Multiple perspectives on early literacy: Staff and parents speak out.
Makin, Laurie ; McNaught, Margaret
Introduction
A number of studies demonstrate the existence of a discrepancy between what many parents want in terms of support for early literacy
and what many early childhood educators believe is appropriate. This
discrepancy often relates to the value placed on school-related skills
such as being able to count and recite the alphabet (Dunn & Kontos
1997, Powell 1996). A lack of shared understandings has been found to
exist in families with a range of different ethnic backgrounds, with
different literacy levels, and across different socioeconomic backgrounds.
Gregory (1994), in a study of Bangladeshi immigrants in the UK,
found major discrepancies between the ways parents believed that
children should become literate, as demonstrated by the methods used in
the home language community schools and the way in which the children
were taught in the first year of school. Some parents did not understand
how children can learn to read by `pretending to read' and did not
accept the value of a play-based approach to early literacy.
Fitzgerald et al. (1991) report that low literacy parents are more
likely to see literacy as a set of skills than as cultural practice, and
it has been noted that low income mothers are less likely to see play as
a learning process. Their home teaching of beginning reading and writing
is less likely to be embedded in everyday literacy events and is
generally less playful in nature. Many middle to high income Asian
parents expect children to learn to read and write at preschool. This is
common in countries such as Hong Kong and Singapore, although it must be
remembered that children begin formal schooling later in these
countries.
Key literacy practices in the years prior to school entry
Diverse researchers such as Snow, Burns & Griffin (1998) and
Clay (1991) agree there is no single literacy learning approach or set
of strategies which is most effective for all children all of the time.
However, there are certain areas of knowledge and skills that appear to
be important. Snow et al. (1998) noted that among those children who
begin school less prepared to learn to read are those who have `less
prior knowledge and skill in certain domains, most notably letter
knowledge, phonological sensitivity, familiarity with the basic purposes
and mechanisms of reading, and language ability' (p.137).
The point at issue is how these skills and knowledge are
appropriately developed in the years prior to school entry. There are
activities and practices that are important for building understandings
about literacy, which are in keeping with the interests and capabilities
of young children and preschoolers, and which can be incorporated into a
prior-to-school program on a daily basis. Makin et al. (1999, pp. 39-40)
summarises these as follows:
* The provision of opportunity for social interaction around
literacy and for adult conversations with individual children about
topics that are meaningful and significant to them. This provides
opportunity for development of the verbal and narrative skills that are
important for later learning and literacy (Snow 1983). The ability of
significant adults (teachers and parents) to provide quality interaction
and feedback to children around literacy and about children's
concepts of print is seen as very supportive of children's learning
to read `naturally' in a similar manner to how they learn to speak
(Teale & Sulzby 1986). However, an increasing number of researchers
(e.g. Heath 1983, Freebody & Ludwig 1995, Gee 1990) point out that
different social practices are differentially valued by teachers, with
the effect that some children's literacy skills, experiences and
knowledge may be marginalised by educational institutions. For example,
oral story-telling, bilingualism and knowledge of popular culture may
not be valued.
* Opportunities to develop decontextualised language. In a
large-scale study of high quality early childhood programs generally
(i.e., without a specific focus on literacy), Epstein (1999, p. 117)
found that `opportunities for children to plan and recall their
activities, as well as children's access to diverse materials, were
the two dimensions of program quality most important for promoting
children's development'. This finding, relating to the
opportunity for talk about the day's activities, could be seen to
link with the views of researchers such as Snow (1983) and Michaels
(1981). They stress the significance of children's use of
`literate' or decontextualised language, which enables them to talk
about objects that are not immediately present. Such talk is related to
ongoing engagement with literacy learning and later successful literacy
development. Children who come to school with different patterns of oral
narrative, for example, those linked implicitly rather than in a linear
and explicit fashion to the particular topic or event, may have
difficulty in a classroom where a mainstream teacher is unaware of this
particular linguistic variation.
* Opportunity to learn more about the sound sequences in words.
This skill is considered to be a necessary, but not sufficient,
precursor of learning to read in an alphabetic orthography. It can be
enhanced through exposure to linguistic awareness games, word plays,
poetry, rhymes and rhythmic word plays from children's own and
other cultures (Jue11991). Adams (1990) suggests that alphabetic
knowledge in the first year of school is an important predictor of later
literacy success.
