Intimate disclosure among best friends of youth: an opportunity for prevention of internalizing disorders.
Buskirk-Cohen, Allison
A Public Mental Health Problem
One of the more significant mental health concerns facing youth as
they transition from childhood to adolescence is the threat of
internalizing disorders. Risks for developing internalizing symptoms and
disorders, namely anxiety and depression, increase during this
developmental period (e.g., Avenevoli, Knight, Kessler, and Merikangas
2008; Hankin et al. 1998; Rutter 1991; Sorenson, Rutter, and Aneshensel,
1991). Additionally, in early and middle adolescence, girls begin to
manifest higher rates of internalizing symptoms that do boys (e.g.,
Hankin et al., 1998; Hussong 2000; Mariano and Harton 2005; Rudolph
2002; Rudolph and Conley 2005). Internalizing symptoms are related to
serious adjustment outcomes for youth, including significant social
impairment (Canino et al. 2004; Gotlib and Hammen 1992; Hammen and
Rudolph 1996) along with impairment in occupational performance,
interpersonal functioning, quality of life, and physical health in young
adulthood (Lewinsohn, Rhode, Seeley, Klein, and Gotlib 2003).
Furthermore, emotional distress during adolescence is associated with an
increased risk of suicide (Harrington et al. 1994; Weissman et al.
1999).
Interestingly, another developmental change is taking place that
ought to provide youth with protection against this mental health
threat. The nature of friendships and the functions they serve change as
children develop into adolescents. In childhood, friends serve as
playmates; in adolescence, they are viewed as confidants who provide
emotional closeness (Buhrmester 1990; 1996; Furman and Buhrmester 1992).
Gender differences in friendship also emerge during the developmental
transition from childhood to adolescence. Adolescent girls tend to place
more importance on affirmation of connectedness in their friendships,
whereas boys favor verification of status or agency (Buhrmester 1996).
Adolescent girls report more intimacy and support within their
friendships than do boys (Burda, Vaux, and Schill 1984; Dindia and Allen
1992; Rose and Rudolph 2006). In sum, adolescent friendships ought to
offer more support than childhood friendships, with the friendships of
girls being more supportive than those of boys. However, these findings
run contrary to the expectation that supportive friendships would offer
protection against emotional distress.
Because intimate disclosure is an important hallmark of the
difference between children and adolescents' friendships and ought
to serve as a buffer against internalizing symptoms, this paper focuses
on youths' responses to disclosure and the impact of these
responses on their emotional adjustment. Thus, the main purpose of this
paper is to explore the role of intimate disclosure as a process
contributing to the increase in internalizing symptoms experienced by
youth as they enter adolescence, with special attention to gender
differences and perspectives from different areas of research. In
reviewing the literature, this paper is organized around two main areas
of conflict in the research: 1) defining and measuring distress, and 2)
over-lapping constructs regarding the disclosure processes. A critical
examination of how different branches of psychology and related
disciplines approach the study of disclosure may lead to a better
understanding with potential to addressing this public mental health
problem.
Issues with Definitions and Measurement
Defining Internalizing Problems. Anxiety and depression are often
referred to as "internalizing disorders," which have been
defined as "conditions whose central feature is disordered mood or
emotion" (Kovacs and Devlin 1998, p.47). The symptoms of
internalizing disorders include withdrawal, fearfulness, inhibition,
and/or anxiety (Hogue and Steinberg 1995; Kovacs 1998). Despite being
recognized as separate disorders by the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric
Association 1994), empirical studies tend to find that depressive and
anxious symptoms overlap considerably, particularly in children and
adolescents (Achenbach and Edelbrock 1978; Reynolds 1992). At
subclinical levels, depression and anxiety occur as part of an overall
internalizing symptom picture; they can only be reliably distinguished
for clinically impaired youth (Achenbach and McConaughy 1992; Brady and
Kendall 1992; Compas, Ey, and Grant 1993).
Recent research efforts have attempted to explain the shared
aspects of these internalizing disorders. Findings of strong
correlations (Brady and Kendall 1992), co-morbid diagnoses (Angold,
Costello, and Erkanli 1999; Zahn-Waxler, Klimes-Dougan, and Slattery
2000), and successive diagnoses (Avenevoli et al. 2001; Mineka, Watson,
and Clark 1998) have led to this increased interest (Jacques and Mash
2004). In the adult literature, a tripartite model has been proposed in
which symptoms of anxiety and depression are considered along three
broad dimensions (Clark and Watson 1991; Watson et al. 1995). The first
dimension consists of general affective distress or negative affect (NA)
and is associated with both depression and anxiety. Physiological
hyperarousal (PH), the second dimension, applies only to anxiety. The
third dimension, a lack of positive affect (PA), applies only to
depression. Researchers believe that the strong association between
anxiety and depression can be explained by the shared symptoms of NA
(Watson et al. 1995). Burgeoning evidence for this tripartite model has
been found in nonclinical samples of children (Chorpita, Daleigden,
Moffitt, Yim, and Umemoto 2000; Muris, Schmidt, Merckelbach and
Schouten, 2001; Philips, Lonigan, Driscoll, and Hooe 2002). However,
studies suggest that the model is best supported in older children and
adolescents (Cole, Truglio, and Peeke 1997; Jacques and Mash 2004).
Measuring Internalizing Problems. In order to assess internalizing
symptoms, various methods may be used, the most common including
self-report measures and surveys completed by important others.
