Reading digital texts.
Walsh, Maureen ; Asha, Jennifer ; Sprainger, Nicole 等
The current emphasis on the teaching and integration of technology
in the primary classroom lacks specific direction for teaching the
reading of digital texts. This paper reports on the findings of a
research project that investigated the way students read and navigate
digital texts. The researchers looked particularly at the students'
'reading practices' and whether the explicit teaching of the
metalanguage of visual grammar allowed students to discuss and
understand digital texts.
Introduction
The current emphasis on the teaching and integration of technology
in the primary classroom lacks specific direction for teaching about and
with digital texts although several researchers (Unsworth, 2001; Kress,
2003; Unsworth, Thomas, & Bush, 2004; Simpson, 2004; Walsh, 2006)
are investigating aspects of this area. In light of the findings from
the National Literacy Inquiry and its report, Teaching Reading (DEST,
2005), it is essential that further research develop evidence for the
ways in which students read and learn from digital texts and how this
evidence may inform both new theory and pedagogy. This paper describes a
small study that aimed to investigate the way students read and navigate
digital texts and whether the explicit teaching of visual grammar, with
a shared metalanguage, allows students and teachers to discuss and
understand digital texts.
Current research
The environment of students today is filled with digital texts. The
textual shift that has occurred entails more than just distinguishing
the difference between reading print-based texts and reading digital
texts. Students are comfortable with the range of technology that allows
them to surf the internet, send a text message or photo to a friend, or
play a digital game while listening to music. They are able to multitask
between a variety of digital media, simultaneously processing the
various modes of print, image, movement, graphics, animation, sound and
music. Such a shift in communication has many researchers contending
that there needs to be a pedagogical shift so that the classroom is able
to incorporate these new modes of communication (e.g. Kress & Van
Leeuwen, 2001; Kress et al., 2001; Kress, 2003; Unsworth, 2001, 2003;
Lankshear, Snyder, & Green, 2000; Lankshear & Noble, 2003;
Lemke, 2002; Gee, 2003).
Research is at the early stages of determining the exact features
that are needed for reading digital texts and how the reading process is
similar to or different from reading conventional texts (Walsh, 2006).
During the 1990s there was a strong move to determine the differences
between reading images and reading print, and to establish image-text
relations, along with attempts to develop a 'visual grammar'
(Kress & van Leeuwen, 1996). There have been more recent attempts to
refine this visual grammar so it applies to digital or multimodal texts
(Unsworth, 2001; Callow & Zammitt, 2002; Simpson, 2004; Noad, 2005).
Kress (2003) has shown that the difference between reading words
and images is in the 'logic of the words' compared with the
'logic of the image'. For example, he shows that the logic of
words is linear and sequential whereas the logic of an image is
non-linear and non-sequential. In an analysis of the similarities and
differences between reading print and multimodal texts, Walsh (2006) has
demonstrated how the similarities are linked to aspects of
meaning-making, whatever the type of text or purpose of the reader. She
shows that the differences are in the processing of the different modes
and in the affordances of these modes. In multimodal texts, compared
with print-based texts, the reader will use various senses (sight,
hearing, tactile, kinaesthetic) to respond to other modes. This
processing may be influenced by the synchronous effects of images,
colour, line, angle, position or the arrangement of these with movement,
animation or sound effects. We need to theorise the interactions that
occur as readers process these modes, separately or simultaneously, in a
digital text.
As well as theorising the reading process with digital texts, we
need to determine those modes that teachers need to be aware of to
assist students in reading digital texts. We have developed some
standard approaches for helping students learn to read, from developing
concepts of print to word recognition and comprehension strategies, but
we have not developed strategies for assisting students with reading
digital texts and determining the specific skills needed. Turbill (2003)
has recently been developing a framework for monitoring 'concepts
of screen' with Kindergarten children and this framework is
patterned on Clay's 'concepts of print' (1979). In a
similar way, researchers in the US, Lefever-Davis and Pearman (2005),
have added aspects of electronic criteria to running records in
assessing students' reading. Further development is needed to
determine whether we 'add to' theories of reading print or
whether we develop quite different criteria. Currently a Special
Interest Group of the United Kingdom Literacy Association is researching
aspects of students' 'Reading on Screen'.
