Developing reading comprehension: combining visual and verbal cognitive processes.
Woolley, Gary
Introduction
The simple view of reading (Gough & Tunmer, 1986) provides a
model to conceptualise reading comprehension as the product of two
dimensions: listening comprehension and word decoding processes (Kirby
& Savage, 2008). The model makes it clear that children may differ
in respect to the two dimensions and, therefore, require different
teaching approaches to support their reading development. For example, a
number of researchers have identified the existence of children with
poor decoding but with good listening comprehension and children who
have good decoding skills but poor listening comprehension (e.g. Catts,
Hogan, & Fey, 2003; Snowling & Firth, 1997). Poor decoders have
difficulty comprehending because they often spend time and conscious
effort decoding individual letters and words, where the meaning is less
evident, rather than clustering words into larger meaningful wholes
(Idol, 1988; Kendeou, Savage, & Van den Broek, 2009; Robinson,
2001). Thus, word decoding is necessary but not sufficient for reading
comprehension and should be complemented by the development of language
comprehension skills (Fielding-Barnsley, Hay, & Ashman, 2005;
Woolley, 2006). Conversely, children with good decoding skills and poor
listening comprehension have language difficulties that inhibit reading
comprehension and are often inappropriately placed in phonic
instructional programs (Kendeou et al., 2009).
This article focuses on children with good decoding skills but with
poor listening comprehension. It will discuss how elaborated mental
models of narrative text promote reader comprehension. It is proposed
that the efficiency of mental modeling is largely determined by the
architecture of working memory and how attentional resources are
allocated. It is asserted that the allocation of cognitive resources
within working memory can be improved with the incorporation of visual
and verbal comprehension strategies. This enables the inferential
linking of information and the formation of more elaborated and coherent
mental models of story content leading to improved reading
comprehension. Furthermore, the article will show how the routine
incorporation of multiple comprehension strategies, using a
metacognitive framework, can increase students' self-regulation and
reading engagement. In doing so, it will address Pressley's (2002)
concern that there is a need to develop more multiple-strategy
intervention programs that are rich in individual instructional
components without simply having them thrown into the mix and made
overwhelmingly too complex for teachers to implement.
Construction of a mental model
A mental model is a cohesive representation of the meaning of the
text content (Kintsch, 1998). However, mental models do not generally
retain the verbatim text information but support more flexible knowledge
structures that can integrate both visual and verbal material. The
construction of a mental model is a dynamic constructive process, partly
determined by the interaction of the reader with the text structure and
story content. Initially, the mental model is goal directed and
predictions are made about upcoming story events, which may be adjusted
to incorporate unexpected scenarios. Skilled comprehenders are likely to
make inferences by incorporating relevant background knowledge to make
sense of implicit information found within texts to enhance
understanding (Pearson & Johnson, 1978; Snow, 2002; Stull &
Mayer, 2007). It is asserted that, when visual and verbal processes are
incorporated, children are much more engaged during reading because they
can actively utilise their prior knowledge more efficiently. Thus,
successful comprehenders tend to be imaginative readers and users of
language who are able to actively select and organise information from
complex texts (Block et al., 2002; Gambrell, 2004; Kamhi & Catts,
2002).
Working memory
Swanson, Howard, and Saez (2006) found that skilled comprehenders
outperformed a range of poor comprehenders on measures related to
working memory. A number of other researchers have posited that, during
reading, the ability to comprehend is enhanced when there is a reduction
in the overall cognitive load in working memory (Daneman & Green,
1986; Manset-Williamson & Nelson, 2005; Pressley, 2002). It is
asserted that memory load is affected by how attention is allocated
within and between the different component subsystems of working memory
during a particular reading episode (Achibald & Gathercole, 2007).
Thus, the way a mental model is constructed may be largely determined by
the architecture of memory and the reader's ability to effectively
operationalise a number of processes simultaneously and to bind or link
visual and verbal information in working memory.
One theoretical construct that is useful for conceptualising how
attention is allocated in working memory was advanced originally by
Baddeley and Hitch (1994) and further developed by Baddeley (2000). It
was proposed that a central executive facility allocates attention
resources for storage or processing and is responsible for temporary
activation of information from long-term memory (Alloway, Gathercole,
Willis, & Adams, 2004; Swanson, Howard, & Saez, 2006). This
executive facility called the central executive is linked directly to
two subsystems: the phonological loop and the visuospacial sketchpad.
The phonological loop retains auditory information in time related
serial order and is limited to what can be retained at any one time. It
has an attention demanding sub-vocal rehearsal process that can be used
to restore rapidly decaying verbal representations to keep them active
while other working memory processes are operating. The visuospacial
sketchpad is a second limited capacity slave system responsible for
holding visual information. Unlike the phonological loop the
visuospacial sketchpad holds visual information in the form of a spacial
representation where each item can be displayed simultaneously (Allen,
Baddeley, & Hitch, 2006; Alloway et al., 2004; Just & Carpenter,
1992). The efficiency of the two slave sub-systems is dependent on the
exclusion of irrelevant material and the quality or intensity of the
items retained (Cowan & Morey, 2006; Vogel, McCollough, &
Machizawa, 2005). The episodic buffer is a third sub-system and it is
also limited in terms of the number of episodes or chunks of material
that it can hold concurrently (Allen et al., 2006). It is assumed that,
within the buffer, representations from the two sub-systems and from
long-term memory are bound in the form of episodic chunks. To be
retrievable, the component representations must be adequately well
linked (Alloway et al., 2004). Information is represented as conscious
experience in the form of a developing mental model that also includes
elements of both time and space (Boa, Li, & Zhang, 2007). It is
asserted that many children who experience ongoing difficulties in
constructing appropriate mental models of story information may have a
deficit in the coordination and linking of information within working
memory (Gathercole, Alloway, Willis, & Adams, 2006; Savage, Lavers,
& Pillay, 2007; Swanson & Jerman, 2007).