* Parents and early childhood staff reading aloud to children. This
provides optimal support for development when the children feel secure
(Bus & Van Ijzendoom 1995) and are encouraged to be active
participants making text-to-life and life-to-text connections. Frequency
of story reading is important (Wells 1985). More recent writers suggest
that it may be the talk and interaction that surrounds the reading that
gives this activity its particular significance (Dickinson & Smith
1994). A variety of text types may be important for certain kinds of
language learning.
* Exposure to print. This helps children understand the purposes
and functions of print and the acquisition of a vocabulary with which to
talk about it (meta-literacy skills). This typically occurs when
children hear and see others reading aloud or when someone demonstrates
or talks about what they are doing with print (Raban & Ure 1999).
* Access to an attractive, comfortable and well-stocked book area
in early childhood services. A broad range of text types should be
available, for example, authentic community texts, books in the home
languages of the children in the setting, books catering to the
interests of the children, rhymes, poetry, maps, street directories,
alphabet books and children's and teachers' own created books
(Schickedanz 1999). The intermittent presence of the teacher in the book
corner to read or interact with the children has been shown to increase
children's usage of the corner (Morrow & Weinstein 1982).
* Sociodramatic play. Early childhood services should include many
areas which contain literacy materials such as magazines and an
appointment book at a hairdresser, recipe books for a home comer,
telephone books, notepads and a calendar in a play office, a workshop
manual for car repairer. Teachers may need to mention and model ways in
which these materials may be used at first (Vukelich 1994).
* A writing/drawing table or corner. Here children may experiment
with a variety of writing and drawing implements and perhaps read, play
with and draw logos and signs, and develop familiarity with writing both
independently and in interaction with his/her peers and with adults. A
great deal of informal tutoring is typically embedded in these
interactions (Schickedanz 1999).
* Congruence between families' and teachers' views.
Establishing shared goals for young children's language and
literacy development and shared understanding of ways to achieve those
goals seems important (Cairney 1994) because it offers teachers and
parents multiple pathways towards the goal of effective literacy
development for all children. A growing emphasis in the literature (for
example, Macleod 1996) is on the need for educational institutions to be
more flexible in using what young children bring to the setting and in
changing towards greater acceptance and valuing of differing home
literacy practices. These practices may be used as a bridge to support
children's access to the `schooled' or mainstream literacies
necessary for later educational and occupational success.
* Provision of on-going opportunities for staff development. This
enables teachers of early literacy to reflect on their practices, in
order to help support a more responsive and critically aware
`pedagogy' in the before-school years (Comber & Cormack 1995).
The project: Mapping literacy practices in early childhood services
in NSW, Australia
A study was recently conducted in New South Wales (Makin et al.
1999) to investigate literacy practices in early childhood services
immediately prior to school entry, to explore staff and parental
understandings of early literacy development, and to determine how
congruent these understandings were. The aim of this investigation was
to establish what needed to be provided to better support young
children's literacy development.
The project was funded by the government departments(1) responsible
for supporting and monitoring children's development within early
childhood services. The funding bodies were responsible for identifying
the research sites and for soliciting participation of staff according
to their criteria for inclusion in this research study, i.e., services
that were in areas of educational or socioeconomic disadvantage,
classrooms in which most of the children would be entering school in the
next year, and services that represented a wide range of service types
(long day care, preschool, private providers, community-run services).
Participation was voluntary. Eighty early childhood classrooms(2)
expressed interest in taking part. All were included. One subsequently
withdrew. Interviews were conducted with 158 staff with a range of
qualifications (degree, diploma, untrained). Nine parent focus groups,
with a total of 60 parents, were conducted. Service staff were asked to
invite parent participation, with the main criterion being that the
parents represent the diversity of the service (languages/cultures,
educational level, socioeconomic level).
Three data collection measures were used in the Mapping Project:
* The Early Childhood Language and Literacy Scale (ECLLS),
developed by the research team, based on the Early Childhood Environment
Rating Scale -- Revised (ECER-R, Harms et al. 1998). ECERS is an
internationally recognised observation scale designed to broadly
describe the daily activities, organisation, and quality of the learning
environment in early childhood services. It is considered to have good
predictive validity. The new ECERS-R scale consists of seven sub-scales:
Space and Furnishings, Personal Care Routines, Language-Reasoning,
Activities, Interaction, Program Structure and Parents and Staff. The
researchers added an eighth sub-scale relating to literacy and modified
some of the other sub-scale items to reflect literacy practices. This
included a focus on the use of information technology. Each classroom
was observed for a period of four hours at the same time of day.