Questionnaires are easy to complete and allow researchers to gather data
on many participants in a short amount of time. These measures assess
the severity and frequency of symptom (Michael and Merrell 1998), but
are not meant for diagnostic purposes. Clinical interviews do allow for
clinical judgment; however, they are extremely time-consuming and
require a reliable interviewer who is a trained clinician (Aschenbrand,
Angelosante, and Kendall 2005). Thus, many researchers rely on
self-report or other-report questionnaires.
There are many self-report measures utilized to identify
internalizing symptoms. The Youth Self Report (YSR; Achenbach 1991) is
an example of one type of self-report questionnaire that assesses the
presence of internalizing symptoms in youth. There are a plethora of
self-report measures that assess only the presence of depressive or
anxious symptoms. For example, the Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale
(RADS; Reynolds 1986), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward,
Mendelson, Mock, and Erbaugh 1961), Children's Depression Inventory
(CDI; Kovacs 1980), and Depression Scale of the Center for Epidemiologic
Studies (CES-D; Radloff 1977; CES-DC; Weissman, Orvaschel, and Padian
1980) are examples of frequently used self-report scales to assess
depressive symptomatology. Several popular self-report measures of
anxiety include the Revised Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale
(RCMAS; Reynolds and Richmond 1985), the revised Fear Survey Schedule
for Children (RSSC-R; Ollendick 1983), the State-Trait Anxiety Scale for
Children (STAIC; Speilberger 1973), and the Multidimensional Anxiety
Scale for Children (MASC; March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, and
Conners 1997).
However, whereas such questionnaires are designed to measure
symptoms of only one disorder, they frequently include symptoms of
comorbid disorders as well. For example, the Children's Depression
Inventory (CDI; Kovacs 1980) includes symptoms of anxiety in addition to
symptoms considered to be more characteristic of depression (Compas et
al. 1997).
There also are a multitude of other-report questionnaires
available, but researchers have cautioned that a report by the child of
his or her own state is important since internal distress may not be
identifiable by others (Flanery 1990). Maternal reports have been
utilized, but findings suggest that there are discrepancies between
maternal reports and youth's self-reports of internalizing symptoms
(e.g., Breslau, Davis, and Prabucki 1988; Friedlander, Weiss, and
Traylor 1986; Jensen, Traylor, Xenakis, and Davis 1988), which have led
researchers to question the validity of maternal-reports, especially
concerning ratings of depressive symptoms. Several explanations for this
disagreement have been suggested. For example, Compas et al. (1997)
suggested that perhaps because many symptoms of depression are not
readily observable, it might be difficult for others to reliably assess
another's depression. Another explanation suggests differences
between clinical and community samples. Studies indicate that there
seems to be better agreement between maternal- and self-reports in
non-clinical samples (e.g., Butler, MacKay, and Dickens 1995;Thomas,
Forehand, Armistead, Wierson, and Fauer 1990). Discrepancies in clinical
samples may be related to higher incidences of maternal psychopathology
(e.g., Berg-Nielsen, Vika, and Dahl 2003; Najman et al. 2001).
In addition to determining who reports on the youth's level of
distress, another area of contention is establishing what constitutes
distress. Some researchers employ clinical cut-off scores to categorize
youth, while others use continuous scores. All indices of distress may
be important to study, in that even at the symptom-level (as compared to
the disorder-level), they are related to dysfunction (Judd, Akiskal,
Maser, Zeller, Endicott, and Coryell 1998; Kandel and Davies 1982; Pine,
Cohen, Cohen, and Brook 1999). For example, moderate depressive symptoms
have been found to be associated with academic and peer relationship
problems (Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, and Seligman 1992; Susman, Dorn, and
Chrousos 1991). Even subclinical levels of depressive symptoms have been
associated with significant psychosocial impairment (Judd et al. 1998;
Lewinsohn, Solomon, Seeley, and Zeiss 2000; Pickles, Rowe, Simonoff,
Foley, Rutter, and Silberg 2001). Like individuals with sub-clinical
levels of depression, individuals with sub-clinical levels of anxiety
tend to suffer on adjustment indices compared to non-anxious individuals
(Papsdorf and Alden 1998). Thus, all the different classifications are
important areas of legitimate study (Compas et al. 1993).
Summary of Distress Findings. Comparing the results of studies is
difficult because of lack of agreement among researchers for what
constitutes emotional distress and how to ascertain it. Special
attention must be paid to which measures are used, and how they are used
before conclusions may be drawn. Conflicting results may be a result of
differing methodologies. When studying internalizing disorders in youth,
researchers need to ensure that their methods are developmentally
appropriate and sensitive to the issues children and adolescents face.
One way to approach the study of youth disorders it to identify
important developmental tasks and assess whether symptoms affect these
tasks.
Forming close peer relationships is an important developmental task
of the pre-adolescent and adolescent period (Berndt 1982; Daley and
Hammen 2002; Parker, Rubin, Earth, Wojslawowicz, and Buskirk 2006). The
influence of parents and peers changes as a child moves into
adolescence. Contact with peers increases, whereas parental monitoring
decreases during the transition into adolescence (Brown 1990). Research
indicates that during adolescence, best friendships become increasingly
important as sources of social support, and that close friends
significantly contribute to adolescents' self-concept and
adjustment (Buhrmester 1990; Compas, Slavin, Wagner, and Vannatta 1986;
Furman and Buhrmester 1992).
Numerous studies have documented the association between
adolescents' peer relationships and internalizing symptoms of
depression and anxiety (e.g., Hecht, Inderbitzen, and Bukowski 1998;
Vernberg 1990). Children who have friends seem to negotiate the middle
school transition better than those who do not (Hartup 1996; 2000).