Visualisation strategies
The efficiency of working memory can be improved by the
incorporation of some structured inferential learning activities
(Gathercole et al., 2006). In particular, a number of researchers have
proposed that visual imagery techniques can be effective inferential
learning strategies that can improve working memory efficiency by
reducing the cognitive load associated with the mental modeling process
(Joffe, Cain, & Maric, 2007; Sadoski & Willson, 2006; Woolley
& Hay, 2004). Yuill and OakHill (1991) contended that inferential
strategy instruction should improve working-memory efficiency because
linked story information can be processed more economically. However,
some researchers maintain that some instructional procedures can be
counter productive because they can incorporate unnecessary cognitive
activity that places heavy demands on working memory (McKeon, Beck,
& Blake, 2009; Sweller, 1988). The concern is that some strategic
processes will cause the learner to engage in extraneous cognitive
processing that does not support learning (Stull & Mayer, 2007). For
example, when word recognition is not automatic, letter-by-letter
decoding will be attention demanding and interfere with reading
comprehension. In situations like this executive processes will be
involved in more active or conscious chunking as opposed to automatic
chunking of information (Allen, Baddeley, & Hitch, 2006; Montgomery,
Magimairaj, & O'Malley, 2008; Stull & Mayer, 2007).
Initially the utilisation of any reading strategy may involve resource
demanding conscious attention (Afflerbach, Pearson, & Paris, 2008).
However, by developing a routine of stopping and discussing
visualisations at three different phases in the reading lesson: before,
during, and after reading (See Table 1) children are more able to
practise the strategy until it becomes automatic. The expectation is
that when they read independently they should focus on meaning (McKeon
et al., 2009) and automatically visualise story content throughout the
reading process. The assertion is that the use of a combination of
visual and verbal strategies may initially be attention-demanding
processes but can become automatic through explicit instruction and
practised on a variety of texts (Woolley & Hay, 2004). A number of
researchers have attested to the effectiveness of a range of different
visualisation strategies. Some of these will be examined below.
Illustrations
Illustrations can compensate for weak listening comprehension
skills in that they provide visual representations of main ideas and may
also provide a visual summary, particularly for younger readers (Kendeou
et al., 2009). Readers can also derive character depth and meaning from
illustrations in picture books (Roser, Martinez, Fuhrken, &
McDonnald, 2007). It has been shown that elaborated mental models are
constructed and reading comprehension is enhanced when children are
instructed to attend to story illustrations (Duke & Pearson, 2002;
Van Meter, Aleksic, Schwartz, & Garner, 2006). For example, Glenburg
and Langston (1992) found that during the reading process reader's
constructed and incorporated visual information from the illustrations
into their mental models using read text information. They asserted that
good illustrations help younger or less experienced readers by linking
their background knowledge with the unfolding text ideas and filling in
information not provided by the text. Van Meter, et al. (2006) have
suggested that, when illustrations are provided, verbal and nonverbal
representations are organised in working memory and are then linked or
bound to enhance mental modeling. Hibbing and Rankin-Erickson (2003)
maintained that older and more able readers tend to naturally visualise
story content and rely less on illustrations than younger or less
skilled readers.
Drawing
A number of researchers have also suggested that drawing could also
enhance the construction of elaborated mental models (Kintsch, 1994).
Van Meter, et al. (2006) maintained that drawing was strategic because
it is a goal-directed activity that enables improved reading
comprehension through the organisation of knowledge. Furthermore, the
researchers found that learners who made drawings engaged in more
self-monitoring behaviours than learners who did not draw. They
suggested that when readers draw, selected elements are organised to
construct a mental model as the learner activates prior knowledge to
connect with new understandings.
Van Meter et al. (2006) maintained that the integration of
knowledge is different when learners read and draw rather than when they
read and examine illustrations. The assumption is that when
illustrations are provided, the verbal and imaginal representations in
working memory are organised and then linked to form the mental model
(Van Meter, et al., 2006). However, in situations where the learner
encounters concepts for which no stored visual impression exists the
verbal description is used to generate online visual imagery. Perfetti,
(2007) asserted that the quality of the word representations affects
reading skill, including comprehension. Thus, one would expect that the
descriptive richness in the read story together with the child's
own extended vocabulary would influence the quality of the mental model
being constructed. Descriptive text-based richness is crucial to the
drawing strategy because it is the verbal representation that
complements and possibly directs the construction of the nonverbal
representations in memory (Van Meter, et al., 2006). Thus, drawing and
the quality of language of the text base mutually reinforce one another.