* Staff interviews. Following the classroom observations, teachers
and their assistants or aides were interviewed and audio-taped using a
20-item standardised questionnaire designed to obtain information about
their beliefs and practices relating to young children's literacy
development. This measure was designed by the research team to
incorporate research relating to good early literacy practices.
* Focus group discussions. A total of nine focus group interviews
were conducted involving approximately 60 parents. Two of the groups
comprised parents from specific cultural backgrounds. One was held
within an Aboriginal community and was moderated by a community member
who was also the Aboriginal aide in the preschool. The other brought
together families from a specific language other than English (LOTE)
background. The group discussion was conducted in their language,
facilitated by a university lecturer who had considerable experience in
working within the community. A third group was multicultural in
composition and conducted in English. The other groups were primarily
English-speaking Australian in nature. Focus questions were designed to
identify parents' existing knowledge about their children's
early literacy development and their understanding and perceptions of
their children's literacy experiences in early childhood services.
ECLLS data were subjected to spreadsheet analysis. Interview data
was tape-recorded, transcribed and analysed using appropriate software
(NUD*IST) designed to help identify themes in non-numerical data.
Information was compared and contrasted and related to demographic
details of family and staff backgrounds.
Findings relating to the multiple perspectives of staff and parents
One of the major findings revealed through analysis of the
interview data was that staff and parents held differing perceptions of
what constitutes appropriate, high quality support for early literacy.
While both parents and staff recognised the importance of support for
early literacy, there were differences in terms of what the groups saw
as appropriate practices in early childhood services. A number of areas
were identified in which the views of parents and staff differed.
First, many parents expected a skills-based focus as necessary
preparation for school entry, whereas staff often resisted this. Many
parents wanted to see more writing and learning of the alphabet in
services.
These statements reflect the differing perspectives:
... no matter how much we talk to parents about how it is not appropriate
to teach a three-year-old sounds and writing, they still feel we are not
doing our job. (Staff)
Parental expectations is (sic) that the child should be taught to read and
write. I think it is important that we let them know that our philosophy is
not to do that. We want to give them lots of opportunities for reading,
lots of opportunities for story telling, singing, dramatic play which is
going to promote language in a much more informal way. (Staff)
I am paying for her to learn more so that she will be ready for school.'
(Parent)
I would like them to learn the alphabet and counting, a few more structured
things. (Parent)
I think they should have the alphabet and copy the letters. (Parent)
A second area of difference in the perspectives of staff and
parents related to the role of home and community in supporting
children's early literacy. Two areas in which this was evident
were:
1. the impact of technology and popular culture on children's
early literacy, and
2. the potential of literacy in languages other than English.
Impact of technology
Technology, including television, and popular culture were seen by
the majority of parents as important to their children's literacy
development:
Watching Sesame Street and Playschool [is] the best advantage a child can
have. (Parent)
Many staff, on the other hand, were concerned that children were
spending too much time watching television and that parents were not
reading books to children:
Some parents just stick their child in front of the TV. (Staff)
Data from ECLLS indicate that while television and videos were
available in many early childhood services they were not generally used
to support language and literacy development. Field notes included as
part of the ECLLS suggest that computers were not often available for
children's use. When they were, staff did not often interact with
children engaged in these experiences.
Literacy in languages other than English
In terms of bilingual/biliteracy development, parents appeared to
be in a difficult position. For parents from language backgrounds other
than English, learning to speak English was seen as a particularly
important part of preparing for later school literacy in English. On the
other hand, children learning English as a second language in the early
childhood program were reported by many parents to want to speak only
English at home:
My son now speaks everything in English ... I didn't imagine him learning
like that, I mean that fast! (Parent)
Parents were concerned about language loss, as children's home languages were not perceived as being supported in the service. Staff
interviews indicated that staff were not negative towards bilingual
development but saw the issue as problematic:
If we have afternoons when we may have a guest speaker, [parents] tend to
want, ... the few that are there, to listen, but often they don't have the
language to listen and it is very hard if we have translators there with
that group translating what the speaker has said. Because you can have
three or four different groups. Yeah, so that's quite difficult and we are
also trying to do it whilst running the program at the same time. (Staff)
Discussion
The multiple perspectives on literacy revealed in this project have
the potential to enrich early childhood programs and to improve support
of young children's early literacy. However, this potential was
often not realised. The rather restricted literacy practices reflected
in many of the early childhood programs that were observed appeared to
mean that valuable skills, resources and knowledge gained by children in
home and community experiences were ignored. Children's experiences
of information technology, popular culture and literacy experiences in
languages other than English were not generally recognised by staff or
included in programs.