Research on reading digital texts is new and more studies have been
conducted to date with the reading of CD ROMs. Unsworth (2003) has
developed a detailed analysis of electronic books and CD ROM narratives.
He contends that the negotiation of hypertext and hypermedia links in
such texts is more likely to augment than to harm the reading process.
He shows how some of the ways multimodal resources are used in digital
texts can facilitate more active, reflective reading. Unsworth
emphasises the importance of educators mediating the interaction between
new digital media and conventional text forms. Asha (2005) has shown how
a classroom teacher can integrate the reading and use of websites into
teaching children's literature.
Studies in the United States have highlighted advantages and
disadvantages of students' reading on screen, particularly CD ROMs.
For example, Doty et al. (2001) have shown that visuals, graphics and
sound effects assist prediction, comprehension and vocabulary knowledge.
Mathews (1996) found that children engaged in richer storytelling after
reading CD ROM stories while Lefever-Davis and Pearman (2005) found that
CD ROMs assisted children's decoding of words and aided
self-confidence in pronunciation, while the self-selection features of
programs meant children were in control of their own learning.
Disadvantages were that children became dependent on electronic features
for decoding, they were distracted by hot spots or gaming features not
linked to the storyline, and there was often frustration with electronic
features if there was slowness in animations or in the transfer of
electronic pages. More recently web-based programs seem to have taken
over from CD ROMs, although the quality of many of these varies.
Presumably these will improve with the sophistication of technology and
with the possibilities of podcasting and e-books.
In this project we worked with teachers in five schools, who
selected small groups of students to be reading digital texts. The focus
of the study was to examine how the students were reading digital texts
and whether an understanding of visual grammar contributed to the
reading process with digital texts. Due to the highly visual nature of
digital texts it was thought that drawing students' attention to
the visual devices used by the creators of digital texts would better
equip them to meet the reading/viewing challenges of digital texts. By
giving teachers and students a shared metalanguage it was hoped that
clear discussion of digital texts would also be facilitated.
Research design
The methodology chosen was a case study in three Stage 3 primary
classrooms involving sessions with teachers and observations of small
groups of students. Five teachers, working in pairs where possible,
volunteered for the project and met several times with the researchers.
The study developed in three stages:
Stage 1: One of the researchers observed and videotaped students as
they read and navigated digital texts that teachers were incorporating
for research within specific curriculum areas of English, Science and
HSIE. These were all non-fiction sites that included an author study of
Chris van Allsburg, a study of Antarctica, work on the theme of
'Built environments and Bridges', and research on the
discovery of gold in Australia (see References for these websites).
Stage 2: Teachers participated in a session with the researchers on
both Luke and Freebody's four 'reading practices' (2002)
and ways of reading digital texts through the metalanguage of visual
grammar (Kress & van Leeuwen, 1996). Terms were explained such as
use of colour, angles, perspective framing, salience, vectors, reading
pathway, given and new, demand and offer. Teachers were encouraged to
then teach this visual grammar in lessons with their students using
digital texts.
Stage 3: After teachers indicated that they had been using these
approaches in their teaching, one of the researchers returned to the
same classrooms, observed and videotaped students reading and navigating
different websites (see References for sites on Anzacs, Life Saving, and
authors Roald Dahl and Libby Hathorn).
In classroom observations and analysis of video data, the
researchers were searching for answers to the following two questions:
* What specific aspects of 'reading' were evident in
students' processing of digital texts?
* Did the students' 'reading' and navigation of
digital texts change after the teachers were given professional
development on the metalanguage of visual grammar?
In the analysis of data, the researchers examined student
interactions with digital texts using the following criteria:
* Evidence of aspects of the 'reading process' using Luke
and Freebody's (2002) four reading practices of coding, semantic,
pragmatic and critical practice. Luke and Freebody's (2002)
criteria are well established as covering all aspects of the reading
process with conventional texts. It is a relevant model as it is not
tied to one particular pedagogy for teaching reading but looks at the
different practices required for reading, including decoding, different
levels of comprehension as well as critical and social responses to
texts.
