The resonance of children: educating the literacy educator.
Latham, Gloria
Long, long ago in the Dreamtime there was this huge ball of flaming
fire even bigger than the sun and then there was a shooting star
that came along and bumped into it and made it glow. Then all these
little asteroids came off of it and they had lava in the middle and
rocks formed in the earth's centre and some of the rocks were wet
and some of them were dry and some had glowing heat inside them. A
huge comet smashed through and a planet blew up ...
This story of the world's beginning began to unfold alongside
hundreds of others as I journeyed into the physical and spiritual world
of five- to seven-year-olds. What follows are some of the pedagogical and moral understandings gathered as a literacy educator, a role I have
assumed for well over 25 years.
My qualitative phenomenological study was a search to better
understand what the world is like for children, van Manen (1990)
suggests that phenomenological research is an attempt to describe and
interpret the essential meaning of a phenomenon. The acknowledged
founder of phenomenology, Husserl (1931, 1933) saw reality as
consciousness, and in order to achieve consciousness he believed the
researcher had to 'return to the things themselves.' A
phenomenological approach also asks the researcher to lay aside what is
known and understood about the phenomenon in order to enter the
experience as a stranger. The world is rendered paradoxical and as such
it is necessary to work towards being utterly lost in the experience.
The comfort of familiar contexts becomes less familiar, eliciting an
element of surprise and allowing the teacher/researcher to act in new
ways. It was my intention to move towards new embodied meanings of how
children experience the world in order to alter my teaching practice and
also to inform and encourage change in the pre-service teachers with
whom I work.
Colin Lankshear (1997) reminds us that while change is occurring
all around us, and at many levels, many areas of education fail to
change at all. 'It's business as usual' (p. 1). Almost
fifty years ago, Dewey forewarned educators about fixed metaphors of
teaching and asked them to break through the crust of conventionalised
consciousness and return to common sense thinking, as they can be easily
'lulled into an intellectual apathy ...' (p. 183). Arendt
(1958), Bauer and Varnice (1992) and Greene (1995), among others, have
also warned of the dangers inherent in adhering to fixed metaphors of
teaching.
There have been many forces taking up the struggle to enact
productive change. For instance, some researchers (Denzin, 1997;
Josselson, 1996; Richardson, 1997, 2000) are attempting to readdress
pedagogical issues and alter the moral and emotional aspects of their
work by using a reflexive approach. Further, a number of educators and
theorists (Damasio, 1999; Arnold, 2000) are examining and applying
empathic moral education employing reflexive means.
In my study traditional notions of teaching were problematised and
revisioned. I worked with five groups of between three to five children
on a daily basis over a three and a half month period. The study
required me to lay aside aspects of the traditional teacher's role
and become a wanderer who entered the 'lived world' of
children. In this position I was afforded the opportunity to stand apart
and examine my teaching with renewed clarity; to be led rather than to
lead. The prospect of surrendering to the experience under investigation
appeared daunting. My teacher education course at university was
traditional in nature, defining the teacher as someone skilled in
controlling classroom events and who functions as the main dispenser of
knowledge. The fear was not of losing my sense of control while working
with the phenomenon, but rather of being out of control.
What was reaffirmed during the research process was my long-held
belief that young children are robust philosophical thinkers.
Children's natural curiosity about their surroundings renders their
world problematic and therefore purposeful. Five- to seven-year-olds are
in a highly formative time of life, making it most worthy of
investigation. Although a plethora of research studies has been amassed
on infancy and the first three or four years of life, there are far
fewer studies of children in the five- to seven-year age-range. Yet this
is a time when children begin to take on the stories, beliefs and
observances that symbolise belonging to their communities, at a time
when they formulate and consolidate their beliefs. It is therefore a
pivotal time in children's lives. My research study examined
children's theories at a time when they were relatively tentative.
The children I worked alongside of were immensely curious about the
world and their place within it. As philosophers (lovers of wisdom),
young children are tilled with wonderment. It also seems that children
move with their entire sensory system in motion to help answer the
questions that puzzle them. Connected to the cosmos, they move in-step,
and in-tune with their surrounds. As I worked to enter their life world
they appeared to ask questions to move their wondering outside of their
heads; to verbalise and/or draw ideas about questions, so that that they
might one day arrive at the answers. They did not appear to ask
questions in order to have them answered. The following is an example of
Peter's wondering.
I wonder why we have shadows? When I'm looking upwards over the
fence I can see the light over the fence, it's going downward over
the fence. And when I think about it, it's going down, down, down
under the Earth way, way down under the Earth.