Research indicates that close friends significantly contribute to
adolescents' self-concept and adjustment (Asher, Parker, and Walker
1996; Buhrmester 1990; Compas et al. 1986; Furman and Buhrmester 1992;
Rubin, Dwyer, Booth-LaForce, Kim, Burgess, and Rose-Krasnor 2004).
However, it is important to note that friendships can serve as a source
of support (Berndt, Hawkins, and Jiao 1999) or as a source of stress
(Elias, Ubriaco, Reese, Gara, Rothbaum, and Haviland 1992).
Disclosure in Best Friendships of Youth
Intimate disclosure may include discussion of daily thoughts and
concerns or more serious problems, such as parental divorce (Rotenberg
1995). As noted above, in adolescence, youth depend more on their
friends for intimacy, self-disclosure, and help solving problems (Berndt
and Perry 1990; Buhrmester 1990, 1996; Newcomb and Bagwell 1996).
Research on communication in relationships indicates that as
relationship closeness increases, communication becomes higher in both
breadth and depth of disclosure, meaning that close friends share more
intimacy and that this sharing generates feelings of emotional closeness
(Altman and Taylor 1973).
One important aspect of friendship is intimate disclosure.
Disclosure may include discussion of daily thoughts and concerns or more
serious problems, such as parental divorce (Rotenberg 1995). In the
study of disclosure processes, it is believed that both verbal and
nonverbal communications vary along a depth continuum, from very
superficial to very personal (Altman and Taylor 1973). Developmental
theorists (e.g., Sullivan 1953; Youniss 1980) have posited that as youth
age, disclosure becomes more central to their friendships. There is an
increase with age in disclosure to peers above that to parents
(Buhrmester and Prager 1995). Particularly in middle childhood and early
adolescence, youth are more likely to disclose to (and share more
intimate disclosures with) same-sex friends than other-sex friends or
parents (Buhrmester and Prager 1995). As children enter adolescence,
youth depend more on their friends for intimacy, self-disclosure, and
help solving problems (Berndt and Perry 1990; Buhrmester 1990, 1996;
Buhrmester and Furman 1987; Newcomb and Bagwell 1996). Research on
communication in relationships indicates that as relationship closeness
increases, communication becomes higher in both breadth and depth of
disclosure, meaning that close friends share more intimacy and that this
sharing generates feelings of emotional closeness (Altman and Taylor
1973).
Research also has indicated that there are gender differences
pertaining to normative disclosure processes. In general, females tend
to disclose more than do males (Buhrmester and Prager 1995). Females
report more breadth and depth of disclosure in their interpersonal
relationships than do males (Baxter and Wilmot 1983). Female friendships
are more likely to entail intimacy and disclosure than male friendships,
especially in adolescence (Berndt 1982; Cohn and Strassberg 1983; Cooper
and Ayers-Lopez 1985). Research from the sociolingual perspective
mirrors these findings; girls talk to their best friends about troubles
while boys are uncomfortable with such as task (e.g., Tannen 1990).
Research comparing males and females has indicated that females place a
greater emphasis on harmonious relationships and demonstrate more
social-evaluative concerns (Cross and Madson 1997; Maccoby 1990). Some
researchers have suggested that females' investment in
relationships may expose them to more stress (e.g., Gore et al. 1993;
Kessler and McLeod 1984). By being emotionally involved in the lives of
others, females may become vulnerable to the stresses of their own life
events and to the stresses of their friends' life events (Kessler
and McLeod 1984).
Since disclosure is such an important aspect of interpersonal
relationships, especially during this developmental period, it is
important to examine whether distressed youth have difficulty with this
aspect of their friendships. Research with emotionally distressed
individuals suggests that their social skills make them less than ideal
candidates for disclosure. Depressed individuals have been found to make
less eye contact, talk less, and smile less in conversation than
nondepressed people (e.g., Segrin 1990; Segrin and Abramson 1994). The
behavioral manifestations of anxiety are similar to those of depression,
including lack of eye contact; speech disturbances, such as stutters and
omissions; and fewer facial expressions, which also may make them less
desirable partners for disclosure (Cheek and Buss 1981; Jones and
Carpenter 1986; Papsdorf and Alden 1998). Socially anxious individuals
are viewed as less likable, less sympathetic, and less easy to talk to
by their friends and family members (Jones and Carpenter 1986). Some
studies have suggested that anxious individuals spend less time talking,
select less intimate topics, and fail to reciprocate their
partner's level of disclosure (Bruch, Gorsky, Collins, and Berger
1989; Cheek and Buss 1981; Meleshko and Alden 1993).
However, an important limitation to this work is that many of these
studies assess conversations between the distressed individual and
strangers, such as an interviewer or unknown peer (Edison and Adams
1992; Papsdorf and Alden 1998), roommates (Burchill and Stiles 1988) or
spouses (Ruscher and Gotlib 1988)--but not friends. In an exception,
Segrin and Flora (1998) compared college undergraduate students'
verbal behaviors in conversations with friends to conversations with
strangers. Half of the depressed participants (those with BDI scores
greater or equal to 12) were invited to the laboratory with a close
friend, while the other half was matched with an unfamiliar peer. (Close
friends were not assessed for level of depressive symptomatology.) The
dyads were instructed to discuss their day and any other topics that
emerged in conversation. When conversing with friends (as compared to
strangers), depressed adolescents used more negative language, including
criticism, negative solutions, justifications, disagreements, and
negative self-disclosure. Surprisingly, they found that depressed
individuals used more partner-focused speech (which included utterances
that reflected an understanding of the partner's experience by the
speaker, such as a confirmatory statement) than nondepressed youth,
especially in conversations with friends.