* Evidence of specific ICT skills that were needed and used as
students interacted with digital texts. These are discussed separately
in order to consider the level of reading response that students engaged
in, and to consider how similar or different reading practices might be
when combined with the ICT skills that are needed when engaging with
digital texts compared with traditional texts.
* Students' use of the metalanguage of visual grammar after
the researchers' session with teachers and the teachers'
subsequent classroom work on visual grammar.
The analysis is comparative and interpretive. Findings in relation
to the two questions are now discussed in relation to 'Stage
1' and 'Stage 3' of the study.
Discussion of findings
Analysis of Stage 1: Students' reading practices with websites
In each classroom teachers had provided students with questions and
tasks, akin to 'web quests', that would lead them in their
research using the different websites. Similar patterns were evident in
each of the classes with most responses being evidence of coding,
semantic or pragmatic practice combined with ICT skills needed for the
tasks. There was no evidence found of critical practice or of the use of
any metalanguage related to visual grammar or an awareness of how the
visuals were constructing meaning. Samples of students' responses
in relation to the reading practices and use of ICT skills are
summarised in Table 1 then discussed.
Evidence of coding practice
As students navigated each website they needed to decode information on search engines or a home page to find the information
they needed and to know where to go from the home page. There were
different types of decoding needed as a website requires use of images,
icons, hyperlinks, navigation of the home page and links to other pages.
Students found out that typing and spelling the URL correctly was an
essential coding practice for using a search engine. Other associated
skills were using keyboard shortcuts, using drop down boxes and using
the 'Home' link/icon, using 'highlight, copy and
paste' to put unknown words into a search engine, clicking up/down
arrows to scroll text, minimising or maximising pages/windows,
scrolling, clicking, reading the code of coloured bars across the bottom
of a screen, moving to a new site through logo/banner, changing volume
settings, using 'back' icon, using delete key as a short cut
to go back, identifying the end of a page, using the properties of
'right click' and moving between open windows using a task
bar. Students who were working together in pairs or small groups read
aloud to each other. Some students used their fingers to trace along the
screen or others would point to where they wanted another student to
click on the mouse. Often students would spell words to each other when
using search engines. These could all be described as 'coding
practice', since the students were involved in decoding each
website text and learning its patterns and conventions.
Decoding of digital texts therefore involved the integration of ICT
skills with the decoding of words, images and graphics. The use of key
words to search in this context differs from searching using key words
in a book. An important question emerged for the researchers: Does this
process involve the traditional reading practice of decoding combined
with ICT skills? Or does the reading of digital texts such as websites
involve a new way of reading? If this is a new way of reading, as
ongoing research suggests, how do we describe and theorise this process?
Decoding was occurring differently in the way images were used on
screen. The salience of images and graphics was significant in the way
screens were accessed and navigated. Although at this stage students or
teachers were not using any metalanguage related to the visuals, it was
apparent they were led to particular aspects on a screen depending on
the colour, size, animation, and/or positioning to locate particular
information. Sometimes enlarging images was of benefit to students and
students were attracted by the colourful bars of an index.
Students' navigation of all sites demonstrated the salience of
hyperlinks, thus showing the crucial importance of these links as part
of the reading of digital texts. This occurred particularly with the
Antarctica site, which had many links from the home page to different
types of information. In observing student exploration of all sites, it
was apparent that images were usually more salient as were hyperlinks,
well known images, or using the first hit on the list. Sometimes a
familiar cover or a book would become salient if presented on a screen.
Movement or animation also became salient: as a student commented in one
instance, 'Look at the little feet wiggling'. Often students
expected movement of some images or graphics and would comment:
'It's not moving' as they moved the mouse over an image.
Evidence of semantic practice
Drawing on background knowledge, different levels of understanding,
and making intertextual links are part of semantic practice. In this
study students' semantic practice was evoked not just by text, but
also by images, text, hyperlinks and other digital features. Semantic
practice was evident in students' comments during their navigation
and use of websites. For example, in the site for author Chris van
Allsburg, students answered literal questions about the author with
information from the text and talked about their own experience of the
author and his books. Engagement with the texts caused students to call
to mind previous experiences of texts or to make intertextual links. For
example when viewing the Antarctica site the image on the screen
prompted a student to react and tell a story beginning with the comment,
'Ooh. They're in the frozen ice. [Shiver] Ooh. Just imagine
falling in that ice and something wrapping around your leg. I'd
scream.'