Elaborated instructions can also direct a learner's attention
to the structures that connect one feature to another (Van Meter, et
al., 2006). Van Meter, et al. found that upper primary school
participants learned more when the drawing strategy was used with
supported dialogue. Thus, the nonverbal or visual representation may be
complemented by the use of verbal descriptions and directions. For
example, when drawing, a learner may see the need to determine the
specific location of an item. Such a realisation may lead to a
reappraisal of the text and selection of specific verbal information to
direct the learner's construction of a visual representation.
Manipulations
Roser et al. (2007) maintained that a story character's world
could become more concrete, comprehensible, and more able to be
discussed when objects, items, pictures, and maps are used in
conjunction with read stories. Glenberg et al. (2007) found that having
readers manipulate objects to emulate characters and their actions in a
text greatly enhanced comprehension as measured by both recall and
inference tests. The researchers maintained that manipulations enabled
links of words to objects as well as requiring the reader to visualise
story elements and relationships as directed by the narrative syntax.
Visualisation
Glenberg et al. (2007) observed that children retained visualised
images when tested using texts connected to object manipulations several
days later. Furthermore, a number of other researchers have also found
that when readers used mental imagery as a strategy it has resulted in
improved reading comprehension outcomes (Pressley, 2002; National
Reading Panel, 2000; Sadoski & Quast, 1990). It is assumed that
students' use of mental imagery actively links read text
information to the reader's own background experiences and provides
a memory strategy that enhances recall and comprehension of the text
(Joffe et al., 2007; Kosslyn, 1976; Romeo, 2002). For example, Romeo
(2002) found that when reading tutors read rich descriptive texts aloud,
it enabled children to use enhanced visualisations that lead to improved
comprehension. At a number of stages during the reading the tutors were
instructed to stop and ask the children to visualise the story events
and then discuss their images. Later, it was discovered that the
children automatically imaged the text events during reading without
prompting. It has been suggested that the associated focused discussion
can enhance the vividness of mental imagery and improved comprehension
(Woolley & Hay, 2004). Farah (1995) suggested that the vividness of
mental imagery activated during reading increases reading engagement,
possibly because imagery relies heavily on the activation and
utilisation of past experience.
Characterisations
One effective way for readers to decipher a complex plot thread in
narratives is to try and understand the protagonist's perspective
through story events. For example, Emery, (1996) found that students who
discussed the story from a character's perspective, after reading a
passage, had more positive ratings on their re-telling of the story and
were better able to identify the story's central problem. The
inter-relationships of the main characters, in particular, may provide
coherence between several sub-plots or incidences within the narrative
structure (Roser et al., 2007). Emery (1996) maintained that a
'characters' beliefs, desires, feelings, and thoughts are the
glue that holds the story together' (p. 534). Roser et al. (2007)
maintained that it is the inner character traits that give the most
insights on story plots and themes. Characters are shaped by their
contexts and are developed by the interaction with other characters,
settings and plots. When children are encouraged to focus on character
perspectives it guides them through their stories, helping them to
understand plots and consider overall story themes (Roser et al., 2007).
Wade, Buxton, and Kelly (1999) argued that readers' interest could
also be further enhanced by enabling them to visualise the story content
by identifying with the central character and mentally placing
themselves in a story scene.
Younger children tend to place more importance on the actions of
characters in their causal models of stories. In contrast, older readers
are more able to focus on the mental states of characters and on the
more abstract event features. Older children also seem to show a
stronger tendency in establishing causal connections across story
episodes and longer text discourse than younger children (Rapp, Van den
Broek, McMaster, Kendeou, & Espin, 2007). Thus, skilled or older
readers are more able to progress to lengthier, more complex books by
encountering increasingly more well-developed characters that react to
circumstances in more meaningful and predictable ways throughout the
course of the narrative (Roser et al., 2007).
Text structure and organisation
In general, the ability to integrate contextual information within
a text is important for comprehension because it helps the reader to
build a coherent representation of a text's meaning (Kintsch, 1998;
Van der Schoot, Vasbinder, Reijntjes, Horsley, & Lieshout, 2009).
However, less skilled readers have more difficulty utilising overall
text structure, such as grammatical and contextual meaning cues, and
need to be encouraged to use their inferential skills when reading
(Catts, Hogan, & Fey, 2003; Bishop, 1997; Bowyer-Crane &
Snowling, 2005). In particular, such readers appear to have difficulty
visualising story content at the local level of understanding and also
have difficulty retelling the gist of stories at the more global level
(Diehl, Bennetto, & Young, 2006; Pressley, 2002, Woolley & Hay,
2004).
Knowledge of a story or text structure is often one of the most
important elements in the comprehension of the narrative (Marr &
Gormley, 1982; Pearson, Roehler, Dole, & Duffy, 1992; Whaley, 1981a;
1981b). For a text to be processed and understood it must have a logical
structure and contain cohesive devices to assist with the construction
of mental models of the read text. For example, understanding time order
sequences in text passages will help to facilitate the reader's
ability to comprehend and logically organise the narrative information
(Morrow, 1985; Trabasso & Sperry, 1985). Understanding the causal
connections, as they relate to an event, or sequence of events may also
play an important role in establishing the coherence of a story (Renz,
Lorch, Milich, & Lemburger, et al., 2003). Such structures are
important cohesive devices that enable the global organisation and
understanding of read text information because they incorporate the
story theme or gist (Kintsch, 1982). Thus, the theme of the story has a
critical role to play in the coherence of texts and the development of
appropriate mental modeling of text information (Harris & Pressley,
1991; Zhang & Hoosain, 2001). Normally, skilled readers use the
largest, most general existing schema frame to construct their mental
models, by organising their stories into schemas with settings, plots,
and episodes (Diehl et al., 2006). That is, a skilled reader may
activate the most appropriate stored schema to facilitate the
organisation of read text information together with the retrieval of
linked memory information. In this way a skilled readers'
comprehension of the read text is directly associated with, and
influenced by, their own past experiences.
