly technical nouns, abstract nouns, and
expanded noun groups--are often used to construe technicality,
generalisation, agency, and density.
In the academic context, students are often expected to write in a
style featuring technicality, density, and generalisation (Christie
& Derewianka, 2008; Schleppegrell, 2004). This is especially true of
science reports, which often adopt a technical, dense, abstract,
authoritative, and formal style. Nouns are one of the key grammatical
resources that enable the author to meet this expectation, as technical
nouns create technical taxonomies, nominalisations synthesise
information into abstract entities, and expanded noun groups pack a
large quantity of information. Based on the analysis of noun groups
presented earlier, we have noted that Text 2 draws heavily on technical
nouns and expanded noun groups, which are connected by linking verbs be
and have, to construct specialised knowledge and construe a more static
world full of technical or virtual entities; whereas Text 1 relies
primarily on simple nouns and pronouns, which work with action verbs to
construct commonsense knowledge and construe a dynamic world full of
action. In short, the analysis of noun groups suggests that Text 2
assumes a more technical, dense, and formal style of writing than does
Text 1.
The stylistic difference between the two sample reports is also
evident in other word choices. Text 1 uses an intensifier
'really' before adjectives (e.g., really wide, really tight),
a conjunction with vague meaning (so), colloquial expressions (get
scrambled, the babies), and a personal pronoun (I). These language
choices betray an informal, interactive style that is more typical of
spontaneous speech. Text 2, on the other hand, assumes a more detached,
formal stance of writing that enables the author to present content in a
more objective and authoritative manner. This finding is further
supported by the analysis of Theme choices earlier (see Table 5), which
indicates that Text 1 uses Themes that are more typical of spontaneous
speech (see, for example, Eggins, 2004) and Text 2 uses Themes that are
more characteristic of academic registers (see, for example,
Schleppegrell, 2004).
A summary measure of whether a text is written in a more or less
formal/ academic style is lexical density. The index measures the degree
of formality in a text and can be calculated by dividing the number of
content-carrying words (e.g., nouns, verbs, adjectives, some adverbs)
over the number of nonembedded clauses in a text (Halliday, 1985). The
higher the index, the more formal the text. The lexical density for Text
1 is 2.44, whereas that for Text 2 is 4.25. These indices corroborate
the findings from our analysis of word choices above and are consistent
with what has been found to be typical of informal everyday speech and
the more formal, academic writing (Halliday, 1985; Christie &
Derewianka, 2008).
Summary
Our linguistic analyses reveal significant differences in the
quality of the two sample reports. Text 1 contains mostly doing
processes (constructed in action verbs) and generic participants
(constructed in simple nouns). It presents a random listing of what
crocodiles do with little evidence of rhetorical crafting. Moreover, the
text uses colloquial vocabulary and a repeating pattern of Thematic
progression, both typical of everyday conversational language. These
pieces of evidence suggest that the author of Text 1 fails to meet the
linguistic requirements expected (implicitly or explicitly) of him for
the writing task. Text 2, on the other hand, can be considered an
exemplary science report. It focuses on classifying, defining, and
characterising fish. Information in the text is generalised and then
elaborated through judicious use of doing processes to support relating
processes. The zig-zagging and repeating patterns of Thematic
development facilitate the presentation of information and the
establishment of text flow. The use of technical nouns and expanded noun
groups contributes to a dense, formal style of writing that positions
the author as a content expert who presents information in an objective,
authoritative manner.
Discussion
Functional language analysis helps us identify the language
patterns related to content, organisation, and style, giving us valuable
insights into what makes a text successful/effective or less
successful/effective. While we have focused only on one or two aspects
of the grammar in each analysis of content, organisation, and style, it
is important to note that additional analysis often yield convergent or
complementary results, as language is an interlocking system of
grammatical choices, with choices in one grammatical system often
(albeit not always) affecting choices in other grammatical systems.
