Literacy in the transition years.
Cairney, Trevor ; Buchanan, John M. ; Sproats, Eira 等
The transition from primary to secondary school is a constant source
of concern for teachers, parents and students. In this article Trevor Cairney et al. report on a major study that particularly focussed on
literacy development and practices in this transition period. The study,
which followed Year 6 students into their first year of secondary
school, identifies some important issues for teachers, parents and
administrators to consider.
Introduction
The divisions between primary, secondary and tertiary education have
long been considered at best arbitrary, and at worst open to challenge
(Nisbet & Entwistle, 1969). While the divisions are longstanding,
educators have increasingly begun to examine the impact of the structure
of our educational system into phases with 'hard' transition
points. One area of inquiry has been the impact on students'
literacy practices of movement across these educational boundaries. The
study described in this article was funded by DEETYA under the National
Children's Literacy Projects program (Cairney, Lowe & Sproats,
1994) and investigated variations in literacy practices across the
primary /secondary divide. In particular, it focussed on the effects of
transition on students' literacy development.
The nature of school and the transition process
While many have claimed that the transition period is disruptive of
students' literacy development, little systematic study has
occurred concerning the literacy practices experienced by Year 6 and
Year 7 students. The common belief that the transition period is
disruptive of students' learning is evident in the number of
educational systems that are currently placing an emphasis on curriculum
and professional development work in years 6-8.
Much of this interest can be linked to a recent influential study
that showed that literacy development, like other scholastic abilities,
seems to plateau in the first two years of secondary school (Hill,
Holmes-Smith & Rowe, 1993). Other earlier researchers such as Power
& Cotterell (1979, 1981) have also observed that despite a degree of
continuity between primary and secondary practices, a change in
emphasis, from the mastery of literacy skills to their use, is evident.
It was the desire to more systematically explore the differences and the
impact of changing literacy practices across the primary/secondary
transition that led to this study.
Foundational to the study was a belief that schooling is a social
practice, or rather an amalgam of social practices. Its practices
reflect, and at the same time shape, the culture to which they belong.
If observed at one level, as skills to be mastered, the literacy
practices of primary and secondary schooling look remarkably similar.
However, this study was driven by the belief that differences in
practices require more than a simple analysis of the products or
processes of literacy. All literacy practices are social phenomena which
are part of the cultural fabric of groups. As such, the literacy
practices of schools may in fact privilege certain academic procedures
(Gee, 1990), and randomly promote certain social expectations (Bourdieu,
1977). Others have gone as far as to suggest that schools arbitrarily
privilege certain academic procedures, and also promote specific
definitions and traditions of literacy (Bourdieu, 1977; Friere &
Macedo, 1987; Lankshear & Lawler, 1987; Street, 1984). Primary and
secondary schools differ not only in their highly visible structures,
but also, more subtly, in their cultural expectations. Students are
faced with this adjustment during the transition into adolescence (Bezzina, 1988) with its inherent pressures related to social,
physiological and emotional changes.
Studies of the transition period
Some studies of the transition period have focussed on broad
curriculum issues and the degree of contact between primary and
secondary. For example, Power and Cotterell (1979,1981), in a study of
sixty-two primary and seventy-two high schools in Australia, observed
that compared with primary schools, high schools created environments
which are more goal-orientated and cohesive, less structured and more
conducive to independence. The degree to which content material built on
primary school work varied considerably between subjects. Later, Eltis,
Low, Adams and Cooney (1987) observed that while the transition to high
school was positive for most students, there existed little
curriculum-related contact between primary and secondary schools.
Other studies have shown that particular groups of students are
more affected than others by the transition process. It has been
variously suggested that special problems seem to exist for the socially
unconfident (Ward et al., 1982), aggressive or disruptive students
(Mekos, 1989), those from lower socio-economic backgrounds (Murdoch in
Nisbet & Entwistle, 1969) and those in the lowest performance
decile (Hill, Holmes-Smith & Rowe, 1993). Ward et al. (1982) also
observed that students regarded by their teachers as `dependent'
also encountered difficulties, in that they needed intensive support in
the form of feedback and direction. Such students, while working capably
in small groups, had problems in whole-class learning situations.
Needless to say, the above groups are already among those most at risk.
Other findings are less conclusive. For example, findings have
varied on the comparative effects on boys and girls (Nash, 1973; Ward,
1982; Mertin, Haebich & Lokan, 1989). As well, there appears to be
little information on the impact of the transition process on students
whose home languages differ from the language of instruction. One
interesting finding is that the students who are the most apprehensive
about adjusting to high school are not necessarily those who ultimately
encounter the most problems in the transition process (Mekos, 1989). In
general, students anticipated that high school would provide an increase
in challenges and relevance to adult life. Subsequent attitudes to high
school appear to depend on the extent to which these expectations were
fulfilled (Power & Cotterell, 1979, 1981).