Here is a six-year-old posing a question and then meandering in the
wonder of it. Once questions such as Peter's surfaced, I turned
them back to the group of children, allowing them time to speculate on
possible explanations. Finklestein and Ritter (1990) conducted research
to learn whether or not answering a child's question with a
question produced further analytical questioning. Their findings
indicated that although furthering children's questions with a
question did not produce an increase in the number of questions posed,
it did increase children's ability to ask higher-level questions.
Theory building and testing
Given time, children happily roam around in robust theories of
meta-physical thought, while listening attentively to the thoughts of
others and often adjusting their theories. I began to believe that
adults who provide answers to children's questions contribute to
the demise of wondering as children are soon taught that they are devoid
of wisdom. When they are supplied with answers, they may believe they
have been offered fixed 'truths' and there is no longer need
to speculate about life's mysteries.
The children frequently referred to their ideas as theories such
as:
This is just a theory but I believe ...
My theory is ...
Pavlov imagined theories as sets of wings. He told scholars that
theories allow humans to rise to the heavens. But he considered facts to
be the atmosphere against which those wings beat, and without which the
soaring bird would plunge back to earth. On the other hand, Pollock
(1942) likened the making of a theory to the making a map. It is not
discovering new territory, but rather analysing already existing
relationships. Children are theory builders from a very young age, and
their development in cognitive processes is identical to the cognitive
processes developed in scientists. Children develop their intuitive
theories of the world in the context of a society that already has much
knowledge of the world. Central to this notion of theory building is
that the information children build about the world is derived from
sources of authority rather than evidence, interpretation and
explanation.
Talking oneself into meaning
The spoken word was a powerful means to elicit the children's
fragile theories. Oral discourse is thought of as weaving or stitching.
While meaning is being stitched a metamorphosis occurs and newer
meanings are created. The children had particular ways of expressing
language that informed their meaning making. For instance, many used
hesitations, um's and ah's and repetitions to help move their
thoughts further and allow them thinking time. They often backed up and
circled an idea in order to ultimately snare it. In the following
example, it appears that Cal's beliefs were being framed as they
were spoken:
I think um I think that when I think about um the World like
sometimes I think about ... well sometimes I think about um like um
kind of like um, it's a bit hard to understand, but sometimes when
um I'm really interested in something like um I keep on asking
questions like what really happened in the olden days? Like I kind
of think that it [the first person in the world] was a gorilla and
it changed into a person.
Often the children's thoughts were not caught and remained in
the air unresolved even to the speaker. Will said, 'I can't
even understand my words because Adam was the first person in the world
and how did they make him?' Some children returned time and time
again to a previous thought after the discussion had moved on. For
instance five-year-old Will said:
I'm still thinking about those first people who came alive first
and the first person was Bubba Jesus when he married one of the
cherubs and they made some children and then they (the children)
became grown-ups and they made a girl and boy and it keeps going on,
girls then boys ...
During the conversations, the children were gathering and
assembling vast amounts of information in order to understand the world.
They used everything at their disposal to assist them: the volume and
timbre of their voices, repetition for emphasis, hand and facial
gestures, role play, drawings, diagrams, music and rich expressive
language. They were naturally adept at using multimodal approaches to
learning. When the words they went in search of could not be found they
thought nothing of inventing words that suited:
We live in treasurous times.
There were wee little bits of rain, like little skimmers of them.
First there was this wiggly, oogley stuff, that went into frogs and
they turned into monkeys.
Before mankind the Earth was all crackled up ...
Words tumbled out of them in quick succession, then, once outside
themselves, the words began to be ordered and thought upon. It is
documented that Heidegger (1929, 1949, 1962), the German
phenomenologist, also used language to think with. Rather than forming
clear and decisive thoughts for his readers, it appears that Heidegger
was attempting to make sense of his own thoughts. In this fashion he was
always on the way towards understanding rather than having arrived.
There is something about the fragility of thought that I understood to
be important to preserve as a literacy educator: my own thoughts and
those of my students.
During the collection of children's thoughts, I felt it
essential that children discuss their ideas in small groups, affirming
my belief that knowledge is a consensus. Dialogical learning is an
exchange between people seeking mutual agreement and understanding.