A key literacy practice identified earlier is opportunity for
conversations about topics meaningful to children. Such topics could
include computer games, television programs, and popular culture figures
such as Barbie or characters in Star Wars. However, the data showed that
these areas were largely ignored by staff or dismissed as inappropriate:
Not much is going on in the home, usually just watching TV--this is the
place for literacy development. It's here or nowhere. (Staff)
It may be that the impact of technology and popular culture is
relatively new. Technology may be a powerful tool for early literacy,
but there has been little research carried out in preschools apart from
the study of word processing using computers and inconclusive studies of
the effects of television viewing on children's socialisation.
Early childhood educators also appear to be somewhat reluctant to engage
with the area (Dockett et al. 1999). Only a small minority of services
rated highly on the ECLLS item `Use of TV, video, and/or
computers'. Yet all children are experiencing an explosion of
technology and popular culture that requires different literacy skills
than those required for paper-based literacy.
Another of the key literacy practices outlined previously was
opportunity to learn about sound sequences in words, seen as a precursor
of reading in an alphabetic orthography. Data from the project showed
that parents wanted to ensure that their children developed the skills
they need for successful later literacy learning. Teachers were inclined
to be critical of those parents who placed a clear emphasis on teaching
letters and sounds to their preschoolers because this was perceived to
be developmentally inappropriate. The role of early childhood staff
should be to develop shared understandings with parents about how
phonological/ alphabetic skills are appropriately supported in early
childhood services without introducing a push-down curriculum. Dunn
& Kontos (1997, p. 9) suggest that 'It]he emotional costs of
academically-oriented classrooms, particularly for minority children
from low-socioeconomic families and especially boys, are real. Many
children from all backgrounds exhibit more stress in didactic settings
than in child-initiated environments'.
Adams (1990) cautions that children who come to school with a good
knowledge of the alphabet have also typically spent many hundreds of
hours in a variety of interactive literacy experiences. It may not be
the knowledge of the alphabet per se that is significant here but what
has underpinned such knowledge acquisition.
The funds of knowledge that children may bring to school
(knowledge, experiences, beliefs) are considered to be key factors in
successful later literacy learning according to a significant Australian
language and literacy policy document (ALLC 1995). Reports from
different countries (for example, Snow et al. 1998, Gregory 1994, Comber
& Cormack 1995) agree that children at risk of low literacy are
usually in one or more of the following categories: they come from low
income, low literacy homes; they come from bilingual homes; and/or they
have cognitive, hearing or learning disabilities. There were few
children in the last category in this study. However, there was a high
level of socioeconomic and educational disadvantage. Risk may well be
compounded when there is a low level of congruence between home and
early childhood services about how children's early literacy should
be supported.
There is a tension between educational discourses of difference and
deficit. Educators need to be mindful of the danger of disadvantaging
children through being so aware of difference that children do not gain
access to the dominant discourses of educational institutions. Too great
an emphasis, for example, on popular culture as a foundation for
literacy development can limit children's access to the more
traditional range that is needed for school success. On the other hand,
a narrow, traditional definition of literacy omits the experiences,
interests and skills of many students. Children in today's society
need to become competent in many literacies. Educational institutions
from preschool to tertiary education must facilitate this.
Implications for early childhood educators
The experiences, skills and domains of knowledge that support
successful literacy would all appear to be achievable in quality
prior-to-school programs. Intervention studies in preschools and homes
have shown a positive relationship between high quality
preschoolschool-based experiences and later success in literacy
learning. Even relatively modest fine-tuning of lower quality
prior-to-school environments has been shown to correlate positively with
children's later language and literacy development (Dickinson &
Smith 1994).
The years before school are where wide variation in the quantity of
verbal input and interaction with young children seems to begin to make
its mark on children's vocabulary growth, which is itself a
significant predictor of later literacy success (Hart & Risley
1995).
With regard to bilingual development, Australia's monolingual mind-set has proven resistant to change. Children who begin to learn a
new language in a successive situation (i.e., after the age of three) in
the preschool years are at risk of losing their home language (Makin et
al. 1995) and with it all the cognitive and linguistic benefits of
bilingualism, such as enhanced metalinguistic awareness. Development of
the home language supports the learning of a new language. However, in
Australia, bilingual early childhood programs are, by and large,
conspicuous by their absence.