Just as culture is not static, it is not internally consistent.
Within school cultures, sub-groups are constantly forming and evolving.
Commencement at a new school provides an environment conducive to
changes in such groups. This can have potentially positive and negative
benefits. Nash (1973) observed that the nature of these subgroups, and
teachers' perceptions of them and of their members, were likely to
influence the nature of teacher-student relationships.
However, from an educational point of view, it appears that
transition processes tend to compromise academic progress for all
students, albeit temporarily (McGee, 1989; Galton & Willcocks, 1983;
Nisbet & Entwistle, 1969), with the effects being most enduring on
the least able (Hill, Holmes-Smith & Rowe, 1993).
Responses to perceived differences
Given that the division between primary and secondary schooling is
likely to remain in some form, several researchers have made
recommendations to facilitate the transition process. Whitta (1977), for
example, advocated the establishment of orientation programs and
increased opportunities for primary and secondary teachers to
familiarise themselves with each other's situation and practices,
as well as the development of extra-curricular activities during the
first year of high school, capitalising on friendship groups throughout
the transition process.
Others have recommended the establishment of middle schools
(Hough, 1989), peer support programs (Reinstra, 1987), a more gradual,
or staggered, transition process (Clarkson, 1988) and increased
information sharing between schools' personnel (Beddoe, 1985; Dean,
1985; Jensen, 1984). The Beazley Report (Beazley, 1984) recommended the
appointment of one primary teacher in each school to be responsible for
the transition, and urged that attention be given to the social, as well
as the physical, environment of the high school in orientation programs.
It would appear that some of these recommendations have been trialled
but rarely at a system level. One exception was the priority given to
the transition period in NSW in 1997.
Studies focussing more specifically on literacy-related issues
As mentioned in the introduction, one of the key concerns of the
study described in this article was the effects of the transition
process on literacy development. Literacy could be described as the
'legal tender' of school and of society, the currency in which
academic and social transactions take place. Not surprisingly, then,
literacy practices play a central role in any changes encountered in the
transition process. They constitute an important indicator and potential
determinant of one's membership of, or exclusion from, particular
social groups. As such, they have been studied by a variety of
researchers and educational systems.
One major study was the Writing Reading Assessment Programme
(Education Department of South Australia, 1992) which sought to
investigate literacy practices in years 6 and 10 in South Australian schools. The Programme examined school curricula and assessed students
on a range of tasks, and also reported on the links between attitude and
literacy performance. In terms of range of texts, and purposes for which
literacy was used, similarities across the years appeared more evident
than differences.
Other researchers have investigated secondary students'
interest in reading (Atwell, 1987; Bintz, 1993) and text suitability
(Homer & Moore, 1981). For example, Bintz (1993), working with
forty-four students from eleven high schools, identified three levels of
reader enthusiasm, describing readers as avid, passive or reluctant. He
argued, that much reluctance towards reading derives from the mismatch between in-school and out-of-school reading. Bintz observed that
secondary teachers, in the face of their students' apparent
disinterest, further tightened control of reading processes and
materials. In his view, it is lack of choice in reading material which
quenches much student interest.
Beavis (1981) examined the apparent decline in enthusiasm for
reading among secondary students when compared with their primary school
counterparts, and advocated a literacy-rich environment, where students
could immerse themselves in conversation and literature (including their
own compositions). Such an approach would include allowing students to
`make thinking public' (Schmidt, Barry, Maxworthy & Huebsch,
1989: p. 430), sharing and exchanging their thoughts in draft form. This
collaborative dimension of literacy is reinforced by Furniss and Green
(1991), who observed students' capacity, when support was offered,
to compose sophisticated texts.
Goodall (1981) suggested that the `human dimension' may be
the central component of a program's success, observing that, `It
is an interesting possibility that the success of some special
transition programs ... may not be due to how they cushion the impact of
transition but to the fact that for once a group of teachers are taking
a personal and organisationally sensitive interest in the students as
people rather than learning units' (p. 34). According to Gunter
(1981: p. 50), attention to literacy development in the transition years
Will not only assist students educationally, but can help them make
sense of the transition process, serving to `encourage confidence in the
present, and understanding of coming life situations'.
On the basis of previous work conducted and the on-going concerns
of schools about the transition period, DEETYA identified this area as a
priority in 1993. The project described here was conducted in 1993-94.