Smaller units within the text structure may also contribute to the
enhancement of the mental model. For example, cohesive devices such as
introductory paragraphs may help organise and introduce preceding frames
and story lines. Diehl et al. (2006) maintained that an event's
relative importance to a story increases with the number of causal
connections. This makes it easier for the reader to form links with
existing ideas about the story and helps the reader to relate the story
information to prior knowledge. However, when elements within the text
are less cohesive it can make it more difficult for the reader. For
example, Meyer (1975) found that information further away from the main
theme was generally forgotten faster than information with direct
connections to the overall content structure. A number of researchers
have found that graphic organisers can link global conceptualisations of
the text and story structure, particularly when used as a cooperative
group activity where discussion of related ideas can take place (Nesbit
& Adesope, 2006; Van Boxtel, Van der Linden, Roelfs, & Erkens,
2002).
The use of visualisation and verbal strategies within an
intervention framework (see Table 1) may partly be determined by how
suitable they are for the readers' stage of development and at what
phase they are used in the reading lesson. Table 1 has been included to
provide an example of a flexible multiple-strategy framework to show how
and when these two aspects can be orchestrated within a lesson.
Multiple-strategy framework
In the past individual strategies have been introduced one at a
time and children were unsure of when, where, and how to apply them to
new reading situations (Pressley, 2002; Afflerbach et al., 2008). What
is needed is the development of reading comprehension interventions that
will enable skills to be developed routinely and practised on a number
of reading passages (Block, Paris, Reed, Whiteley, & Cleveland,
2009). A number of researchers have also emphasised that the
simultaneous teaching of a combination of techniques needs to be a
priority (NRP, 2000: Pressley, 2002). When considering what should be
included, other researchers have suggested that both verbal and visual
mental imagery techniques should be incorporated in the mix because it
promotes inferential linking, deeper engagement, and interest in reading
(Long, Winograd, & Bridge, 1989; Sadoski & Quast, 1990; Romeo,
2002; Tobias, 1994). Although multiple-strategy interventions are not
new, visualisation and verbal strategies have yet to be fully utilised
and included in classroom reading comprehension practice (Pressley,
2002).
Linking visual and verbal strategies
Many executive function difficulties can be attributed to
difficulties in the ability to use language to organise one's
thinking and behaviour (Leekam, 2007). For example, vocabulary knowledge
underlies all learning and is one of the most significant predictors of
reading comprehension. However, the acquisition of vocabulary and its
usefulness depends on the quality of word representations and the way in
which they are encoded and linked in working memory (Perfetti, 2007; Van
der Schoot, Vasbinder, Reijntjes, Horsley, & Lieshout, 2009).
Blachowicz, Fisher, and Ogle (2006) maintained that the ability to make
inferences is a crucial component in learning the meaning of new words
and in reading comprehension. They suggested that having students make
semantic connections among words, and verbalising or explaining those
connections, supports learning their meanings. Moreover, Van Boxtel, Van
der Linden, Roelfs, and Erkens (2002) suggested that making a concept
map during a reading lesson helps learners develop the depth of word
meanings and contributes to the development of an integrated mental
model. They asserted that, when used in a collaborative setting, focused
discussion enhances students' understandings and develops the
quality and depth of their word representations (also Perfetti, 2007).
It has been demonstrated that, when reading and thinking processes
are taught to students through dialogic interactions, they increase
students' engagement and control of the reading comprehension
process (Cole, 2002; Guthrie & Davis, 2003; Hareli & Weiner,
2002; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1988). Higher student achievement and
more positive social, motivational, and attitudinal outcomes have also
been found to occur in collaborative learning contexts (Gambrell,
Malloy, & Mazzoni, 2007; Overett & Donald, 1998; Woolley, 2007).
The involvement of students in group discussions during and after
listening to a story has been shown to lead to improved comprehension,
particularly when the teacher asks questions or prompts students to
describe what they have read (Gambrell, Mazzoni, & Almasi, 2000).
Directed questions may also contribute to reading comprehension by
focusing attention on text segments containing information being sought
(Taboada & Guthrie, 2006). Explanatory answers to those questions
can further improve the students' comprehension of read text and
enable a more efficient use of language through focused dialogue (Snow,
2002). Thus, requiring students to self-explain during rereading will
promote active learning that has been shown to lead to a significant
improvement in self-monitoring for all the readers (Griffin, Wiley,
& Thiede, 2008).