Through the analysis of processes and participants, we discover that
Text 2 construes more specialised, technical content that classifies
fish and attributes qualities to it; whereas Text 1 construes more
everyday, commonsense content that describes a sequence of actions
involving crocodiles. Through the analysis of Theme/ Rheme, noun groups,
and other word choices, we are able to conclude that Text 2 features a
formal, objective, academic style of writing; whereas Text 1 takes on a
more interactional, informal style of writing. Through the analysis of
Theme/Rheme and process types, we find that Text 2 has an organisation
structure that facilitates the presentation of information and the
development of discursive flow, whereas the information in Text 1 is
presented in a haphazard manner without any logical sequence. On the
basis of the linguistic evidence generated from these analyses, we are
then able to determine with some degree of certainty and objectivity the
overall quality of Text 1 and Text 2. This is unlike in the
rubrics-based assessment, where teachers often have to rely on their own
intuition without having to consciously search for concrete linguistic
clues to support their judgment in evaluating student writing.
Knowing the relative strengths and weaknesses of a text is a
prerequisite for designing effective instruction that accentuates
students' strengths and addresses their needs. Teachers using
rubrics-based assessment may intuitively judge Text 1 as nontechnical,
unorganised, unscientific, or nonacademic, but they often are not able
to pinpoint the exact sources of the failure (e.g., what is it about the
organisation of the text that are not 'focused' or
'effectively organised in logical and creative manner' and in
what way does the text demonstrate a lack of 'high degree of
craftsmanship') and have little to say about how the text can be
improved. Functional language analysis, on the other hand, provides
teachers tools for understanding how a text is more or less successful,
enabling them to identify linguistic issues that can be the focus of
subsequent instruction or remediation. For example, Text 1 shows that
the student author has a working knowledge of some basic features of the
report genre (e.g., the use of generic nouns and timeless verbs), but is
potentially unaware of other context-sensitive discursive features
(e.g., density, technicality, abstraction) that he is expected to
demonstrate in the writing. He also knows quite a few facts about
crocodiles, but these facts are constructed in language patterns that
approximate those of everyday spontaneous speech. Drawing on the
functional language analysis strategies illustrated above, teachers can
design lessons that give the student insights into (a) how the use of
linking verbs enables the author to develop general statements that
classify crocodiles as well as supporting details that attribute
qualities to the animal, (b) how noun groups of varying
complexities--particularly technical nouns, abstract nouns (i.e.,
nominalisations), and expanded noun groups--contribute to the
construction of technical, abstract, and dense content, (c) how Theme
choices impact the development of information flow and the organisation
of the text, and (d) how different language choices (e.g., verbs, nouns,
vocabulary, Themes) affect the style of writing. These lessons can be
made an integral part of the reading or writing workshop, where the
student engages in reading/ writing authentic texts and in comparing
what he has written with similar texts written by disciplinary experts.
Such targeted instruction is more likely to heighten the student's
awareness of the role language plays in fashioning a text, enabling him
to more successfully meet disciplinary expectations for language use in
school-based tasks such as writing a science report.
Of course, we are not suggesting that teachers do frequency counts
of every linguistic feature when evaluating student writing. Our intent
in quantifying the linguistic variables in this article is to illuminate
the power of functional language analysis in differentiating text
quality. What the functional language analysis approach calls for is
that teachers move beyond a rubric-ese mentality and focus instead on
equipping themselves with deep knowledge about how language works in
different genres and registers and then use that knowledge to guide them
in (a) identifying the most salient and relevant linguistic features for
evaluating a particular type of text, (b) generating systematic
linguistic evidence that supports whatever judgment they render on the
text, and (c) planning subsequent instruction or remediation that
addresses student needs. The point of our linguistic excursion is to
demonstrate the potential of explicit knowledge about language for a
better understanding of students' discursive competence so that
better pedagogical decisions can be made about how to improve student
writing.
Despite recent recommendations that teachers and teacher candidates
need to develop more explicit knowledge about language and linguistics
(AATE, 1999; Adger, Snow & Christian, 2002; DfEE, 2000), current
literacy textbooks and professional development materials are replete
with strategies that teachers can use to engage their students in the
processes of planning, drafting, composing, and sharing, but are short
on strategies that give teachers insights into what exactly makes a text
more or less effective/valued and that help them teach students to use
language in ways that are expected of them in school-based tasks. And
when language does become a focus during the writing process, attention
is often given to such issues as spelling, punctuation, capitalisation,
transition words, subject-verb agreement, verb tense, and idiomatic
expressions. Prevalent literacy pedagogies rarely acknowledge the
important role of teachers in both understanding the linguistic features
of different written genres and registers and in teaching students to be
aware of these features (Christie & Derewianka, 2008; Schleppegrell,
2004). Teacher education programs need to do a better job of preparing
teachers to teach writing, helping them develop a sound understanding of
how grammar can be used as a creative resource for making meaning,
rather than as rules and conventions to be feared or memorised and
mechanically applied in writing. When teachers are consciously aware of
the particular linguistic require ments of different writing tasks, they
will be better able to anticipate their students' needs, to
evaluate their writing, to provide instruction that improves their
writing skills and proficiencies, and to promote their academic success
(Christie, 2002; Macken-Horarik, 2006; Schleppegrell, 2004).