Students' comments showed they could scan a site for relevant
information and that they could locate relevant key words on a web page
to correlate with questions they had been given, choosing information
from a site to copy down or put into their own words. Alternatively they
would reject sites that seemed irrelevant, as shown in comments such as,
'We clicked on a site that didn't say anything about
tourism,' or 'I don't think that's something we
need.' Many students found it difficult to understand how to refine
a search. Students scanned 'hit lists' of search engines for
relevant information but would commonly use the first hit on the list,
just working from top to bottom, suggesting literal understanding
prompted by the salience of the first item on the list. At other times
students would choose which sites from a hit list to visit. Inferring
was evident when students decided on relevance of information and
rejected sites. For example, with the Bridge site, as students scanned
'hit lists' they were refining search criteria with comments
such as, 'This one might be more about materials they use'. In
some instances students made connections between a book read and
relevant websites on a search engine hit list.
Images influenced students' interpretations. In the Gold
website, the images provided a large amount of information about the
size and shape of gold nuggets. Children decided that this site was
'a good one' because the images were highly realistic. In this
site students looked more closely at images and commented on images
regarding their interpretation of the issue it was illustrating.
Students used pictures and labels to gather information--for example:
'The sun comes up at 10.40am'--and were also able to
generalise: 'Most days the sun rises at 10.30am.' Students
made connections between labels and images: 'That's what it
looks like now,' or made comparisons between pictures: 'It
hasn't changed all day.' In one case, one student wanted to
write the comment: 'It's windy there,' whereas the other
child asked 'How can you tell? It's not moving.' These
comments reveal the students were using the images to infer, drawing
conclusions or correcting a conclusion. Students were using picture/text
correlation to make inferences about the features of bridges and
responded to the modality in the colour of images to gauge
relevance/usefulness of the site for gathering information about the
appearance of certain bridges and features. Students used diagrams and
labels to gather literal information as diagrams triggered prior
knowledge of structures and construction. For example, it prompted one
child to recount a science program he had been watching. Students were
inferring/generalising using information from a number of diagrams.
Images were significant in the Chris van Allsburg site. The use of
an image of one of his picture book covers on the site became a salient
part of the home page and prompted one student to ask: 'Is he an
artist or an author?' This showed that information was being
gathered from the image as much as from the written text. Students'
oral responses showed a clear enjoyment of the experience. Boys viewing
the Chris Van Allsburg site expressed their enjoyment of the task
through comments such as: 'Cool sculptures.' One student asked
another student for his opinion of sculptures then offered his own
interpretation. However, as stated previously, there was no evidence
through all our observations of the students evaluating or critiquing
the construction of the websites.
Evidence of pragmatic practice
The understanding of the social purpose of texts is part of
pragmatic practice. The research-based activities set by teachers
afforded opportunities for students to use digital texts for purposes
other than those specifically set by the teacher while supporting their
work. For example, students used Google definition searches to locate
the meaning of unfamiliar words from web pages, to help them make sense
of the digital text they were reading. Students used technical words
from diagram labels to create their own written text explaining features
of the topic they were investigating, such as bridges.
Students gave their opinion of a site based on what they had
expected to see or how they expected to be able to interact with the
page and shared their discoveries with other groups, as was shown in
comments such as 'It's to the right side of the page,'
'Let's go to the Home,' 'Click on Science,' or
'We found something called fact files on the left.' Other
pragmatic practices that arose from the use of digital texts included
those that were an extension of the set reading/viewing or were somehow
prompted by the set activity. Some students participated in the game
playing aspect of a site.