It was predicated on the assumption that further research was needed
into the strategies primary and secondary teachers use to support their
students' literacy, and the different purposes for which literacy
is used, as well as into the wider cultural context in which words and
ideas are exchanged.
Methodology
The expected outcome of the study (outlined in the DEETYA project
brief) was the provision of information concerning effective strategies
to bridge the gap between the primary and the secondary school. To do
this, the study explored the effects of the transition process. More
specifically, it sought to describe literacy practices in a defined set
of primary and secondary schools, and investigated ways in which
students cope with the literacy demands in each context as they move
from primary to secondary school.
Four high schools were identified in socio-economically diverse
communities. These schools and all of their primary feeder schools served as the sites for the study. The research consisted of two
distinct stages. In the first, the research team spent a period of four
months (one day per week) in a total of thirteen Year 6 primary school
classrooms, observing the nature of literacy practices and forms of
teacher support. In the second phase, thirty-five of the students from
within the above thirteen classrooms were observed in the following year
in a variety of Year 7 classes over a period of three months in the
first and second school terms. In this phase the students were
`followed' as they moved from class to class.
The first community is located in Sydney's inner western
suburbs, and is characterised by high levels of unemployment and family
breakdown. The local students come from a range of language backgrounds.
The second community is situated in an economically disadvantaged area
of Western Sydney. About two-thirds of the local housing is
government-owned and the level of unemployment is above state and
national averages. The third neighbourhood is in Sydney's
outskirts. The area is populated by a Large proportion of professional
people, is demographically stable, and enjoys moderate affluence. The
fourth of these communities is located about three hours' drive
west of Sydney. The high school, in a rural town of about 2500 people,
draws its students from relatively small, far-flung primary schools,
including a one-teacher school.
An attempt was made to identify student participants who
represented the diversity of the populations present in the focus
schools of the thirty-five students interviewed for the study. Factors
considered included gender, ability levels, and language background
(English-speaking or non-English-speaking). Similar numbers of boys and
girls were included, and nine of the thirty-five students were from a
non-English-speaking background. Ability levels of the students were
varied; thirteen were considered as `above average' by their
teachers, fourteen were deemed to be `average', and the other eight
were regarded as `below average'.
Data collection procedures were similar for the primary and
secondary phases, and consisted of interviews, classroom observations,
the collection of artefacts such as student work samples, and the
analysis of school policies and teachers' programs (see Figure 1).
Students were interviewed on three occasions, once during the primary
stage and at the beginning and end of the secondary stage. Parents were
interviewed once during the primary stage, and again during the
secondary stage. Teachers, principals and librarians in the
participating primary and secondary schools were interviewed during the
corresponding stages of the project. Further information was also sought
from primary school teachers, in the form of a questionnaire during the
secondary stage.
[Figure 1 ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Examination of the teacher's role as a supporter of literacy
learning was accomplished as a result of 260 hours spent observing the
twenty primary school and 102 secondary school teachers who agreed to
participate in the study. Observations covered as wide a range as
possible of Key Learning Areas. Data were also gathered from interviews
with the stakeholders, i.e. the students, teachers and other staff, and
parents. Parents from twenty-seven of the thirty-five participating
families made themselves available for the initial interview. Of these,
six were from non-English-speaking backgrounds. At the end of the
secondary phase, eighteen parents consented to be interviewed.
Student interviews sought to explore a variety of issues such as
their attitudes to reading and writing, reading tastes, and sources of
ideas for writing. Year 6 students were asked about their expectations
regarding reading and writing in Year 7. Once in Year 7, the students
were asked how they had settled into high school, how it matched their
expectations, and how it compared to primary school. They were also
asked about the kind of help they received in certain subjects. Later in
the year, they were asked similar questions, as well as asked about
their favourite reading experience and their best piece of writing in
high school.
Parents of sixth graders were asked to identify how their children
learned to read and write, and how they themselves supported this
learning process for their children. They were also asked about their
children's current literacy practices and any communication they
had received from the high school. In the following year, they were
asked about their children's adjustment to high school, their
progress, the workload and assistance provided by the school and its
teachers.
Primary teachers were asked about their views on literacy
acquisition, their classroom literacy practices and support materials,
their expectations for their students in the following year, and the
ways they helped them prepare for secondary school. Secondary teachers
were asked how they supported literacy, the resources they used, their
writing, spelling and assessment procedures, as well as the ways in
which primary school prepared their students for high school.
Extensive field notes were taken during classroom observations.
Other forms of data collection included the recording of the amount of
time allocated to various literacy events.