Topics of Disclosure. The research concerning topics of disclosure
is somewhat sparse. In general, youth disclose more intimate information
to friends than nonfriends (Rotenberg and Sliz 1998). The topics of
disclosure may depend on the gender of the individuals disclosing.
Girls' conversations with best friends tend to be more tightly
focused and cover a smaller number of topics than boys'
conversations (Tannen 1990). Research on interpersonal relationships
suggests that individuals approach friendships with a specific type of
orientation that may be genderspecific (Cantor and Malley 1991; McAdams
1984). Girls tend to be relationship-oriented and focused on receiving
peer approval, whilst boys tend to be more activity-focused (Rose and
Rudolph 2006; Rudolph and Conley 2005). More specifically, girls are
more likely to share information about interpersonal problems and
concerns than are boys (Buhrmester and Prager 1995). These findings
suggest that topics of disclosure vary by gender, with girls discussing
problems relating to interpersonal relationships and boys discussing
problems relating to activities.
In one of the most thorough investigations of disclosure topics,
Carlson, Schwartz, Luebbe, and Rose (2006) asked 10th grade same-sex
friend dyads to discuss problems for 16 minutes while being videotaped.
Dyads discussed an average of five problems in that time. Evidence for
gender differences in problem types was found. Girls were most likely to
discuss problems about other close friends, whereas boys were most
likely to discuss problems about extra-curricular activities. Other
types of problems discussed included those with romantic partners, peers
in general, parents, other family members, and academics. Unfortunately,
this information was presented at a conference and has yet to be
published.
Importantly, distressed youth may have different concerns that they
discuss in their friendships. Heller and Tanaka-Matsumi (1999) matched
depressed adolescents (ages 13 through 18) with a non-depressed peer
(within 3 years of age of each other). Dyads were instructed to discuss
two topic areas, "negative, sad experiences" and
"positive, easy to discuss experiences." Researchers found
that the topics of conversation included friends positive, friends
negative, family positive, family negative, hobbies, disease/sickness,
death, sex, drugs/alcohol/cigarettes, physical/sexual abuse, school,
future goals, and other. In a study of college undergraduates, Jacobson
and Anderson (1982) investigated spontaneous self-disclosure and
depression. Depressed and nondepressed undergraduates waited in a room
with an unfamiliar peer (a confederate) while researchers audiotaped
their interactions. Depressed undergraduates made more negative
self-statements than nondepressed adolescents. Additionally, in response
to neutral remarks by the confederate, depressed undergraduates were
more likely to self-disclose.
In sum, more research is needed to determine topics that youth
disclose with their best friends. The aforementioned studies suggest
that there are gender differences, with girls discussing relationship
problems and boys discussing activity-related problems. However, it is
quite common in these studies for investigators to direct youth to
discuss problems--these conversations do not spontaneously occur.
Possibly, the topics that youth disclose naturally may differ.
Responses to Disclosure. There are various ways in which youth may
respond to selfdisclosures. Derlega and Grezelak (1979) posited that
there are five functions of disclosure. They include 1) receiving social
validation, 2) gaining social control (or managing others'
impressions of the self), 3) achieving self-clarification, 4) exercising
self-expression, and/or 5) enhancing relationship development.
Considering these functions of disclosure, it would seem that most
individuals expect their friends to respond to a self-disclosure in a
kind and helpful manner. However, youth may respond in a more negative
fashion, perhaps becoming irritated if they find the disclosure to be
inappropriate. The study of how youth (and adults) respond to disclosure
has been segregated by various areas of psychology and across different
disciplines; the following sections attempt to address over-lapping
constructs and ideas.
Reciprocity of disclosure. Social psychologists argue that there is
a tendency for individuals to respond to disclosure with their own
disclosure, known as reciprocity of self-disclosure (Jourad 1959; Miller
and Kenny 1986; Worthy, Gary, and Kahn 1969). Reciprocity is thought to
demonstrate a mutual understanding, which is important to friendship at
any age (Rotenberg and Sliz 1998). Disclosure reciprocity is considered
normative, especially in close relationships (Caltabiano and Smithson
1983). In studies of disclosure reciprocity, researchers typically
investigate whether individuals respond to a statement of
self-disclosure with their own disclosure. In one such study, Cohn and
Strassberg (1983) played prerecorded disclosures that varied in intimacy
to 3rd graders and 6th graders. Participants were asked to respond by
disclosing on topics varying in intimacy. The researchers found that
youth spent more time providing intimate disclosures when they heard a
high-intimate disclosure than when they heard a low-intimate disclosure.
No age differences were found in this study.
However, other studies of youth have found that disclosure
reciprocity may be a developmental skill. For example, Rotenberg and
Chase (1992) created videotapes of children providing statements of
varying levels of intimate disclosure. The videotapes were shown to
children and young adolescents (in kindergarten, second, fourth, and
sixth grades) who were asked to respond to the statements with
information about themselves. The statements they responded to were of
high, medium, and low intimacy. Young children (those in kindergarten
and 2nd grade) did not demonstrate reciprocity of self-disclosure.
Pre-adolescents (4th graders) demonstrated reciprocity during the high
and low intimacy conditions. Young adolescents (6th graders)
demonstrated reciprocity in all three conditions- high, medium, and low
intimate disclosures. It is important to note that in the aforementioned
studies, youth disclosed to unfamiliar peers, not friends.
These findings may not apply to the friendships of distressed youth
as several studies have demonstrated that both anxious and depressed
youth do not display normative patterns of disclosure. Anxious
individuals fail to reciprocate their partner's level of
disclosure, spend less time talking, and select less intimate topics
than do non-anxious individuals (Bruch et al. 1989; Cheek and Buss 1981;
Leary, Knight, and Johnson 1987; Meleshko and Alden 1993). Depressed
individuals, on the other hand, disclose at higher levels of intimacy,
especially about negative topics (Blumberg and Hokanson 1983; Jacobson
and Anderson 1982). Furthermore, that which determines reciprocity of
disclosure may not be internalizing symptoms, but rather, the similarity
of partners.