Analysis of Stage 3: Students' use of visual grammar when
reading websites
As a result of the explicit explanations of visual grammar from
their teachers, students seemed more aware of the constructedness of the
digital texts they viewed. Their understanding of visual concepts
underpinned their discussions although their confidence with terminology
was still developing. There was a marked increase in the incidence of
metalanguage use during conversation with the teacher, researcher and
each other about their work. Students used metalanguage such as
'layouts, centred, left to right, top to bottom, symbol, vectors,
line, salience' during discussion of what had been learnt. This
metalanguage did not occur very often as part of students' own
discussions about websites, however. Their talk was more focussed on the
task of locating information. When asked, they could apply their
knowledge of visual grammar to the page they were viewing but the use of
metalanguage was more artificial at this stage of their learning. Table
2 shows examples that were typical of the range of responses.
As the responses in Table 2 show, students' repertoire of
metalanguage included some visual grammar terminology. They were
beginning to break the design code of the text and in the process they
were using semantic, pragmatic practice and some aspects of critical
practice as they looked more closely at how meanings were conveyed
through a visual, digital text.
Two girls were able to explain why an aspect of a digital text was
salient, although they did not use the term 'salience'. They
used their knowledge of size of font and colours that attracted their
attention. Children's use of left to right, top to bottom layouts
and salience was observable as they 'read' the texts they were
using. This seemed to be unconscious on their part. They were able to
describe these visual aspects when scaffolded through questioning by the
researcher. When questioned about layout by a researcher, two boys were
able to explain the layout of a page they were viewing: 'It's
left to right because it has the links on the left and the writing on
the right.' Another student commented: 'This one is top to
bottom. And it has pictures that are hyperlinks.' The second part
of the comment shows the extreme salience of hyperlinks that quickly
attract the attention of students, although they are not recognising the
manner in which such aspects guide their reading of the digital text at
this stage of their learning.
Teacher input varied from direct instruction of visual grammar
terminology to teachers using their new knowledge of visual grammar in
their choice of texts and activities for students. The second round of
observations showed that students who were given direct instruction on
visual grammar not only began to use the metalanguage, but were also
becoming more aware of the constructedness of texts. This change was
revealed in comments such as a comment on layout: 'This page has
lots of lines separating the paragraphs. So it's much easier to
read. If it was just like that you wouldn't be able to see it
properly;' and a comment on the effect of top/bottom and left/right
layouts: 'It's a lot easier doing it like that than having
hyperlinks in the middle of nowhere or just at the top having heaps more
because then it would be really confusing.' Other comments showed
awareness of the authors or constructors of a website, for example:
'Whoever constructed this website ...', and 'The creators
made the web page top to bottom (layout) and they used movement, like
that, and they used clear, bright colours to attract your
attention.' When students were talking about the hyperlinks and the
movement on the Roald Dahl, they compared them with other websites:
'It makes you in control of the website,' 'We can do what
we want ... you can choose what you want to go to,' and 'It
makes it more interesting to search around yourself.' Students had
some time to begin developing their own website designs. This made them
more aware of aspects of layout, colour and salience, as shown in
comments in Table 3.
Developing a description of digital reading practices
Using Luke and Freebody's framework of four reading practices
(2002) was helpful in this study to analyse the different ways of
reading that students engaged in as they responded to digital texts and
to compare students' responses in Stage 3 with those in Stage 1.
Observation of students throughout the study has prompted the
researchers to apply these observations to Luke and Freebody's
framework as shown in Table 3.
Table 3 presents an initial attempt to suggest a framework for the
reading practices that occur in the reading of digital texts. This
framework is principally focusing on the reading of websites and does
not include sound or movement that occurs in some sites and in other
digital texts such as e-literature, film or gaming. There is much that
needs to be further researched in this area, particularly to what extent
the metalanguage of visual grammar, and ongoing research in this area by
others, can be applied to the reading of digital texts.
Conclusion
Although this small study does not offer enough evidence to be
generalisable, it offers insights that contribute to the corpus of
knowledge about the reading of digital texts. The study revealed that
students in the study were highly motivated to work with digital texts
and they were able to use basic ICT skills to navigate sites. However,
their reading responses and understanding seemed to be at a literal,
often superficial, level with little evidence of inferring, evaluating
or critical reading. Several questions arise from these observations.