Given that the classroom observations were extensive,
incorporating diverse perspectives, the data collected were
descriptively `thick' (Geertz, 1973). Procedures for data analysis
were recursive, in that findings from previous visits informed later
investigations. Data analysis methods corresponded to assumptions from
traditions of qualitative research (e.g. Guba & Lincoln, 1981;
Erickson, 1986). Analysis of the data drew on the principles of
`grounded theory' (Glasser & Strauss, 1967), to identify themes
as they emerged.
Key issues and factors in the transition period
A. Students', parents', and teachers'
apprehensions
Consistent with findings from previous studies, (e.g. Power &
Cotterell, 1981) most students' fears regarding high school were
unfounded or short-lived. Apprehensions concerning matters such as the
difficulty and amount of reading and maths, `initiations' and
getting lost at the school quickly disappeared. On the other hand, some
of the students actually looked forward to the new social and curricular
opportunities high school offered. At the end of the secondary period,
many of the students found Year 7 to be easier than Year 6 (e.g. 46 per
cent had expected reading to be harder)Some of them felt that high
school curriculum material overcompensated for perceived difficulties
associated with the transition process. A number of students complained
that they had covered certain material in Year 6, including, in some
cases, having read an assigned book before. In the words of one teacher,
high schools were, `Treating Year 7s like babies, giving them too many
workshops, wonder words...'. One of the students' expectations
which was fulfilled was the increased amount of homework in Year 7. Many
of the students (31 per cent) indicated that in Year 7 they did not have
the time to read books for pleasure due to other school reading demands.
As well, the proportion of boys who read for pleasure dropped from 83
per cent to 56 per cent. The girls in the study remained more avid
readers than the boys across years 6 and 7, but still experienced a
slight reduction in reading for pleasure (from 94 per cent to 88 per
cent).
It would seem that parents were yet more anxious than their
children about the transition to high school. While it was not a focus
of the research, it may be that these fears were associated with
perceptions of their own diminishing ability to support their children
as the level of difficulty of work increased. Most parents (80 per cent)
nevertheless felt that primary school had prepared their children well
for secondary education.
Parental involvement was considerably greater in primary than in
high schools. Nevertheless, support for their children's success at
school did not diminish from Year 6 to Year 7. Many parents continued to
help their high school children with research, either by providing
resources such as encyclopaedias at home or assisting with trips to the
local library. Some children, however, having entered high school, began
to resist help from their parents. Several primary schools offered
training programs for parents in areas such as computer literacy, or
support with literacy learning at home, e.g., Talk to a Literacy Learner
Programme (Cairney & Munsie, 1992).
Among primary tear-hers' apprehensions concerning high school
was the fear that less able students would not receive the help they
needed. Responses suggested that Year 6 teachers felt that there were
considerable differences between the expectations of primary and high
school. All schools in the study had implemented strategies to
facilitate the transition process. These included visits by Year 6
students to the high school, lessons taught to Year 6 students by high
school teachers, and peer support programs including camps, as well as
information sharing between primary and secondary teachers.
B. Variations in resources
Students and parents readily identified variations in resources
across the transition period as both an opportunity and sometimes a
threat While the use of computers was limited at both levels, high
schools made greater use of them. Most primary classrooms had access to
only one computer, which was not always in use, and although some
students were avid computer game players, use of computer software was
uncommon. High schools tended to have computer rooms with a minimum of
twenty computers, but in neither case did students seem to identify
computers as a central component of their education.
Use of the school library tended to differ between primary and
high schools. Whereas 57 per cent of Year 6 students claimed to borrow
books, only 38 per cent of Year 7 students reported doing so. It would
appear, though, that this difference represents part of a trend which
takes place over several years. Some students appeared to struggle
initially when using a larger, more diverse high school library.
Library lessons were more common at primary level. Given the
greater size and complexity of the school libraries this is
problematical. The purpose for using the library also varied between the
cohorts. While it was not uncommon to observe sets of library books
being used for research purposes in primary classrooms, none of the
participating students mentioned using the library for research purposes
during Year 6. In Year 7, however, 50 per cent reported doing so. There
appeared to be a corresponding decrease in library use for leisure
reading. Of those case study students making use of the library, only 20
per cent borrowed fiction books in high school, whereas in Year 6, 70
per cent had done so.
Primary and secondary classrooms in the study tended to present
quite different learning environments in terms of resources. Primary
classrooms contained materials such as reference books, as well as
fiction and non-fiction texts. Posters, photo-stories of school events,
students' work samples, merit charts and awards were visible.