Papsdorf and Alden (1998) matched anxious undergraduate women with
a confederate. Confederates were female research assistants, two of who
were not anxious and two of who indicated moderate anxiety. Papsdorf and
Alden (1998) found that confederates were not different from each other
on any of the dependent variables; thus, all data were combined. The
dyad was instructed to get to know each other using a list of provided
topics. They found that anxiety did not predict self-disclosure; rather
intimacy of disclosure predicted ratings of similarity, which impacted
partners' liking of each other and desire for future contact. While
these researchers did investigate potential partner differences on
dependent variables, they did not assess the differences in dyads with
two anxious females versus dyads with only one anxious female. It is
also important to note that many of these studies relied on
undergraduate samples (e.g., Meleshko and Alden 1993; Papsdorf and Alden
(1998); thus, further research is needed to explore whether these
findings hold true for youth.
Thus, from a social psychology perspective, when youth respond to a
friend's disclosure with one of their own, they are simply
demonstrating reciprocity of disclosure. They have been socialized to
interact in this way. This construct explains why friendships with high
amounts of disclosure are rated as high quality friendships. Some
evidence suggests reciprocity is a developmental skill (e.g., Rotenberg
and Chase 1992) corresponding with the finding that the friendships of
older youth feature more intimate disclosure. An important limitation to
this area of work is that gender differences have not been explored and
more developmental work is needed.
Excessive reassurance seeking. Clinical psychologists focus on
intimate disclosure among the relationships of those suffering from
internalizing disorders. There is reason to believe that depressed and
anxious youth may evoke negative responses from others. Coyne's
(1976) interactional theory of depression (sometimes referred to as his
interpersonal theory, e.g., Potthoff, Holanhan, and Joiner 1995) argues
for a cyclical pattern in which depressed individuals frustrate their
significant others, eroding the relationship and eliciting depression in
both themselves and the others. Similarly, Arkin's (1981) and
Millon's (1981) theories suggest that anxious individuals employ
self-protective behaviors that induce negative emotions in their
relationship partners and result in partner avoidance. Building upon
Coyne's (1976) interactional theory, Joiner, Metalsky, and
colleagues (Joiner 1994; Joiner, Alfano, and Metalsky 1992; Joiner and
Metalsky 2001) have suggested that depressed individuals seek
reassurance about significant others' feelings for them. However,
when the other provides reassurance, the depressed person doubts the
support and seeks further reassurance. The repeated demands for
assurance frustrate the other and erode the relationship. Research has
verified a link between excessive reassurance seeking and depressive
symptoms, but many of these studies were conducted with adults (e.g.,
Gotlib and Beatty 1985; Gotlib and Robinson 1982; Strack and Coyne
1983).
In recent years, there have been some studies conducted with
children and adolescents. Studies of undergraduate samples have
replicated these patterns, showing that depressed adolescents are more
likely to exhibit an excessive reassurance seeking style and, thus, be
more likely to be rejected by significant others (e.g., Joiner 1994;
Joiner et al. 1992; Joiner and Metalsky 2001; Joiner, Metalsky, Katz,
and Beach 1999). In a study of psychiatric inpatients, ages 7 through
17, Joiner (1999) found that depressive symptoms were associated with
self-reported interpersonal rejection, especially among those who
indicated an excessive reassurance seeking style. Age differences were
not found, but a limitation to this study is that the sample was rather
small (N= 68) and may not have provided enough variability for
developmental analyses. Another study of psychiatric inpatient males
also found that depressive symptoms were associated with self-reported
interpersonal rejection, especially among those who indicated an
excessive reassurance seeking style (Joiner and Barnett 1994).
An important note is that most of these studies utilize clinical
samples, which may offer a narrow perspective how reassurance seeking
operates. In a study of non-clinical youth, Prinstein and colleagues
(Prinstein, Borelli, Cheah, Simon, and Aikins 2005) gathered information
over the course of three years on peer acceptance, friendship quality
and stability, and depressive symptoms from youth in the 6th through 8th
grades. They found that depressive symptoms were associated with less
stability in friendships and with increases in perceptions of negative
friendship quality. Girls and boys did not report significantly
different levels of reassuranceseeking behaviors. However, it was only
for girls that reassurance seeking had a significant negative
consequence on friendship, and this relation depended on the reporter.
Adolescent girls reporting on their own behaviors did not perceive their
own reassurance-seeking behaviors to be responsible for decreases in
positive friendship quality. However, their friends' reports
indicated such a relation. Furthermore, for girls, increasing levels of
reassurance seeking were predicted by depressive symptoms and peer
rejection combined with perceptions of negative friendship quality.
These findings highlight the importance of considering individuals'
perceptions and the role perspective plays in interpersonal
relationships.
It is important to note that these studies utilized self-report
scales. The only observational study of excessive reassurance seeking
was conducted with college undergraduate students (Joiner and Metalsky
2001). Additionally, there have been few investigations of excessive
reassurance seeking and anxiety (e.g., Burns, Brown, Plant,
Sacs-Ericsson, and Joiner 2006; Joiner and Schmidt 1998). Considering
the co-morbidity of depression and anxiety along with the similarity in
behavioral manifestations, it seems quite likely that the current
findings may apply to anxious individuals as well. Studies do suggest
that negative reactions from others perpetuate the negative social
expectations and avoidance found in anxious individuals (Alden and
Bieling 1997; Clark and Wells 1995). However, studies assessing whether
excessive reassurance-seeking predicts anxiety have found no evidence
for this relation (Burns et al. 2006; Joiner and Schmidt 1998) and
suggest that excessive reassurance-seeking is specific to depression.