The public activity the children in the study engaged in also
provided them with a small audience for their ideas. They assumed a
variety of roles and in so doing they took on suitable language
registers. To illustrate, the following extract is from a child acting
as a reporter and commenting on the future:
This is reporter Clemmy (five-years-old) on live news. I'm here to
report about the year 2020. The days will go longer and the year
will go close to us in the future. And in the past there hasn't been
a lot of nature. Only some trees are left only a tree in every
garden but there are no other trees. And nature will be coming back
in the year nineteen hundred and fifty thousand which is quite short
but long for us to wait.
Appropriating a variety of roles and contexts required the children
to think in more expansive ways.
Yet the colourful children's stories and songs often meandered
in terms of the meanings expressed and at times the rhythm took
precedence. Thus, it was necessary for me to ask for further
clarification, and to try, to follow the slippery trails of the rhythms
in order to bring back their messages as one unified voice.
The children appeared to be most comfortable when framing
scientific ideas as stories. They utilised a wide range of narrative
devices to. entice their listeners:
Well let me tell you how it happened.
A long, long time ago before anybody was alive ...
And that's my story from Clemmy to the World.
And then what happened is ...
Narrative devices were familiar to them and assisted their meaning
making. Although these stories were often entertaining and revealing, it
was easy to be lulled by the narratives, pulled into the dramas they
created without attending to the messages unfolding.
The art of listening
It was critical to listen attentively to the children and pay heed to the ideas expressed. Implicit in the notion of learning from children
is the ability to listen to them in valued ways. I learned to wait
patiently and allow time to let their stories surface. Silence was not
space to be filled, but rather concealed thought brimming with
potential. Bollnow (1982) believes that speech rises out of silence and
returns to silence. It needs to be listened to. I also needed to
recognise the great chasm that exists between oneself and others in
terms of mood, thoughts, work habits, and orientation to the world.
Thus, my understanding of children's ideas about the world might
differ considerably from their understanding.
They seldom composed thoughts in their heads, and therefore needed
a great deal of time and patience from their listeners to allow them to
wander in thought and ultimately arrive at ideas and shape them. After
observing these children absorbed in the creation of a series of
group-devised maps of the world, I recognised the enormous potential of
listening and thinking on paper. Generally, the children's drawings
and diagrams preceded their verbal explanations.
Arts researchers have long been investigating art as a means
towards the expression of feelings. Many of the children's drawings
were indeed expressions of their sensory impressions of the world
coloured in emotions. I hoped to understand the children's empathic
relationship with the world, their world knowledge, paper and pens,
along with the verbal exchanges that seemed to enhance their
experiences. To assist active listening, I draped the table with large
sheets of paper. The children would enter the space, seat themselves,
pick up a drawing implement and someone would ask, 'What should we
talk about today?' I would ask what they would like to discuss and
off they went. Drawing assisted active listening. Although their heads
were often lowered to the page, their ears were attuned to the speaker.
At times the children's hands made no marks on the clean surface
but they remained in readiness, almost trance-like. I wrote about this
experience.
Listening hands
A child is drawing, head down, curled into the paper's whiteness.
Arms and shoulders shield the image.
The pencil is poised yet motionless.
There is an alertness in the body,
as a gentle swaying from the spine
Lifting the body upright.
Chin then eyes, gaze up at the speaker.
Burgeoning thoughts bubble forth
into an uttered response.
'That's an interesting idea, but do you think it's possible that the
earth could have formed another way?'
Drawings were also used for clarifying ideas. When any member of
the group was not certain about what was being expressed, the speaker
would grab a pen or a pencil to assist in clarifying his or her ideas.
This was especially apparent when discussing complex notions, such as
gravity or the life cycle.
Moving beyond scripts
However, even with these methods in place, I had to reconcile
notions that many of the ideas the children related had been simply
handed down to them by others. It was necessary to help them move beyond
their set 'scripts', while still recognising their importance.
In order to do this, I questioned their statements in some depth. For
instance, most of the children knew that we don't fall off the
world because of gravity. Although they freely used the term
'gravity' I felt it was necessary to try and understand their
deeper understanding of how gravity operated. I returned in a subsequent
session and asked the children to draw a diagram and explain gravity.
Some of the drawings helped the children go beyond the set information
they had been provided with by others. Anna was trying to understand
gravity for herself as she explained, 'First I have to understand
how the apple falls and then I can begin to understand gravity'.
In a further instance, the analysis of the children's
discussions appeared to suggest their strong belief that the world is
alive and therefore feels joy and pain. I tried to deepen my
understanding of their beliefs as the children were asked to describe
the earth as their body. Six-year-old Anna, related it this way:
The Moon is my best friend Winnie
My body is divided into parts; north, south, east and west
but all the parts are connected.