Several primary classrooms had books belonging to `packaged'
reading schemes. Overall, primary classrooms were more inviting than
many secondary schools, which were often barren -- not surprising given
the multiple use of general classrooms and the large number of students
who move in and out of them. An exception to this were the various
specialist rooms (e.g. Science and Technics) which students experience
for the first time at high school.
C. Variations in policies and classroom practices
Another factor contributing to the varied nature of classrooms across
the transition was a range of different policies and practices. For
example, increased mobility in high schools led to less specific
classroom `ownership' on the part of teachers and students. In most
cases, students and teachers brought their books with them to the
classroom. Nevertheless, some secondary classrooms and corridors
displayed posters and work samples. Reasons for this included the
opportunity to share examples of `best practice', and as a source
of ideas. Owing to the differing expectations regarding the autonomy of
primary and secondary students, the latter are more likely to be in
classrooms without a teacher. There was a fear among some teachers that
unsupervised students might damage materials left unattended. Indeed,
one text type more evident in secondary than in primary school
classrooms was graffiti. A further difference which impacted on primary
and secondary teaching strategies was the time factor. Teachers in
primary schools enjoyed more flexibility in terms of session timing;
unlike their secondary counterparts they were free of the tyranny of
bells ringing every forty minutes or so.
Assessment procedures also differed, and tended to be more
formally administered in high school. There was a range of assessment
approaches to writing in primary school, from the identification of
spelling and punctuation errors in writing, to assessment of aspects
such as creativity. In some schools, students were tested and graded
according to their reading ability on entry to Year 7. Reflecting the
corresponding curricular emphases, testing in secondary school was
inclined to focus on content material, rather than on the skills
involved in writing. Primary assessment of writing tended to focus on
content, process and skills.
Forms of reporting also differed, although primary and secondary
schools both conducted parent-teacher interviews, and issued
twice-yearly reports. Primary school reports tended to be more
comprehensive in terms of literacy performance. Components of Year 6
reports, such as writing, spelling, reading (oral), comprehension and
listening were subsumed under the term `English' in most high
school reports. One of the high schools, however, referred to
competencies such as `ability to write for different audiences'
within English.
There appeared to be minimal expectations regarding homework in
primary school. As mentioned earlier, student participants observed that
there was considerably more homework in secondary school. Homework was
set in almost all Key Learning Areas. Much of this consisted of
exercises begun in class, or research projects.
One of the more paradoxical differences between primary and high
school was the variation in levels of responsibility. While students
gradually acquire more autonomy during their school career, they go from
a situation in primary school where they are the most senior students to
one in where they are the most junior. As a result, they may be accorded
less responsibility on arrival at high school. Similarly, primary
students appeared to be given more responsibility in school matters,
e.g., representation on the student council. Other reasons for this
diminution in older students' responsibility may be student
disinterest, or peer pressure not to display interest in the
establishment. Moreover, it may be that, whereas primary students in the
role of advisers are seen as endearing, adults feel comparatively
threatened by adolescents in the same role. Nonetheless, one of the
secondary schools tailored a peer tutoring program for Year 7 and Year 8
students, using Year 11 students as tutors, under staff supervision.
Another of the schools operated a peer support program in order to help
students work collaboratively.
Major variations also occurred in relation to classroom
organisation. Group work was the rule rather than the exception in the
primary schools visited, and was both student- and teacher-initiated. It
occurred in matters of curriculum (collaborative problem solving) and
administration (school council). Although group work was considerably
less prevalent in the high schools, student interaction, whether
sanctioned or not, was common in both contexts. Topics ranged from the
work at hand to totally unrelated themes such as leisure activities. The
physical set-up of some `practical' rooms in high schools (for
example, science labs) facilitated interaction, and some secondary
school teachers arranged the furniture in their rooms so as to establish
small groups.
On occasion, secondary students, too, were directed to share their
discoveries with the rest of the class, or were encouraged to ask their
peers for help. Nevertheless, much secondary teaching involved direct
instruction. It may be that older students feel less at ease than their
primary school counterparts asking for, and offering, assistance, and
the result is a more teacher-centred approach. Further research may
reveal whether this drives, or is driven by, the differences in school
practice. It would seem reasonable to expect, though, that students who
have come from an environment rich in student interaction may take some
time to adjust to a less collaborative milieu. Discipline was a more
complex problem in secondary than in primary schools, and this, too, may
further contribute to some teachers' preference for more formal
lessons.