Both of these studies utilized college undergraduate samples; more work
is needed with other age groups.
One may also view these negative responses as part of a
"misery loves company" scenario, which has been suggested for
shy, withdrawn children (Rubin et al. 2006). Rubin and colleagues found
that shy/withdrawn children (in the 5th grade) were more likely to
befriend other shy/withdrawn children and that these friendships tended
to be lower in quality than those of typical children. They hypothesized
that the coping of two shy/withdrawn children who have poor
self-perceptions and trouble with peers may lead to internalizing
problems. It seems reasonable to extend this line of thought to the
friendships of anxious/depressed youth.
Co-rumination. Finally, co-rumination, a relatively new construct
from developmental psychology, refers to excessively discussing problems
within a dyadic relationship (Rose 2002; Rose, Carlson, and Waller
2007). It integrates the construct of intimate disclosure with a social,
interactive form of rumination. Co-rumination includes frequently
discussing problems, discussing the same problem repeatedly, mutual
encouragement of discussing problems, speculating about problems, and
focusing on negative feelings. In the context of co-rumination, this
sharing of negative feelings is thought to be maladaptive (Rose 2002;
Rose, Carlson, and Waller 2007). Since this construct is in its infancy,
currently, much of the research comes from the originator's
laboratory. It is important to note, however, that this construct has
received much attention in the peer relationships literature. A search
in the database Google Scholar revealed that Rose's (2002) initial
study has been cited 230 times as of May 2012.
In the first study, Rose (2002) assessed co-rumination, friendship,
and internalizing symptoms in children in grades three, five, seven, and
nine. She found that co-rumination was related to high quality, close
friendships, and internalizing symptoms of anxiety and depression. There
also were age and gender "main effects," with girls reporting
co-rumination more than boys, and adolescent girls co-ruminating more
than any other group. These higher amounts of co-rumination helped to
account for girls' higher friendship quality and greater
internalizing symptoms. Thus, Rose (2002) suggests that higher quality
friendships may provide an opportunity for co-rumination, which may lead
to more emotional distress.
In a later study, Rose, Carlson and Waller (2007) conducted a
6-month longitudinal study with children and adolescents examining
whether co-rumination served as both a risk factor for internalizing
symptoms and a protective factor for friendship problems. Youth in the
3rd, 5th, 7th and 9th grades completed self-report questionnaires on
co-rumination, friendship quality, depression and anxiety. Significant
gender differences emerged. Among girls, co-rumination predicted
increases in self-report depressive and anxious symptoms along with
increased positive friendship quality, which contributed to greater
co-rumination. Among boys, co-rumination did not predict increases in
depressive and anxious symptoms, only increased positive friendship
quality. Interestingly, developmental differences did not emerge, which
may reflect the young age of this sample. The results of this study
indicate that how boys and girls approach problems in their friendships
may differ and, furthermore, that this difference may be linked with
adjustment outcomes. Thus, it seems that for girls, co-rumination may
serve as a mechanism for emotional problems among friends while it
strengthens boys' friendships without any drawbacks.
A longer (one year) longitudinal study by Starr and Davila (2009)
had contradictory findings regarding the link between co-rumination and
psychological distress. Seventh and eighth grade girls completed
questionnaires relating to co-rumination, depressive symptoms and social
anxiety, peer relational style, self-perceived competence in peer
relationships, and romantic experiences. Data was collected again one
year later. Like previous findings (Rose, 2002; Rose et al., 2007), this
study found that co-rumination is related to increases in depressive
symptoms and positive aspects of friendship. However, in Starr and
Davila's study (2009), corumination, on average, did not predict
changes in depressive symptoms. Their study did find that co-rumination
correlated negatively with social anxiety. Furthermore, co-rumination
was associated with high levels of romantic experiences, and romantic
involvement moderated corumination's impact on depressive symptoms.
The authors suggest that further research should examine the conditions
under which co-rumination increases risk for depression. They also not
that co-rumination is a public form of self-disclosure, unlike
rumination which is private experience. Further research needs to attend
to this difference as well.
In the longest study to date, Stone and colleagues (2011) found
that co-rumination predicted the onset of depressive symptoms over a
two-year follow-up, and levels of corumination mediated the gender
difference in time to depression onset. This study was different from
previous work in that clinically significant depressive episodes were
assessed, not general psychological distress. Also, youth in this study
ranged from age 11 to 15 years, with a median age of 13. As with
previous work, this study found that girls reported significantly higher
levels of co-rumination than boys and girls exhibited a significantly
shorter time to onset of depression than boys. Results from Stone and
colleagues' study (2011) suggest that co-rumination might serve as
one mechanism for adolescent girls' increased risk for depression.
Summary of disclosure responses. Thus, there is evidence for a
variety of ways youth may respond to disclosures within their best
friendships. Clear gender differences have emerged, with girls
disclosing more (Burhmester and Prager 1995), having tighter focus in
their conversations and covering a smaller number of topics (Tannen
1990), and concentrating their talk on interpersonal problems and
concerns (Carlson, Schwartz, Luebbe and Rose 2006) than boys. In terms
of responses to disclosure, the findings are not so clear. Regarding
reciprocity of disclosure, it seems to increase with age (Rotenberg and
Chase 1992) but gender differences have not been explored. Girls and
boys do not seem to differ in levels of excessive reassurance seeking,
but it appears to have a negative impact only for girls (Prinstein et
al. 2005). Similarly, girls co-ruminate more than boys, and
co-rumination only has a negative impact on girls (Rose, Carlson and
Waller 2007; Stone et al. 2011).