Conclusion
In academic writing, students are expected to display knowledge, be
authoritative, and structure text in certain ways (Schleppegrell, 2004).
These expectations call for the use of certain structural and
lexicogrammatical features that are different from those used in the
more commonsensical language of everyday life. Functional language
analysis enables us to examine whether or not students meet these
expectations in their writing. More importantly, it yields specific
information about the strengths and weaknesses of student writing,
making it possible for teachers to design effective intervention that
addresses student needs. The approach overcomes the often vague,
subjective nature of many popular assessment tools such as rubrics. It
allows teachers to make explicit the specific linguistic requirements
that are expected of students in school writing assignments. Teachers
need to develop a solid understanding of the linguistic features that
characterise different genres and registers in order to effectively use
functional language analysis for assessment and instructional purposes.
Recent scholarship in literacy education along the systemic functional
framework (e.g., Christie & Derewianka, 2008; Droga & Humphrey,
2003; Fang & Schleppegrell, 2008; Locke, 2010) has provided
valuable, teacher-friendly resources that can facilitate this
development. With conscious linguistic knowledge and understanding,
teachers will be better equipped to answer Myhill's (2009) recent
call to develop student writers as designers along the three
complementary and overlapping trajectories 'from speech patterns to
writing patterns, from declaration to elaboration, and from translation
to transformation' (p. 412).
References
Adger, C.T., Snow, C.E. & Christian, D. (Eds.) (2002). What
teachers need to know about language. Washington, DC: Center for Applied
Linguistics.
Australian Association for the Teaching of English (AATE) (1999).
AATE position papers: Professional development for English teachers.
Norwood, SA: AATE.
Beard, R., Myhill, D., Riley, J. & Nystrand, M. (Eds. (2009).
The Sage handbook of writing development. London: Sage.
Bereiter, C. & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of
written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S. & Finegan, E.
(1999). Longman grammar of spoken and written English. London: Pearson
Education Limited.
Christie, F. (2002). The development of abstraction in adolescence
in subject English. In M.J. Schleppegrell & M.C. Colombi (Eds.),
Developing advanced literacy in first and second languages: Meaning with
power (pp. 45-66). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Christie, F. & Derewianka, B. (2008). School discourse:
Learning to write across the years of schooling. London: Continuum.
Department for Education and Employment (DfEE) (2000). English: The
national curriculum for England. London: DfEE, QCA.
Droga, L. & Humphrey, S. (2003). Grammar and meaning: An
introduction for primary teachers. Berry, NSW: Target Texts
Eggins, S. (2004). An introduction to systemic functional
linguistics (2nd ed.). London: Continuum.
Fang, Z. & Schleppegrell, M.J. (2008). Reading in secondary
content areas: A language-based pedagogy. Ann Arbor, MI: The University
of Michigan Press.
Fang, Z. & Schleppegrell, M.J. (2010). Disciplinary literacies
across content areas: Supporting secondary reading through functional
language analysis. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 53(7),
587-597
Fang, Z., Schleppegrell, M. & Cox, B. (2006). Understanding the
language demands of schooling: Nouns in academic registers. Journal of
Literacy Research. 38 (3), 247-273.
Graham, S. & Perin, D. (2005). Writing Next: Effective
strategies to improve the writing of adolescents in middle and high
schools. New York: Carnegie Foundation of New York.
Halliday, M.A.K. (1985). Spoken and written language. Geelong,
Victoria: Deakin University Press.
Halliday, M. & Matthiessen, C. (2004). An introduction to
functional grammar (3rd ed.). London: Arnold.
Hammond, J. & Macken-Horarik, M. (2001). Teachers' voices,
teachers' practices: Insider perspectives on literacy education.
Australian Journal of Language and Literacy, 24(2), 112-132.