Self-regulation and engagement
Effective comprehension strategy users are metacognitively aware
and have control of the reading process by being flexible in adapting
their actions as they read. In doing so they make deliberate, goal
directed attempts to gain meaning when they read (Afflerbach et al.,
2008). A metacognitive focus involves active and reflective engagement
in reading. It often involves a collaborative approach incorporating
techniques such as questioning and peer assisted learning in which
children adopt collaborative roles to corporately analyse texts (McKeon
et al., 2009; Pressley et al., 2006). Such techniques can be augmented
by other strategies such as: comprehension monitoring,
self-explanations, mental imagery, identification of the main idea,
previewing, predicting, and summarising text etc. (Kirby & Savage,
2008). Zimmerman (2002) suggested that a metacognitive focus would lead
to self-regulation. He maintained that self-regulation could be simply
viewed as having three phases whereby learners set goals, monitor
progress, and reflect on learning. Self-regulation techniques should be
incorporated in instructional intervention frameworks to support the
integration of new and existing strategies. In Table 2 the three
self-regulation phases have been incorporated to show how they can be
linked to the before, during, and after phases of the framework and how
they can be associated with some visual and verbal strategies.
Glenburg and Langston (1992) found positive results for reading
comprehension when readers were encouraged to focus on illustrations and
to use self-questioning strategies about story events. Other researchers
have also found that students who compose and answer their own questions
were perceived as taking a more strategic and self-regulatory role in
the learning process (Palincsar & Brown, 1984). Taboada &
Guthrie, (2006) maintained that higher order questions were associated
with high levels of conceptual knowledge gained from text, showing a
clear correlation with questioning levels and reading higher
comprehension outcomes. The researchers suggested that reading
comprehension could be greatly enhanced when students are taught to ask
questions that go beyond the literal level and require integration of
information between the text and the reader's prior knowledge.
Taboada and Guthrie (2006) postulated that students who tend to ask
lower order questions focus more on the local level information and
struggle with identifying the overall global text structure. It was
assumed that higher order questions enable the reader to connect their
prior knowledge to the text base more easily for several reasons. These
questions anticipate a possible macrostructure of the mental model that
includes the major interrelationships among the concepts within the
story. The reader, who asks high-order questions, preconstructs a mental
model into which the text base can be more easily integrated. Thus,
higher order questions tend to organise and represent knowledge built
from text into hierarchical cognitive structures. Readers, who ask high
order, conceptual questions are more able to anticipate and bring to the
text an elaborated global structure. Such readers tend to build fuller
text representations and richer mental models, characterised by a larger
number of connections and relationships among the major concepts in the
text and with their background knowledge (Kintsch, 1998). When students
are instructed to ask their own questions it enables them to enter into
a deeper interaction as they ponder relationships among different
aspects of the text. As a result, they are more able to selectively use
attention to focus on different sections of the text, develop main
ideas, hypothesise about content, make predictions about upcoming
information, and monitor their own comprehension. For example, when they
are reading they may ask themselves questions such as 'Does this
make sense?' If it does not make sense, they apply suitable repair
strategies to restore comprehension (Van der Schoot et al., 2009).
Summary
Successful comprehenders construct mental models that incorporate
elaborated text based information with their available prior knowledge.
Mental models are flexible representations that are constantly updated
to reflect the most recent conceptualisations of read text information.
However, the ability to form adequate mental models may be largely
determined by the efficiency of working memory in allocating resources
effectively. The ability to coordinate and allocate limited resources is
vitally important to effective reading and comprehension. Visual and
verbal instructional techniques can help overcome cognitive capacity
limitations by utilising the subsystems of working memory more
efficiently. Thus, reading comprehension is enhanced when visual and
verbal information is utilised and linked in the working memory. When
children are encouraged to visualise story content and to enter into
dialogue with others it elaborates and deepens the quality of their
mental representations. It also enables the reader to make connections
between verbal and visual content in a much more integrated way. Thus,
the quality of a reader's mental model will be enhanced by the
quality of the linking of information within working memory.
Despite the efficacy of visual and verbal comprehension strategies,
they have not as yet been used extensively in multiple-strategy
interventions programs. However, there are a number of evidence based
visualising strategies that can be employed in intervention programs to
enhance the local and global levels of understanding. The implication is
that positive literacy outcomes for readers with comprehension
difficulties will result when visual and verbal comprehension strategies
are employed in a well coordinated reading intervention framework. It is
important to use a number of strategies in an intervention framework and
to apply these strategies routinely over several reading episodes to
consolidate those strategies and to develop automaticity. Strategy use
will be enhanced when readers are encouraged to actively monitor and
reflect on the comprehension process by using self-questioning and
self-explanations. Moreover, when applied in an interactive
collaborative context, self-regulation and reading engagement will be
promoted.
Acknowledgement
This submission has had research support from the Faculty of
Education, Griffith University, Australia.
References
Achibald, L.M.D., & Gathercole, S.E. (2007). The complexities
of complex memory span: Storage and processing deficits in specific
language impairment. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 177-194.
Afflerbach, P., Pearson, D., & Paris, S.G. (2008). Clarifying
differences between reading skills and reading strategies. The Reading
Teacher, 61(5), 364-373.
Allen, R.J., Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. J. (2006). Is the
binding of visual features in working memory resource demanding? Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 135(2), 298-313.
Alloway, T.P., Gathercole, S.E., Willis, C., & Adams, A.