Are we as educators assuming that students will transfer reading
practices for the reading of print-based texts to digital texts? Do
these reading practices need to be more explicitly taught with digital
texts? Do teachers know how to do this? Is there a different way of
teaching needed for processing digital modes? These are all questions
that need further investigation.
Even though students of today are growing up in the midst of
sophisticated digital communication, they need a great deal of
understanding of the messages and knowledge being created through
different modes of communication, as well as the ability to discriminate
and critique these messages. The small scope of the study did not
provide enough evidence to support our premise that the teaching of the
metalanguage of visual grammar would assist the reading of digital
texts. There is much more research needed to understand whether the
reading of digital texts requires a new theory of reading and, if so,
how pedagogy needs to be developed to incorporate the reading of digital
texts within the curriculum
Acknowledgement
We wish to acknowledge and express our appreciation to the teachers
and students who participated in this project. We thank the Catholic
Education Office Parramatta, the School of Education (NSW), and the
Australian Catholic University, for their contribution to this research.
References
Asha, J. (2005). Asnapshot of three teachers' classroom
practices integrating ICT and children's literature. In L.
Unsworth, A. Thomas, A. Simpson, & J. Asha, Children's
literature and computer-based teaching (pp. 89-105). Maidenhead, Berks.:
Open University Press.
Callow, J., & Zammitt, K. (2002). Visual literacy: From picture
book to electronic texts. In M. Monteith (Ed.), Teaching primary
literacy with ICT (pp. 189-201). Buckingham: Open University Press.
Clay, M. M. (1979). The early detection of reading difficulties
(2nd ed.). Auckland: Heinemann.
Department of Education, Science and Training (DEST). (2005).
Teaching reading. Canberra, ACT: Commonwealth Government of Australia.
Doty, D. E., Popplewell, S. R., & Byers, G. O. (2001).
Interactive CD-ROM storybooks and young readers' reading
comprehension. Journal of Research on Computing in Education, 33,
374-384.
Gee, J. (2003). What video games have to teach us about learning
and literacy. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Kress, G., & Van Leeuwen, T. (1996). Reading images: The
grammar of visual design. London: Routledge.
Kress, G., & Van Leeuwen, T. (2001). Multimodal discourse.
London: Routledge.
Kress, G. (2003). Literacy in the new media age. London: Routledge.
Lankshear, C., Snyder, I., & Green, B. (2000). Teachers and
technoliteracy. Sydney: Allen & Unwin.
Lankshear, C., & Knobel, M. (2003). New literacies changing
knowledge and classroom learning. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Lefever-Davis, S., & Pearman, C. (2005). Early readers and
electronic texts: CD-ROM storybook features that influence reading
behaviors. The Reading Teacher, 58(5), 446-454.
Lemke, J. (2002). Travels in hypermodality. Visual communication,
1(3), 229-325.
Luke, A., & Freebody, P. (2002). Further Notes on the Four
Resources Model. Reading Online. Retrieved Oct. 5, 2006, from
http://www.readingonline.org/research/lukefreebody.html
Mathews, K. I. (1996). The impact of CD-ROM storybooks on
children's reading comprehension and attitude. Journal of Education
Multimedia and Hypermedia, 5, 379-394.
Noad, B. (2005). Reading multimodal texts (PEN 149). Sydney: PETA.
Simpson, A. (2004). Visual literacy: A coded language for viewing
in the classroom (PEN 142). Sydney: PETA.
Turbill, J. (2003). Exploring the potential of the digital language
experience approach in Australian classrooms. Reading Online. Retrieved
14 Oct., 2006, from http://www.readingonline.org/international/inter_index.asp? HREF=/international/turbill7/index.html
Unsworth, L. (2001). Teaching multiliteracies across the
curriculum: Changing contexts of text and image in classroom practice.
Buckingham: Open University Press.
Unsworth, L. (2003). Re-framing research and literacy relating to CD ROM narratives: Addressing 'radical change' in digital age
literature for children. Issues in Educational Research, 13(2), 55-70.
Unsworth, L., Thomas, A., & Bush, R. (2004). The role of images
and image-text relations in group 'basic skills tests' of
literacy for children in the primary years. Australian Journal of
Language and Literacy, 27(1), 46-65.