The old pedagogical principles apply to early literacy as they do
to other areas: start where the child is and move from there; work from
what is known to what is unknown. This implies accepting children's
home languages and incorporating them into early childhood programs
(Mills & Mills 1993). It also implies accepting information
technology and popular culture as important forces in many
children's lives and incorporating them into early childhood
programs (Pahl 1999).
Children need opportunities to become interested in letters and
words. Many young children become interested very early in the first
letter of their own name. Early childhood staff need to be ready to
respond directly, discussing the letter and its sound(s) if the child is
showing interest in this.
Inclusion by staff of aspects of children's literacy
experiences in information technology and popular culture may offer
bridges into literacy for a wider range of children than those who
respond readily to traditional, paper-based literacy.
Conclusion
Over the last few years, there has been an increasing recognition
of the importance of social interaction in learning, and the importance
of partnerships between early childhood staff and parents. Staff and
parents both want children to experience successful literacy learning.
The question is how to do this in a way appropriate to young
children's development, without encouraging a 'push down'
curriculum, while at the same time recognising that what children know
when they reach school entry is a key factor in their successful
literacy (ALLC 1995). The project researchers, on the basis of their own
work in mapping literacy practices in 79 early childhood classrooms and
an analysis of the work of other researchers in early literacy, have
enunciated five principles that should underlie high quality support for
early literacy:
Principle 1: Established systems facilitate and maintain two-way
communication between educators and families about children's
languages and literacies.
Principle 2: Educators build on children's literacy
experiences from home and community, including experiences with
information technology and popular culture.
Principle 3: Planning systems support children's individual
literacies, including both dialects and languages other than English.
Principle 4: Literacy experiences are integrated across the
curriculum and throughout the day.
Principle 5: Educators extend children's constructions of
literacy by mediating their literacy play and by explicitly teaching
literacy concepts, processes and skills relevant to children's
experiences.
It is important not to overstate differences in parent and staff
perspectives. However, often it appears that the differences are most
acute in populations that are identified as vulnerable. If the cycle of
disadvantage and low literacy is to be broken, parents and staff must
both speak out and listen to each other as a necessary precursor to
developing shared understanding in supporting young children's
literacy learning.
(1.) NSW Department of Community Services (DOCS) and NSW Department
of Education & Training (DET)
(2.) In the majority of services, one room only was observed. There
were six services in which observation took place in two rooms.
References
Adams, M. 1990, Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about
Print, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
Australian Language & Literacy Council (ALLC), National Board
of Employment, Education & Training 1995, Teacher Education in
English Language & Literacy, AGPS, Canberra.
Bus, A. & van Ijzendoorn, M. 1995, `Mother-child interactions,
attachment, & emergent literacy: A cross-sectional study',
Child Development, vol. 59, pp. 1262-1272.
Cairney, T. 1994, `Family literacy: moving towards new partnerships
in education', Australian Journal of Language & Literacy, vol.
17, no. 4, pp. 262-275.
Clay, M. 1991, Becoming Literate: The Construction of Inner
Control, Heinemann, Portsmouth, New Hampshire.
Comber, B. & Cormack, P. 1995, `Literacy: social & cultural
practices', in Cornerstones: Module 1: Sociocultural Issues in
Literacy Learning, Department of Education & Children's
Services, Adelaide.
Dickinson, D. & Smith, M. 1994, `Long-term effects of preschool
teachers' book readings on low income children's vocabulary
and story comprehension', Reading Research Quarterly, vol. 29, pp.
104-122.
Dockett, S., Perry, B. & Nanlohy, P. 1999, `Computers in early
childhood services: a part of the educational program or less time to
play?', Australian Research in Early Childhood Education, vol. 6,
pp. 165-176.
Dunn, L. & Kontos, S. 1997, `What have we learned about
developmentally appropriate practice?', Young Children, July, pp.
4-13.
Epstein, A. 1999, `Pathways to quality in Head Start, public
school, and private nonprofit early childhood programs', Journal of
Research in Education, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 101-119.
Fitzgerald, J., Spiegel, D. & Cunningham, J. 1991, `The
relationship between parental literacy level and perceptions of emergent
literacy', Journal of Reading Behavior, vol. 23, no. 2, pp.