(2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive
skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87,
85-106.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of
working memory? Trends in cognitive sciences, 4(1), 417-423.
Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. (1994). Developments in the concept
of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8, 485-493.
Bao, M. Li, Z., & Zhang, D. (2007) Binding facilitates
attention switching within working memory. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 33(5), 959-969.
Bishop, D.V.M. (1997). Uncommon understanding: Development and
disorder of language comprehension in children. Hove, UK: Psychological
Press.
Blachowicz, C.L.Z., Fisher, P.J.L., & Ogle, D. (2006).
Vocabulary: Questions from the classroom. Reading Research Quarterly,
41(4), 524-529.
Block, C.C., & Johnson, R.B. (2002). The thinking process
approach: Preparing students for the future comprehension challenges. In
C. Collins Block, L.B. Gambrell, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Improving
comprehension instruction (pp. 385-389). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Block, C.C., Paris, S.R., Reed, K.L., Whiteley, C.S., &
Cleveland, M.D. (2009). Instructional approaches that signify increase
reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2),
262-281.
Bowyer-Crane, C., & Snowling, M.J. (2005). Assessing
children's inference generation: What do tests of reading
comprehension measure? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75,
189-201.
Catts, H.W., Hogan, T.P., & Fey, M.E. (2003). Subgrouping poor
readers on the basis of individual differences in reading-related
abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 151-164.
Cole, J.E. (2002). What motivates students to read? Four literacy
personalities. The Reading Teacher, 56, 326-336.
Cowan, N. & Morey, C.C. (2006). Visual working memory depends
on attentional filtering. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(4), 139-141.
Daneman, M., & Green, I. (1986). Individual differences in
comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and
Language, 25, 1-18.
Diehl, J.J., Bennetto, L., & Young, E.C. (2006). Story recall
and narrative coherence of high-functioning children with autism
spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(1), 87-102.
Duke, N.K., & Pearson, P.D. (2002). Effective practices for
developing reading comprehension. In A.E. Farstrup & S.J. Samuels
(Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction, (3rd ed.,
pp. 205-242). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Emery, D.W. (1996). Helping readers comprehend stories from the
characters' perspectives. The Reading Teacher, 49, 534-541.
Farah, M.J. (1995). Current issues in the neuropsychology of image
generation. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1455-1471.
Fielding-Barnsley, R., Hay, I., & Ashman, A. (2005).
Phonological awareness: Necessary but not sufficient. Paper presented at
the National Conference of the Australian Association of Special
Education, Brisbane, September, 2005.
Gambrell, L.B. (2004). Exploring the connection between oral
language and early reading. The Reading Teacher, 57, 490-492.
Gambrell, L.B., Malloy, J.A., & Mazzoni, S.A. (2007).
Evidence-based best practice for comprehensive literacy instruction. In
L.B. Gambrell, L.M. Morrow, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Best Practices in
Literacy Instruction (3rd ed., pp. 11-29). NY: The Guilford Press.
Gambrell, B., Mazzoni, S.A., & Almasi, J.F. (2000). Promoting
collaboration, social interaction, and engagement. In L. Baker, M.J.
Dreher, & J.T. Guthrie (Eds.), Engaging young readers: Promoting
achievement and motivation (pp. 119-139). New York: Guilford Press.
Gathercole, S.E., Alloway, T.P., Willis, C., & Adams, A.
(2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265-281.
Gough, P.B., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading, and
reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6-10.
Glenberg, A. M., Brown, M., & Levin, J. R. (2007). Enhancing
comprehension in small reading groups using a manipulation strategy.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 389-399.
Glenberg, A.M., & Langston, W.E. (1992). Comprehension of
illustrated text: pictures help to build mental models. Journal of
Memory and Language, 31, 129-151.
Griffin, T.D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K.W. (2008). Individual
differences, rereading, and self-explanation: Concurrent processing and
cue validity as constraints on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory &
Cognition, 36(1) 93-104.
Guthrie, J.T., & Davis, M.H. (2003). Motivating the struggling
readers in middle school through an engagement model of classroom
practice. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 59-85.
Hareli, S., & Weiner, B. (2002). Social emotions and
personality inferences: A scaffold for a new direction in the study of
achievement motivation. Educational Psychologist, 37, 183-193.
Harris, K.R., & Pressely, M. (1991). The nature of cognitive
strategy instruction: Interactive strategy instruction. Exceptional
Children. 57, 392-404.
Hibbing, A.N., & Rankin-Erikson, J.L. (2003). A picture is
worth a thousand words: Using visual images to improve comprehension for
the middle school struggling readers. The Reading Teacher, 56, 758-770.
Idol, L. (1988). Johnny can't read: Does the fault lie with
the book, the teacher, or Johnny? Remedial and Special Education, 19,
8-25.
Joffe, V.L., Cain, K., Maric, N. (2007). Comprehension problems in
children with specific language impairment: Does mental imagery training
help? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,
42(6), 648-664.
Just, M.A., & Carpenter, P.A. (1992). A capacity theory of
comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychology
Review, 99, 122-149.
Kamhi, A., & Catts, H. (2002). The language basis of reading:
Implications for classification and treatment of children with reading
disabilities. In K.G. Butler & E. Silliman (Eds.), Speaking,
reading, and writing in children with language learning disabilities:
New paradigms in research and practice (pp. 45-72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Pub.