The land is my mouth
and my hair is the food I eat.
Part of my brain is the clouds
and another part is past time
where my memories are kept.
My kidneys are the sea that pours from me.
My heart is the Moon's reflection
because the Moon is my best friend Winnie.
The closest stars would be my parents.
My veins are the trees
and when the trees stop growing,
my blood stops circulating.
There are dangers of war and pollution
in my eyes and running through my arms.
Pollution might break me
but my worst enemy is the Sun,
hot and fierce ...
This is an example of a child wrestling with complex and abstract
concepts. The children I worked with appeared eager to be offered these
mental challenges. I was reminded of the relevance of the Productive
Pedagogies Curriculum in Queensland and the Thinking Curriculum in
Victoria that seek to foster higher- level thinking. Talking about the
world provided opportunity to extend and challenge the children's
scientific and philosophical ideas. The children generally framed ideas
about the world from creationist and evolutionist positions handed down
to them:
God created the world, end of story.
Well God was still alive and he made this earth what made dinosaurs
or animals ... Well, I think that millions and millions of years
ago, before heaven was even invented, before all the planets and
stars and the moon and the sun were around, there came a small ball
of fire ... The world was created when a meteor hit down.
The following discussion shows one instance when the
children's positioned constructs came into contact with alternative
constructs and the blending of creationist and evolutionist thinking
occurred. A small group of children and I were seated at a table,
discussing how the first person came into the world. The question,
generated by Cal, started a flurry of wondering:
Anna: The first person is called Bubba Jesus.
Cal: There was no first person alive.
Anna: Okay, what was first?
Cal: Dinosaurs.
Anna: The first person on Earth would have to be a mother, right?
Cal: How do you know that?
Anna: Cause mothers have babies.
Cal: Well if the first person was a mother, how did the first
person get there?
Anna: Well, we don't even know how we can get the people on
the Earth cause they are born by a person. They are born by a lady and
then there are babies and babies and babies.
Researcher: I guess my question is, how did the first person get
there? What do you think?
Cal: The first people who came to Earth were dinosaurs.
Researcher: What was after dinosaurs?
Cal: People? God?
Researcher: You think God was after dinosaurs?
Cal: God was after dinosaurs. That's exactly right. God was
after dinosaurs. And then after God was Mother Nature.
Anna: Well have you ever heard that people came from monkeys?
Cal: Yes, first we were baby apes and then we kept changing into
humans. Then we died and then we were born again.
Anna: I thought we were first apes and then girls used to be boys
and we were all furry and then we got un-furry.
Cal: Oh, God, you mean I used to be a girl?
Anna: No, girls used to be boys!
Cal: Oh, that's not fair!
Anna: How did the fur come off?
Cal: We kept moulting and then we only had a little bit left on our
face and hands.
Anna: Well I think the first animal came and then lots of animals
came and then the monkey ate a special algae or something and they
formed into the first settlers.
These interactions afforded the children a wide assortment of
beliefs to hear, examine and debate. The conversations were not about
owning knowledge, but rather they allowed thinking time to explore the
thoughts of others. The interactions also afforded them time to
articulate and examine their own burgeoning beliefs in a safe,
supportive environment.
The thinking climate
Teachers and students must seek to find a clearing, an empty space
(mentally, physically and spiritually) in which all participants can
enter and take delight in a thoughtful, lively, passionate multi-modal
community where the expression of ideas are shared and extended. The
need to feel safe and cared for is paramount.
The researcher has transformed the teacher in me. Positioning
myself as the one who learns from the children, assisted in setting free
many fixed views of teaching while enabling me to better capture the
abundant wisdom each child brought to the community of inquiry. I
understood the need to encourage and be more responsive to tentative
ideas. The lessons learned were potent reminders of the need for a
closer and ongoing link between research and teaching.
I became aware that I could never have received such wisdom had I
asked these children to write their thoughts down. This gave me cause to
wonder whether or not literacy educators focus on writing too early in
children's lives, thus minimising opportunities and diminishing the
value inherent in oral expression for thinking. Perhaps many deep and
impenetrable thoughts are silenced because the mode of expression
offered is insufficient. As well, while socially generated ideas are
essential for ideas to flourish, writing is often a solitary act.