191-213.
Freebody, P. & Ludwig, C. 1995, EverydayLliteracy Practices In
and Out of Schools in Low Socioeconomic Urban Communities: A Descriptive
and Interpretive Research Program. Executive Summary, Curriculum
Corporation, Melbourne.
Gee, J. P. 1990, Social Linguistics and Literacies: Ideology in
Discourses, The Falmer Press, London.
Gregory, E. 1994, `Cultural assumptions and early years'
pedagogy: the effect of the home culture on minority children's
interpretation of reading in school', Language, Culture and
Curriculum, vol. 7, no. 2, pp. 111-124.
Harms, T., Clifford, R. & Cryer, D. 1998, Early Childhood
Environment Rating Scale -- Revised Edition, Teachers' College
Press, New York.
Hart, B. & Risley, T. 1995, Meaningful Differences in the
Everyday Experiences of Young American Children, Paul H. Brookes,
Baltimore, Maryland.
Heath, S. 1983, Ways with Words: Language, Life and Work in
Communities and Classrooms, Heinemann, Portsmouth, New Hampshire.
Juel, C. 1991, `Beginning reading', in Handbook of Reading
Research, Vol. 2. eds. R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P.D.
Pearson, Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, New Jersey.
Macleod, F. 1996, `Integrating home & school resources to raise
literacy levels of parents and children', Early Childhood
Development & Care, vol. 117, pp. 123-132.
Makin, L., Campbell, J. & Jones Diaz, C. 1995, One Childhood,
Many Languages: Guidelines for Early Childhood Education in Australia.
Harper Educational, Sydney.
Makin, L., Hayden, J., Holland, A., Arthur, L., Beecher, B., Jones
Diaz, C. & McNaught, M. 1999, Mapping Literacy Practices in Early
Childhood Services, report prepared for NSW DoCS & NSW DET, Sydney.
Michaels, S. 1981, `Sharing time: children's narrative styles
and differential access to literacy', Language in Society, vol. 10,
no. 3, pp. 432-434.
Mills, R. & Mills, J. 1993, Bilingualism in the Primary School,
Routledge, London.
Morrow, L. & Weinstein, S. 1982, `Increasing children's
use of literature through program & physical design changes',
Elementary School Journal, vol. 83, pp. 131-137.
Pahl, K. 1999, Transformations: Children's Meaning Making in a
Nursery, Trentham Books, Stoke on Trent.
Powell, D. 1996, `Teaching parenting and basic skills to parents:
what we know', Family Literacy: Directions in Research and
Implications for Practice, January, pp. 1-8.
Raban, B. & Ure, C. 1999, `Literacy in the pre school: an
Australian case study', in Landscapes in Early Childhood Education:
Cross National Perspectives, ed. J. Hayden, Peter Lang, New York.
Schickedanz, J. 1999, Much More Than the ABCs, NAEYC, Washington.
Snow, C. 1983, `Literacy & language: relationships during the
preschool years', Harvard Educational Review, vol. 53, no. 2, pp.
165-189.
Snow, C., Burns, M. & Griffin, P. (eds.) 1998, Preventing
Reading Difficulties in Young Children, National Academy Press,
Washington.
Teale, W. & Sulzby, E. (eds.) 1986, Emergent Literacy: Writing
and Reading. Ablex, Norwood, New Jersey.
Vukelich, C. 1994, `Effects of play interventions on young
children's reading of environmental print', Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, vol. 9, pp. 153-170.
Wells, G. 1985, The Meaning Makers, Heinemann, Portsmouth, New
Hampshire.
Laurie Makin is Director of the Research Centre for Young
Children's Education at the University of Newcastle, Ourimbah, NSW
and Associate Professor, Early Childhood. Laurie coordinates the early
childhood program and has a particular interest in early language and
literacy in both monolingual and bilingual children. She is currently
editing a book on early literacy with a colleague from the University of
Western Sydney.
Address: University of Newcastle, Ourimbah, NSW 2258
Email: lmakin@mail.newcastle.edu.au.
Margaret McNaught is a lecturer at the Institute of Early
Childhood, Macquarie University. She has a longstanding interest in the
literacy development of young children, arising from her previous work
as an educational psychologist. She has also worked in family-centred
early intervention, and researched systemic approaches to literacy
learning difficulties.
Address: Institute of Early Childhood, Macquarie University, NSW
2109
Email: margaret.mcnaught@mq.edu.au