Harper, H. & Rennie, J. (2009). 'I had to go out and get
myself a book on grammar': A study of pre-service teachers'
knowledge about language. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy,
32(1), 22-37.
International Reading Association (IRA) & National Council of
Teachers of English (NCTE) (2009). Standards for the assessment of
reading and writing. Newark, DE: IRA & NCTE.
Locke, T. (Ed.) (2010). Beyond the grammar wars: A resource for
teachers and students on developing language knowledge in the
English/literacy classroom. New York; Routledge.
Louden, W., Rohl, M., Gore, J., Greaves, D., Mcintosh, A., Wright,
R., Siemon, D. & House, H. (2005). Prepared to teach: An
investigation into the preparation of teachers to teach literacy and
numeracy. Canberra, ACT: Australian Government Department of Education,
Science and Training.
Macken-Horarik, M. (2006). Knowledge through 'know-how':
Systemic functional grammatics and the symbolic reading. English
Teaching: Practice and Critique, 5(1), 1020121.
Martin, J.R. (1989). Factual writing: Exploring and challenging
social reality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Martin, J.R. (1996). Waves of abstraction: Organizing exposition.
In T. Miller (Ed.), Functional approaches to written text: Classroom
applications (pp. 87-104). Paris: TESOL France &: U.S. Information
Service.
Martin, J.R. (2002). Writing history: Construing time and value in
discourse of the past. In M.J.
Schleppegrell & M.C. Colombi (Eds.), Developing advanced
literacy in first and second languages: Meaning with power (pp. 87-118).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Martin, J.R. & Rose, D. (2003). Working with discourse. London:
Continuum.
Martin, J.R. & White, P.R.R. (2005). The language of
evaluation: Appraisal in English. Basingstoke, UK and New York: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Ministerial Council on Education, Employment, Training and Youth
Affairs (MCEETYA) (2008). Assessing student achievement in Australia
2009: Information for parents and the community. Retrieved from
www.mceetya.edu.au.
Myhill, D. (2008). Toward a linguistic model of sentence
development in writing. Language and Education, 22(5), 271-288.
Myhill, D. (2009). Becoming a designer: Trajectories of linguistic
development. In R. Beard, D. Myhill, J. Riley & M. Nystrand (Eds.),
The Sage handbook of writing development (pp. 402-414). London: Sage.
New Zealand Ministry of Education (2006). In focus: Information
kit: Student achievement in New Zealand. Wellington, NZ: New Zealand
Ministry of Education.
National Commission on Writing in America's Schools and
Colleges (2003). The Neglected 'R': The need for a writing
revolution. New York: The College Entrance Examination Board.
Pea, R. & Kurland, D. (1987). Cognitive technologies for
writing. Review of Research in Education, 14, 227-326.
Persky, H.R., Daane, M.C. & Jin, Y. (2003). The nation's
report card: Writing 2002. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. & Svartvik, J. (1985). A
comprehensive grammar of the English language. London: Longman.
Schleppegrell, M. (1998). Grammar as resource: Writing a
description. Research in the Teaching of English, 32(2), 182-211.
Schleppegrell, M. (2004). The language of schooling: A functional
linguistics perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schleppegrell, M.J. & Colombi, M.C. (1997). Text organization
by bilingual writers. Written Communication, 14, 481-503.
Science Voyages: Exploring the Life, Earth, and Physical Sciences
(level red, Florida edition) (2000). Columbus, OH: Glencoe.
Veel, R. (1997). Learning how to mean--scientifically speaking:
Apprenticeship into scientific discourse in the secondary school. In F.
Christie & J.R. Martin (Eds.), Genre and institutions: Social
processes in the workplace and school (pp. 161-195). London: Continuum.
Wilson, M. (2006). Rethinking rubrics in writing assessment.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Zhihui Fang [1] & Zhijun Wang [2]
[1] UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA, USA; [2] SHANGHAI UNIVERSITY OF FINANCE
AND ECONOMICS, CHINA
Table 1. writing components, evaluation questions, and functional
language analysis strategies
Writing Evaluation Questions Functional Language
components Analysis strategies
Content * What is going on * Analyse transitivity
in this text? patterns
* What does the (e.g., participants,
author tell us? processes, circumstances)
Organisation * How does the author * Analyse Themes/Rheme
organise this text? patterns
* Is the text well * Analyse cohesion patterns
organised?