The difference in emphases on group work has implications for the
type and amount of classroom talk in years 6 and 7 Discussion or
brainstorming (whole class and small group) occupied 50 per cent of all
sanctioned talk in the primary school classrooms observed. While such
talk remained at about 43 per cent of sanctioned talk in secondary
school, whole class discussions chaired by the teacher were much more
common than small group interaction. While teacher talk constituted
almost half of all classroom discourse, the proportion of
directive/disciplinary talk rose from 23 per cent in primary to 33 per
cent in high school. Explanations, on the other hand, dropped slightly
from Year 6 to Year 7.
Another variation from primary to secondary which perhaps impacted
on literacy, particularly in relation to teacher support, was that
secondary students tended to regard their teachers as experts in their
field, with specialist knowledge to impart. Correspondingly, secondary
school teachers, apart from those in the English faculty, were less
likely than their primary school colleagues to `foreground' their
role as teachers and models of literacy Subject specialisation also had
an effect on the vocabulary used in secondary classes, where teachers
were at pains to explain the terminology specific to their subject, and
at times changed their students' terms when responding to their
questions. Explanations of word origins were more likely in secondary
than in primary school. In primary schools, new vocabulary was
encountered and reinforced more commonly by means such as find-a-words,
definitions and synonym and antonym exercises. While there was
considerable overlap in the literacy practices required by primary and
secondary schools, the primary teachers were far more likely to focus on
the learning of literacy, whereas secondary teachers placed more stress
on using literacy to learn.
D. Variations in literacy, and literacy-related, practices
Literacy was a dominant practice in primary and secondary classrooms.
Figures 2 and 3 outline the relative amounts of time devoted in primary
and high schools to various classroom activities and indicate the extent
to which literacy was prevalent in primary and secondary classrooms. As
these tables show, the relative proportion of time devoted to literacy
was similar across primary and secondary school. However, there were
many variations in the type of literacy practices demanded. For example,
spelling was taught formally in all but two of the primary classrooms.
Dictations were conducted at the end of the week in some cases. Spelling
revision, as a component of proofreading and conferencing, was also
common in primary schools. In secondary schools, by contrast, spelling
was dealt within a less structured fashion. Spelling tests were still
conducted in some classes, and were observed on three occasions.
Interestingly, the students expressed less concern about spelling and
its capacity to interfere with the quality of their writing in Year 7
than they had in Year 6. This may reflect the differing priorities of
secondary and primary schools in relation to spelling or could be an
expression of an emerging adolescent bravado.
[Figures 2 and 3 ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Reading aloud was almost twice as common in primary as in high
school. On only two occasions were secondary teachers observed reading
from novels under study by the class. Students in secondary school
seemed less willing than they had been in primary school to volunteer to
read aloud, even though they now reported being less concerned at the
prospect of reading aloud than they were the previous year. Drop
Everything and Read (DEAR) programs appeared to be less effective in
secondary than in primary school. The greater time commitment needed for
satisfactory interaction with longer works of fiction may in part
account for this. Secondary students were most often observed reading
magazines and textbooks during DEAR time.
Materials read also differed between primary and high schools.
While novels were commonly read in primary school, most oral reading in
Year 7 consisted of reading from worksheets, overhead transparencies or
the board, for the purpose of gaining knowledge. Comprehension exercises
were common in both Year 6 and Year 7, but were used to different ends.
In Year 6 they served to give practice in reading skills, whereas in
Year 7 they were used to reinforce content material.
Moffett's (1981) hierarchy of writing was adopted as a broad
indicator of degree of intellectual sophistication. Implementing
Moffett's theoretical frame as a guide, we derived three categories
of writing based on the degree of intellectual demand, the extent to
which composing was a central concern, and the complexity of the
discourse. This led to the use of three categories of:
* handwriting, transcribing, copying;
* short answer pieces, and note making, and
* extended discourse. Writing tasks in all of these categories were
evident in both Years 6 and 7. Writing also occurred across a variety of
text types, including narratives, reports and recounts across the two
years.
However, while writing was a central component in both the Year 6
and Year 7 classes, extended discourse diminished across the
primary-high school boundary, from 39 per cent to 28 per cent of total
time spent writing (see Table 1). In Year 7, most of this extended
writing consisted of prose narrative. Correspondingly, short answer
pieces of writing increased from 33 per cent in Year 6 to 45 per cent in
Year 7. Like reading, it would seem that in Year 7, writing was used as
a tool for conveying information rather than as a practice to be
acquired.
Table 1: Proportion of time used for specific writing practices in
Year 6 and Year 7
Writing practices Year 6 Year 7
Transcribing/copying 28 27
Short answer pieces 33 45
Extended discourse 39 28
Conclusion
According to some previous studies, the effects of the transition
process have sometimes been underestimated, while at other times it
appears that schools have overcompensated for the anticipated effects.