Kendeou, P., Savage, R., Van den Broek, P. (2009). Revising the
simple view of reading. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79,
353-370.
Kintsch, W. (1994). Text comprehension, memory and learning.
American Psychologist, 49, 294-303.
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Kintsch, W. (1982). Memory for text. In A. Flammer & W. Kintsch
(Eds.), Discourse processing (pp. 186-204). NY: North Holland.
Kirby, J.R., & Savage, J.S. (2008). Can the simple view deal
with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42(2), 75-82.
Kosslyn, S.M. (1976). Using imagery to retrieve semantic
information: A developmental study. Child Development, 47, 434-444.
Leekam, S. (2007). Language comprehension difficulties in children
with autism spectrum disorders. In C. Cain and J. Oakhill (Eds.),
Children's Comprehension Problems in Oral and Written Language: A
Cognitive Perspective (pp. 104-127). NY: Guilford Press.
Long, S.A., Winograd, P.N., & Bridge, C.A. (1989). The effects
of reader and text characteristics on imagery reported during and after
reading, Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 353-372.
McKeon, M.G., Beck, I. L., & Blake, R.G.K. (2009). Rethinking
reading comprehension instruction: A comparison of instruction for
strategies and content approaches. Reading Research Quarterly, 44(3),
218-253.
Manset-Williamson, G., & Nelson, J.M. (2005). Balanced,
strategic reading instruction for upper-elementary and middle school
students with reading disabilities: A comparative study of two
approaches. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28, 59-74.
Marr, M.B., & Gormley, K. (1982). Children's recall of
familiar and unfamiliar text. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 89-104.
Meyer, B.J.F. (1975). The organization of prose and its effects on
memory. Amsterdam: North Holland.
Montgomery, J.W., Magimairaj, B.M., & O'Malley, M.H.
(2008). Role of working memory in typically developing children's
complex sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,
7:331-354.
Morrow, L.M. (1985). Retelling stories: A strategy for improving
young children's comprehension, concept of story structure, and
oral language complexity. Elementary School Journal, 85, 647-661.
National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to read: Report of
the comprehension instruction subgroup to the National Institute of
Child Health and Development. Washington, DC: NICD.
Nesbit, J.C., & Adesope, O.O. (2006). Learning with concept and
knowledge maps: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 76(3),
413-448.
Overett, J., & Donald, D. (1998). Paired reading: effects of a
parent involvement program in a disadvantaged community in South Africa.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 347-356.
Palincsar, A.S., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of
comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities.
Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117-175.
Pearson, D.P., & Johnson D.D. (1978). Teaching reading
comprehension. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Pearson, P.D., Roehler, L.R., Dole, J.A., & Duffy, G.G. (1992).
Developing expertise in reading comprehension. In S.J. Samuels &
A.E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction
(pp.101-144). Newark: International Reading Association.
Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical Quality to
Comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 357-383.
Pressley, M. (2002). Comprehension instruction: What makes sense
now, what might make sense soon. International Reading Association
Online Document, Accessed from
http://www.readingonline.org/articles/handbook/pressley/ index.html.
Pressley, M.G. (2006). Reading instruction that works: The case for
balanced teaching (3rd ed.). New York: Gilford Press.
Rapp, D.N., Van den Broek, P., McMaster, K.L., Kendeou, P., &
Espin, C.A. (2007). Higher order comprehension processes in struggling
readers: A perspective for research and intervention. Scientific Studies
in Reading, 11(4), 289-312.
Renz, K., Lorch, E.P., Milich, R., Lemberger, C., Bodner, A., &
Welsh, R. (2003). On-line story representation in boys with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
31(1), 93-105.
Robinson, G. L. (2001). Problems in literacy and numeracy. In P.
Foreman (Ed.), Integration and Inclusion (pp.167-229). Victoria: Nelson.
Romeo, L. (2002). At-risk students: Learning to break through
comprehension barriers. In C. Collins Block, L.B. Gambrell, & M.
Pressley (Eds.), Improving comprehension instruction (pp. 385-389). San
Francisco, Jossey-Bass.
Roser, N., Martinez, M., Fuhrken, C., & McDonnold, K. (2007).
Characters as guides to meaning. The Reading Teacher, 60(6), 548-559.
Sadoski, M., & Quast, Z. (1990). Reader response and long-term
recall for journalistic text: The roles of imagery, affect, and
importance. Reading Research Quarterly, 25, 256-272.
Sadoski, M., & Willson, V.L. (2006). Effects of a theoretically
based large-scale reading intervention in a multicultural urban school
district. American Educational Research Journal, 43, 137-484.
Savage, R., Lavers, N. & Pillay, V. (2007). Working memory and
reading difficulties: What we know and what we don't know about the
relationship. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 185-121.
Snow, C.E. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward a research and
development program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Rand
Corp. Retrieved December 12, 2002, from
http://www.rand.org/publications/MR/MR1465/
Snowling, M., & Firth, U. (1997). Comprehesion in hyperlexic
readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 392-415.
Stull, A., Mayer, R.E. (2007). Learning by doing versus learning by
viewing: Three experimental comparisons of learner-generated versus
author-provided graphic organisers. Journal of Educational Psychology.