* By what logic is * Analyse clause types
the text produced? and clause combining
strategies
Style/Tone/Voice * How does the * Analyse mood
author of this
text interact with * Analyse modality
the reader?
* What is the * Analyse word choices
author's and other
perspective? appraisal resources
* What is the tone
of the text?
Table 2. Two sample science reports
Text 1: I am writing about crocodiles.
crocodiles Crocodiles lived when the
(134 words) dinosaurs lived. Crocodiles
can live in water. Crocodiles
can live on land or in water.
They have good vision at
night. They have big mouths.
They like to eat in the sun.
They can open their mouth
really wide. They can close it
really tight. They eat chicken
like us. They like to eat
outside of water. They eat
fish. They eat raw meat. They
swim in lakes. They play in
grass. They are born out of
big eggs. They watch their
nest carefully, so their eggs
won't get scrambled. They
carry their young in their
mouth. They live in soggy
sand, 'the babies'. They have
rough skin. They can crawl up
a tree with a purse on. They
like alley water. They are
reptiles. (by a 9th grade
student)
Text 2: Fish are ectotherms that live
Fish in water and use gills to get
(129 words) oxygen. Gills are fleshy
filaments that are filled with
tiny blood vessels. The heart
of the fish pumps blood to the
gills. As blood passes through
the gills, it picks up oxygen
from water that is passing
over the gills. Carbon dioxide
is released from blood into
the water. Most fish have
fins. Fins are fanlike
structures used for steering,
balancing, and moving.
Usually, they are paired.
Those on the top and bottom
stabilise the fish. Those on
the side steer and move the
fish. Scales are another
common characteristic of fish,
although not all fish have
scales. Scales are hard, thin,
overlapping plates that cover
the skin. These protective
plates are made of a bony
material. (written by a
science expert, from Glencoe,
2000, p. 579)
Table 3. Process types in the two sample reports
Categories Text 1 (Crocodiles) Text 2 (Fish)
Doing Process 18 (72%) 7 (44%)
Sensing Process 1 (4%) 0 (0%)
Saying Process 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
Relating Process 4 (16%) 9 (56%)
Process Complex 2 (8%) (sensing + doing) 0 (0%)
Table 4. Noun groups (bolded) in the two sample reports
Text 1 Text 2
I am writing about crocodiles. Fish are ectotherms that live
crocodiles lived when the in water and use gills to get
dinosaurs lived. crocodiles oxygen. Gills are fleshy
can live in water. crocodiles filaments that are filled with
can live on land or in water. tiny blood vessels. The heart
They have good vision at of the fish pumps blood to the
night. They have big mouths. gills. As blood passes through
They like to eat in the sun. the gills, it picks up oxygen
They can open their mouth from water that is passing
really wide they can close it over the gills. carbon dioxide
really tight. They eat chicken is released from blood into
like us. They like to eat the water. Most fish have
outside of water. They eat fins. Fins are fanlike
fish. They eat raw meat. They structures used for steering,
swim in lakes. They play in balancing, and moving.
grass. They are born out of Usually, they are paired.
big eggs. They watch their Those on the top and bottom
nest carefully, so their eggs stabilise the fish. Those on
won't get scrambled. They the side steer and move the
carry their young in their fish. scales are another
mouth. They live in soggy common characteristic of fish,
sand, 'the babies'. They have although not all fish have
rough skin. They can crawl up scales. scales are hard, thin,
a tree with a purse on. They overlapping plates that cover
like alley water. They are the skin. These protective
reptiles. plates are made of a bony
material.
Table 5. clause themes in the two sample reports
Text 1 (crocodiles) Text 2 (Fish)
1. I 1. Fish
2. crocodiles 2. gills
3. the dinosaurs 3. the heart of the fish
4. crocodiles 4. as blood
5. crocodiles 5. it
6. they 6. carbon dioxide
7. they 7. most fish
8. they 8. fins
9. they 9. usually, they
10. they 10. those on the top and bottom
11. they 11. those on the side
12. they 12. (those on the side)
13. they 13. scales
14. they 14. not all fish
15. they 15. scales
16. they 16. these protective plates
17. they
18. they
19. so their eggs
20. they
21. they
22. they
23. they
24. they
25. they