Although attention has been given to the visible, structural differences
between primary and secondary schools, less energy has been devoted to
the more subtle differences in practices such as the use of literacy.
The research described in this article found that schools were
aware of differences between primary and secondary schools and that they
implemented a variety of transition programs. Some of these addressed
the curriculum as well as the structural differences between primary and
secondary schools.
Nevertheless, our work indicated the need for more information
sharing between schools. This should include exchange of information
between primary and secondary schools, and an increased overlap in the
purposes for and processes by which literacy is used at primary and
secondary levels.
The study confirmed that literacy is a dominant practice in
primary and secondary classrooms. But while the relative proportion of
time devoted to literacy was similar across primary and secondary
levels, there were many variations in the type of literacy practices
demanded in primary and secondary classrooms. For example, spelling was
taught formally in all but two of the primary classrooms; in secondary
schools, it was dealt with in a less structured fashion.
As well, reading aloud was almost twice as common in primary as in
high schools, and materials read also differed between primary and high
schools. While primary students often read independently from novels,
most oral reading in Year 7 consisted of reading from worksheets,
overhead transparencies or the board, primarily to gain knowledge.
Comprehension sheets and exercises were common in both Year 6 and Year
7, but were used to different ends. In Year 6 they provided practice for
reading skills, whereas in Year 7 they aimed to teach content.
Writing was found to be a central component in both the Year 6 and
Year 7 classes. However, extended discourse diminished across the
primary-high school boundary, from 39 per cent to 28 per cent of total
time spent writing, and in Year 7 most of this extended writing
consisted of prose narrative. Depressingly, short answer pieces of
writing increased from 33 per cent in Year 6 to 45 per cent in Year 7.
Nevertheless, like reading, it would seem that in Year 7, writing was
used far more as a tool for conveying information rather than as a skill
to be taught.
One interesting and yet worrying finding in this study was an
apparent decline in student interest in reading in Year 7. This is
similar to the previous findings of Bintz (1993) and Beavis (1981).
However, for the students in this study, the main reason for this
reduction appeared to be reduced time caused by increased homework.
Given the differences in the purposes for which literacy is used
in Year 6 and Year 7, we were surprised to find that most students
adjusted quickly to the transition phase. Our research did not uncover
significant problems for many of the students in making the transition
to Year 7. Of course, it may well be that some students do not
experience literacy difficulties until later in high school (perhaps
Year 8). There is some evidence from the comments of students in this
study, and from other research (Hill et al., 1993), that schools may in
fact `mark time' for a period in Year 7, thus reducing the impact
on students. This might partly explain the fact that this study did not
uncover significant problems for large numbers of students.
Another interesting observation within this study was a lack of
variation in provision for students with different needs. We observed
few adjustments for students from non-English-speaking backgrounds and
found little difference in literacy practices across communities which
at times varied greatly in terms of wealth, parental education levels,
and student ethnicity. A closer examination of literacy practices across
such diverse groups would be a useful extension to our work.
The success with which students adapt to their new academic
environments bears testimony to the efforts, initiatives and
partnerships established by schools. Some students defied their own and
others' expectations by performing better in high school than they
had previously. As well, our study found some support for the
conclusions of Power and Cotterell (1979, 1981) that attitudes to high
school are influenced by the extent to which student expectations prior
to high school are fulfilled.
Nevertheless, some specific groups of students do appear to suffer
academically from the effects of the transition, something which Hill et
al. (1993) have identified. It may also be the case that there are more
subtle changes in literacy practices as students move from Year 7 to
Year 8, something that was not a focus of the present study. Further
research may reveal whether those who arrive at high school with the
greatest `scholastic capital' (Bourdieu, 1977) adjust more easily
to changes, further widening the gap between the literacy `haves'
and `have-nots'.
REFERENCES
Atwell, N. (1987). In the Middle: Writing, reading and learning with
adolescents. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Beavis, C. (1981). Secondary teacher looks at a primary school.
English in Australia, 56, July. pp. 41-46.
Beazley, K. et al. (1984). Education in Western Australia: Report of
the Committee of Inquiry (Beazley Report). Perth: Ministry of Education.
Beddoe, N. (1985). The transition procedures of schools in the
Narrandera High School feeder area: Administrative implications.
Unpublished Masters thesis, University of New South Wales.
Bezzina M. (1988). Meeting the needs of children in transition: A
middle school programme for preservice teacher education. The South
Pacific journal of Teacher Education, 16, 1. pp. 101-108.