Walsh, M. (2006). The 'textual shift': Examining the
reading process with print, visual and multimodal texts. Australian
Journal of Language and Literacy, 29(1), 25-37.
Websites accessed
www.rochedalss.qld.edu.au/anzac.htm www.anzacs.net/AnzacStory.htm
www.dimdima.com/science/science.asp www.royallifesaving.com.au/
www.lifesaving.com.au/home/home.cfm www.anzacsite.gov.au/
www.wiltonlibrary.org/kids/science.asp www.roalddahlmuseum.org/ (Roald
Dahl author site) www.matsuo-bridge.co.jp/english/bridges/index.shtm
(bridge site) www.chrisvanallsburg.com (Chris van Allsburg author site)
www.aad.gov.au/ (Antarctica site) www.libbyhathorn.com/lh/index.asp
(Libby Hathorn author site)
Table 1. Observations Stage 1.
Reading Practices observed Examples of students' comments
Coding practice 'See what's there'; 'it comes up
Involved searching, scrolling, itself'; 'we'll go back to the
pointing to menu bars and key back file thing'; 'look--
words, using 'back' command to 'transport' is there'; 'nothing on
navigate between screens, reading that page, or that'; 'I want to
aloud to each other, point to, see that bit'.
tracing on screen with finger
while identifying the titles or
words for their 'topic'.
Semantic practice 'I chose this one because it said
Involved using key words, locating about him being a governor'; 'we
information, identifying main read the summary below the web
ideas, obtaining facts, a little address and it looked like what we
use of interpreting, inferencing. were looking for'. 'This site
might be more about materials they
use'. Students identified
'triangles' in bridge diagram and
identified this with aspects of
structure.
Pragmatic practice 'We searched Google'; 'I want to
Used 'Google' & other search see what's on that screen'. 'Let's
engines; online dictionaries; go to the home page'. 'Look at the
navigating between sites; feet wiggling'. Students used
enlarging images; printing print out of one web page to
sections. Attracted by animation. compare with another site.
Critical practice No examples.
Table 2. Observations Stage 3.
Reading Practices
observed Examples of students' comments
Coding Practice 'Lines are used'; 'there are bright colours';
'this one is top to bottom'; 'it's left to right
because it has links on the left and writing on
the right'; 'it has pictures that are
hyperlinks'.
Semantic Practice 'The picture shows us what sort of materials the
buildings made out of'.
Pragmatic Practice 'This page has lots of lines separating the
paragraphs. So it's much easier to read. If it
was just like that you wouldn't be able to read
it properly'. Students began creating their own
web pages and PowerPoints: 'we've been making our
own web pages and using top to bottom and left
to right layout and vectors'.
Critical Practice 'The creators made the web page top to bottom
[layout] and they used movement and they used
clear, bright colours to attract your attention'.
'It makes you in control of the website'; 'We can
do what we want ... choose where we want to go';
'The heading is the most salient thing on the
page because the creator wants to emphasise the
name of the author'.
Table 3. Digital reading practices: Evidence derived from the study.
Coding Practices Semantic Practices
operational ICT skills, scanning, exploring web texts for a specific
scrolling--or 'concepts of screen' purpose, understanding, inferring
(Turbill, 2003) using a search engine--key words
decoding text, images, diagrams, interpreting symbolic meanings
graphics acquiring info through visual &
identifying symbolic modes: e.g., multimedia elements
icons, hyperlinks, use of colour, background knowledge of digital
animation texts understanding inter-textual
links
Pragmatic Practices Critical Practices
using digital texts for related critically evaluating web based
tasks: e.g., Google definitions, resources: authenticity, currency,
use of technical language for reliability, credibility
diagrams comparing the construction of
graphics, hyperlinks different sites considering
comparing different websites 'author', 'audience', purpose
relating texts to other known detecting underlying bias, point
internet texts of view, ideologies
creating new texts: web pages, Understanding how visual codes
PowerPoints with digital photos, (colour, framing, angles,
movie clips, e-texts salience, vectors, etc.)
construct meaning