99(4), 808-820.
Swanson, H.L., Howard, C.B., & Saez, L. (2006). Do different
components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading
disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(3), 252-269.
Swanson, H.L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working
memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading
difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 249-283.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects
on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257-285.
Taboada, A., & Guthrie, J.T. (2006). Contributions of student
questioning and prior knowledge to construction of knowledge from
reading information text. Journal of Literacy Research, 38(1), 1-35.
Trabasso, T., & Sperry, L.L. (1985). Causal relatedness and
importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 595-611.
Tobias, S. (1994). Interest, prior knowledge, and learning. Review
of Educational Research, 64, 37-54.
Van Boxtel, C., Van der linden, J., Roelofs, E., & Erkens, G.
(2002). Collaborative concept mapping: Provoking and supporting
meaningful discourse. Theory Into Practice, 41(1), 40-46.
Van der Schoot, M., Vasbinder, A.L., Horsley, T.M., Reijntjes, A.,
& Van Lieshout, E.C.D.M. (2009). Lexical ambiguity resolution in
good and poor comprehenders: An eye fixation and self-paced reading
study in primary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology,
101(1), 21-36.
Van Meter, P., Aleksic, M., Schwartz, A., & Garner, J. (2006).
Learner-generated drawing as a strategy for learning from content area
text. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 142-166.
Vogel, E.K., McCollough, A.W., & Machizawa, M.G. (2005). Neural
measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working
memory. Nature, 438, 500-503.
Wade, S.E., Buxton, W.M., & Kelly, M. (1999). Using
think-alouds to examine reader text interest. Reading Research
Quarterly, 34, 194-216.
Whaley, J.F. (1981a). Readers' expectation for story
structures. Reading Research Quarterly, 17, 90-114.
Whaley, J.F. (1981b). Story grammars and reading instruction.
Reading Teacher, 34, 762-771.
Whitehurst, G.L., & Lonigan, C.J. (1988). Child development and
emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848-872.
Woolley, G.E. (2007). A comprehension intervention for children
with reading comprehension difficulties. Australian Journal of Learning
Difficulties, 12(1), 43-50.
Woolley, G.E. (2006). Comprehension difficulties after year 4:
Actioning appropriately. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties,
11(3), 125-130.
Woolley, G.E., & Hay, I. (2004). Using imagery as a strategy to
enhance students' comprehension of read text. In B.A. Knight &
W. Scott (Eds.), Learning difficulties: Multiple perspectives (pp.
85-101). Frenchs Forest. NSW: Pearson.
Yuill, N., & Oakhill, J. (1991). Children's problems in
text comprehension. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zhang, H., & Hoosain, R. (2001). The influence of narrative
text characteristics on thematic inference during reading. Journal of
Research in Reading, 24, 173-186.
Zimmerman, B.J. (2002). Becoming a self-regulated learner: An
overview. Theory Into Practice, 41, 64-70.
Gary Woolley
Griffith University
Table 1.
A flexible multiple-strategy framework
(incorporating some visualising strategies)
Phases Processes
Visualising/local Visualising/global
Scenes/events Episodes/story
Before reading Visualising similar Using a graphic
scenes from similar organiser to
background experiences understand the
to draw the opening structure of the text
scene of the story. to be read. Asking
questions to help
elaborate drawn
pictures.
During reading Stoping at an Asking and answering
appropriate place in questions related to
the narrative and character actions and
drawing a picture scenes as it relates
related to story to the drawings.
events as the plot
unfolds.
After reading Making a summary Using the graphic
drawing of the organiser as a way to
resolution scene. organise a summary by
placing each of the
three drawings in the
appropriate space on
the organiser and
making an oral
summary.
N.B. The strategies used within this matrix are meant to be examples
only and in practice they would be determined by factors such as
skill level, stage of development and individual or group
characteristics.
Table 2.
A flexible metacognitive framework
(incorporating some metacognitive strategies)
Phases Processes
Visualising/local Visualising/global
Scenes/events Episodes/story
Before Visualising similar Using a graphic
reading scenes from similar organiser to understand
background the structure of
experiences to draw the the text to be read.
opening scene of the Asking questions to
story. help elaborate drawn
pictures.
During Stoping at an Asking and answering
reading appropriate place in the questions related to
narrative and drawing character actions and
a picture related to scenes as it relates to the
story events as the plot drawings.
unfolds.
After Making a summary Using the graphic
reading drawing of the organiser as a way to
resolution scene. organise a summary
by placing each of the
three drawings in the
appropriate space on the
organiser and making an
oral summary.
Phases Processes
Self-regulation and self-
questioning
Before Goal setting What do I
reading think will happen in this
Story? Visualise likely
scenarios
During Monitoring meaning/
reading self-questioning Is the
story similar to what
I imagined at the
beginning? How is it the
same/different? What
do I think will happen
now? Visualise a revised
scenario.
After Reflection on strategies
reading used. What can I imagine
now that I have come
to the end? Were my
predictions accurate?
What was expected/
unexpected? What else
could I have considered?
N.B. As language is instrumental in organising thinking and
vocabulary learning it is important that there is a great
deal of discussion to link visual imaginal representations
with verbal conceptual understandings.