Bintz, W. (1993). Resistant readers in secondary education: Some
insights and implications. Journal of Reading, 36, 8. pp. 604-615.
Bourdieu, P. (1977). Cultural reproduction and social reproduction.
In J. Karabel and A. Halsey (eds), Power and Ideology in Education. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Cairney, T., Lowe, K. & Sproats, E. (1994). Literacy in
Transition: An evaluation of literacy practices in upper primary and
junior secondary schools. Vols 1-3. Canberra: Department of Employment,
Education and Training.
Cairney, T. & Munsie, L. (1992). Talk to a Literacy Learner
Programme. Sydney: University of Western Sydney.
Clarkson, M. (1988). Emerging Issues in Primary Education. London:
The Falmer Press.
Dean, J. (1985). Managing the Secondary School (Dean Report). London:
Croom Helm.
Education Department of South Australia (1992). Writing Reading
Assessment Programme (WRAP). Adelaide: Education Department of South
Australia.
Erickson, F. (1986). Qualitative research. In M. Whittrock (ed.), The
Handbook of Research in Teaching. New York: MacMillan.
Friere, P. & Macedo, D. (1984 Literacy: Reading the word and the
world. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Furniss, E. & Green, P. (eds). (1983). The Literacy Connection.
Melbourne: Eleanor Curtain Publishers.
Galton, M. & Wilkocks J. (eds). (1983). Moving from the Primary
Classroom. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Gee, J. (1990). Social Linguistics and Literacies: Ideology in
discourses. London: The Falmer Press.
Geertz, C. (1973). Thick description: Toward an interpretive theory
of culture. In C. Geertz (ed.), The Interpretation of Cultures. New
York: Basic Books.
Glasser, B. & Strauss, A. (1967). The Discovery of Grounded
Theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago: Aldine.
Guba, E. & Lincoln, Y. (1981). Effective Evaluation. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Heath, S. (1983). Ways with Words. London: Cambridge University
Press.
Hill, P., Holmes-Smith, P. & Rowe, K. (1993). School and Teacher
Effectiveness in Victoria: Key findings from the phase 1 of the
Victorian Quality Schools Project. Melbourne: Centre for Applied
Education Research.
Horner, J. & Moore, F. (1981). Research into reading: A study of
reading in the transition between primary and secondary schools in
Tasmania. English in Australia, 58. December. pp. 43-61.
Hough, D. (1989). Vertical Articulation for the Middle Grades.
Riverside, CA.: California Educational Research Cooperative.
Jensen, C. (1984). Transition from Primary to Secondary School.
Sydney: Inner City Education Centre.
Lankshear, C. & Lawler, M. (1987). Literacy, Schooling and
Revolution. London: The Falmer Press.
McGee, C. (1989). Crossing the divide: Transition from primary to
secondary school. SET, Research information for teachers, 1. pp 1-4.
Mekos, D. (1989). Students' perceptions of the transition to
junior high: A longitudinal perspective. Paper presented at the 1989
Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Indiana.
Mertin, P., Haebich, E. & Lokan, J. (1989). `Everybody will be
bigger than me': Children's perception of the transition to
high school. Australian Educational and Developmental Psychologist, 6,
2. pp 2-5.
Moffett, J. (1981). Coming on Centre: English education in evolution.
Montclair, NJ: Boynton Cook.
Morris, J. (1975). Reading in the later years. Reading. A journal for
the study and improvement of reading and related skills, 9, 2, June. pp
22-30.
Nash, R. (1973). Classrooms Observed: The teacher's perception
and the pupil's performance. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Nisbet, J. & Entwistle, N. (1969). The Transition to Secondary
Education. London: University of London Press Ltd.
Power, C. & Cotterell, J. (1979). Students in Transition.
Adelaide: ERDC.
Power, C. & Cotterell, J. (1981). Changes in Students in the
Transition from Primary to Secondary School. ERDC Report, No. 27.
Canberra: AGPS.
Rienstra, G. (1987). From primary school to secondary school: Making
it easier for students. The Practising Administrator, 9, 4. pp. 28-30,
Schmidt, C., Barry, A., Maxworthy, A. & Huebsch, W. (1989). But I
read the chapter twice. Journal of Reading, 32, 5. pp 430-31.
Street, B. (1984). Literacy in Theory and Practice. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Ward, B. et al. (1982). Junior High School transition study: Volume
VII, Executive Summary, Junior High School Transition Study. San
Francisco: Far West Lab for Educational Research and Development.
Whitta, J. (1977). Environmental press as a factor in primary
/secondary school transition. Queensland Institute for Educational
Research Journal, 11.