I feel certain that the joy children experienced while sharing
their ideas was due in part to the climate created in the exchange. Some
features of the climate were:
* the children were talking about topics of immense interest to
them if the children lost interest in the particular topic it was
abandoned
* the topics were often framed as scenarios or focused narratives
that describe possible futures and pose alternatives (Rohan & Bigum,
1998)
* the children were able to use a wide range of modalities to
express ideas
* there was unlimited time to explore and tinker with issues
* the children had an eager audience for their thoughts
* the views expressed were listened to and valued
* the children felt trust in the small group that was built up over
time
* the children had permission to roam in the world of possibilities
rather than certainties without fear of being wrong
* the children were respected for their ideas and often praised by
their peers
* theories and collected knowledge were contested, deepened and
extended.
Over time, scholars have been warning of the inherent dangers in
schooling the child. 'Successful' students learn how to
'do school', an act that rewards compliance, neatness,
punctuality and answers which align themselves with those of the
teacher. In this process of appropriating the world presented to them,
some children lose their soul. Dewey (1938) questioned the point of
winning information if children lose their soul, their desire and their
ability to transfer knowledge to future events.
As the schooling process is one of the ways in which imposed world
views are passed on, I wondered how I could reposition myself to spend
more time learning about what my students bring to the circle of
discovery. The notion of how learners are viewed is highly significant
to their preservation. We diminish students' worth by positioning
them as receptacles of knowledge, rather than as beings offering forth
wisdom. As I shared my research stories with parents and staff in the
school in both formal and informal ways, it became apparent that these
significant people were often unaware that their children held deeply
philosophical beliefs about the world and the nature of those beliefs. I
wonder whether children are ever asked what it is they wonder about?
I continue to ponder this question and others. As teachers/
researchers we must continue to discover and celebrate new ways of
artful teaching.
References
Arendt, H. (1958) The human condition. Garden City, New York,
Doubleday Anchor Books.
Arnold, R. (2000)
http://www.usyd.edu.au/su/ctl/Synergy/Synergy14/arnold.htm
Bauer, D.H. & Vannice, C.K. (1992) 'Teaching as the art of
living'. The Educational Forum, 4, 422-432.
Bollnow, O.F. (1982) 'On silence-findings of
philosophico-pedagogical anthropology'. Universitas, 24 (1), 41-47.
Damasio, A.R. (1999) The feeling of what happens: Body emotion in
the making of consciousness. New York, Harcourt Brace.
Denzin, N.K. (1997) Interpretive ethnography: Ethnography practices
for the 21st century. Thousand Oaks, CA, Sage Publication.
Dewey, J. (1954) Art and education: A collection of essays by John
Dewey and others (3rd edn). Merion, PA, Barnes Merion Foundation Press.
Finklestein, J.M. & Ritter, V.P. (1990) Does anyone have any
questions? Department of Health, Education and Welfare, Washington DC,
National Institute of Education.
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A.N. (1997) Words, thoughts and
theories. MA, MIT.
Gopnik, A. & Slaughter, V. (1991) Young children's
understanding of change in mental states. Child Development, 62, 98-110.
Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H.M. (1992). Why the child's theory
of mind is really a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145-171.
Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H.M. (1994) The theory theory. In L. A.
Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain
specificity in cognition and culture. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum (pp.
257-293).
Greene, M. (1995) Releasing the imagination. San Fransisco,
Jossey-Bass.
Heidegger, M. (1927) The question concerning technology. In W.
Lovitt (ed.), The question concerning technology and other essays. New
York: Harper and Row (pp. 3-35).
Heidegger, M. (1949) Existence and being. Chicago, Reginery.
Heidegger, M. (1962) Being and time. Oxford, Basil Blackwell.
Husserl, E. (1913/1931) Ideas: A general introduction to pure
phenomenology. W.R. Boyce Gibson (Trans.) New York, Humanities Press.
Lankshear, C. (1997) Changing literacies. Buckingham, Open
University Press.
Pollock T. (1942) The nature of literature. Princeton, Princeton
University Press.
Rowan, L. & Bigum, C. (1998) The future of technology and
literacy teaching in primary learning situations and contexts. In C.
Lankshear, C. Bigum, et al. Digital rhetorics: Literacies and
technologies in education-Current practices and future directions (Vol.
3.) Children's Literacy National Projects. Brisbane, QUT/ DEETYA.
Josselson, R. (ed.). (1996) Ethics and process in the narrative
study of lives (Vol. 4). Thousand Oaks, CA, Sage.
van Manen, M. (1990) Researching Lived Experience: Human Science
for an 'Action Sensitive Pedagogy. Albany, New York, State
University of New York Press.