There are several limitations to the described work. Many of the
disclosure studies were conducted with normative youth samples or
non-friend dyads (e.g., Cohn and Strassberg 1983; Rotenberg and Chase
1992). Furthermore, there is a pronounced need for observational studies
directly examining natural interactions between distressed youth and
their friends. In much of the past work, youth were directed to act in a
specific manner (e.g., discuss their day, Segrin and Flora 1998). There
have been few published studies in which researchers simply observe
youth interacting; most of these studies have focused on observations of
peers, not best friends. As noted earlier, best friendships offer a
unique context for development and are particular important to young
adolescents (Berndt 1999; 2004.) The formation of healthy friendships
has been hypothesized as playing a preventive role against the
development of psychopathology (Oland and Shaw 2005). Thus, an important
question to ask is: How do youth disclose appropriately and respond to
disclosures in ways that promote healthy friendships and do not
contribute to feelings of depression and anxiety?
Opportunities for Prevention
Given the connections between friendship and internalizing
disorders, it would seem prudent to consider the process of intimate
disclosure as an opportunity for prevention. While intimate disclosure
has been found to facilitate closeness among friends, it also appears to
have strong emotional costs, particularly for girls (e.g., Smith and
Rose 2011). When youth do disclose to each other, they may not respond
in the most helpful of ways. Some may continue to seek reassurance from
their friends and, in doing so, inadvertently damage their friendship
(e.g., Joiner and Metalsky 2001; Prinstein et al. 2005). Others,
particularly girls, may find themselves caught in a cycle of
co-rumination, which may increase their emotional suffering (e.g., Smith
and Rose 2011; Stone et al, 2011). Given that much of the research
points to a developmental shift that takes place as youth enter
adolescence (e.g., Avenevoli et al. 2008; Prinstein et al. 2005; Rose
2002; Tannen 1990), late childhood may offer a key time for prevention
efforts.
Perhaps most integral to prevention efforts is the unification of
the work on disclosure and internalizing problems. Researchers from
social, clinical and developmental psychology need to combine their
efforts to identify appropriate conversation efforts for youth.
Historians have noted that psychology today is hardly a unified
discipline (e.g., Goodwin 2008), with some arguing the concept of a
single field ought to be replaced with the idea of a set of
psychological studies (Koch 1984). While interdisciplinary work should
be encouraged, especially with sociolinguists (e.g., Tannen 1990), work
among the different areas of psychology is essential to the success of
any efforts.
Public health models often approach prevention at three levels
(Lorion, Myers, Bartels, and Dennis 1994). Primary prevention aims to
reduce the incidence of a problem before it occurs; secondary prevention
targets individuals by minimizing or reducing the severity and
continuation of a problem; and tertiary prevention attempts to minimize
the course of a problem once it is evident and causing harm (Wolfe and
Jaffe 1999). An important first step in developing prevention efforts
would be to identify skills involved in responding to intimate
disclosure that facilitate friendship support without sacrificing mental
wellness. Rather than view boys' discussions as being superficial,
perhaps they offer an opportunity to study alternate ways of providing
healthy support within a friendship. Interdisciplinary work between
social/clinical/developmental psychologists and sociocultural linguists
should be encouraged. Many constructs discussed in this paper seem to
overlap; understanding their connections could move this area forward.
Research also would need to determine the best developmental stage as
which to offer such conversational training. Primary prevention efforts
may be best utilized within elementary school aged children, while
secondary and tertiary efforts may be targeted toward middle and high
school aged adolescents.
There are a number of empirically validated programs that take
place within schools (where large groups of children can be reached
easily) focusing on the prevention of substance use, mental disorders,
and delinquency (for reviews, see Domitrovich and Greenberg 2000;
Greenberg, Kusche, Cook, and Quamma 1995). Programs that incorporate
social and emotional skills building (e.g., Greenberg, Kusche, Cook and
Quamma 1995) may add curriculum devoted to intimate disclosure. Theory
of emotional intelligence (Goleman 1995; Salovey and Mayer 1990)
suggests that self-awareness and social awareness are best learned
during childhood (Rathus 2011), linking back to the developmental piece.
Certainly, additional work is needed to ascertain the appropriate level
of prevention and how to best incorporate social and emotional skills
related to intimate disclosure. Future research may address prevention
strategies and curriculum development so that youth are better equipped
to respond to their friends' disclosures.
References
Achenbach, Thomas M. 1991. Manual for the Youth Self-Report and
1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of
Psychiatry.
Achenbach, Thomas M. and Craig Edelbrock. 1983. Manual for the
Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington:
University of Vermont, Department of Psychology.
Achenbach, Thomas M. and Stephanie H. McConaughy. 1992. Taxonomy of
internalizing disorders in children and adolescents. In W.M. Reynolds
(Ed.), Internalizing disorders in children and adolescents. New York,
NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Alden, Lynn E. and Peter Bieling. 1997. The interpersonal
consequences of safety behaviors. Behavior Research and Therapy, 36,
53-64.
Altman, Irwin and Dalmas A. Taylor. 1973. Social penetration:
Development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart,
and Winston.
Angold, Adrian, E. Jane Costello and Carol M. Worthman. 1998.
Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal
timing. Psychological Medicine, 28, 55-61.
Arkin, Robert M. (1981). Self-presentation styles. In J.T. Tedeschi
(Ed.), Impression management theory and social psychological research
(pp. 311-333). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Aschenbrand, Sasha G., Aleta G. Angelosante and Philip C. Kendall
2005. Discriminant validity and clinical utility of the CBCL with
anxiety-disordered youth. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Psychology, 34, 735-746.
Asher, Steven R., Jeffrey G. Parker, J.G. and Diane Walker. 1996.
Distinguishing friendship from acceptance: Implications for intervention
and assessment. In W.M. Bukowski, A.F. Newcomb, & W.W. Hartup
(Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Avenevoli, Shelli, Erin Knight, Ronald C. Kessler, and Kathleen
Ries Merikangas. 2008. Epidemiology of depression in children and
adolescents. In J.R. Z. Abela and B.L. Hankin (Eds.), Handbook of
depression in children and adolescents (pp. 6-32). New York: Guilford
Press.
Baxter, Leslie A. and William W. Wilmot. 1983. Communication
characteristics of relationships with differential growth rates.
Communication monographs, 50, 264-272.
Beck, Aaron T., Clay H. Ward, Myer Mendelson, John E. Mock and J.K.
Erlbaugh. 1961. An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of
General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571.
Berg-Nielsen, Turid Suzanne, A. Vika, and Alv A. Dahl. 2003. When
adolescents disagree with their mothers:
CBCL-YSR discrepancies related to maternal depression and
adolescent self-esteem. Child: Care, Health and Development, 29,
207-213.
Berndt, Thomas J. (1982). The features and effects of friendship in
early adolescence. Child Development, 53, 1447- 1460.
Berndt, Thomas J. 1992. Friendship and friends' influence in
adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 156-159.
Berndt, Thomas J. 2004. Children's friendships: Shifts over a
half-century in perspectives on their development and their effects.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 206-223.
Berndt, Thomas J. and T. Bridgett Perry. 1990. Distinctive features
and effects of early adolescent friendships. In R. Montemayor, G.R.
Adams, and T.P. Gullotta (Eds.), From childhood to adolescence: A
transitional period. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Berndt, Thomas J., Jacquelyn A. Hawkins, and Ziyi Jiao. 1999.
Influences of friends and friendships on adjustment to junior high
school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 13-41.
Blumberg, Stephen R. and Jack E. Hokanson. 1983. The effects of
another person's response style on interpersonal behavior in
depressives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 196-209.
Brady, Erika U. and Philip C. Kendall. 1992. Comorbidity of anxiety
and depression in children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 111
, 244-255.
Breslau, Naomi, Glenn C. Davis and Kenneth Prabucki. 1988.
Depressed mothers as informants in family history research--are they
accurate? Psychiatry Research, 24, 345-359.
Brown, Bradford B. 1990. Peer groups and peer cultures. In S.S.
Feldman & G.R. Elliot (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing
adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruch Monroe A., Janet M. Gorsky, Tom M. Collins and Pat A. Berger.
1989. Shyness and sociability reexamined: A multicomponent analysis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 904-915.
Buhrmester, Duane and Karen Prager. 1995. Patterns and functions of
self-disclosure during childhood and adolescence. In K.J. Rotenberg
(Ed.), Disclosure processes in children and adolescents (pp. 10-56). New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Buhrmester, Duane. 1990. Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal
competence, and adjustment during preadolescence and adolescence. Child
Development, 61, 1101-1111.
Buhrmester, Duane. 1996. Need fulfillment, interpersonal
competence, and the developmental contexts of early adolescent
friendship. In W.M. Bukowski, A.F. Newcomb, and W.W. Hartup (Eds.), The
company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 15-45).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Burchill, Sue Ann L. and William B. Stiles. 1988. Interactions of
depressed college students with their roommates: Not necessarily
negative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 410-419.
Burda, Phillip C., Jr., Alan C. Vaux, and Thomas Schill. 1984.
Social support resources: Variation in sex and sex-role. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 119-126.
Burns, Andrea B., Jessica S. Brown, E. Ashby Plant, Natalie
Sacs-Ericsson and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. (2006). On the specific
depressotypic nature of excessive reassurance-seeking. Personality and
Individual Differences, 40, 135-145.
Butler, Stephen M., Sherri A. MacKay and Susan E. Dickens. 1995.
Maternal and adolescent ratings of psychopathology in young offender and
non-clinical males. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 27,
333-342.
Caltabiano, Marie Louise and Michael Smithson. 1983. Variables
affecting the perception of self-disclosure appropriateness. The Journal
of Social Psychology, 120, 119-128.
Cantor, Nancy and Janet Malley. 1991. Life tasks, personal needs,
and close relationships. In G.J.O. Fletcher & F.D. Fincham (Eds.),
Cognition in close relationships (pp. 101-125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carlson, Wendela, Rebecca A. Schwartz, Aaron M. Luebbe and Amanda
J. Rose. 2006, March. Gender differences in frequency of problem types
during conversation with friends and ratings of problem salience. Poster
presented at Society for Research on Adolescence, San Francisco, CA.
Cheek, Jonathan M. and Arnold H. Buss. 1981. Shyness and
sociability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 330-339.
Chorpita, Bruce F., Eric L. Daleigden, Catherine Moffitt, Letitia
Yim and Lori A. Umemoto. 2000. Assessment of tripartite factors of
emotion in children and adolescents: I. Structural validity and
normative data of an affect and arousal scale. Journal of
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 22, 141-160.
Clark, David M. and Adrian Wells. 1995. A cognitive model of social
phobia. In R. Heimberg, M. Liebowitz, D. Hope, & F. Schneier (Eds.),
Social phobia, (pp. 69-93). New York: Guilford Press.
Clark, Lee Anna and David Watson. 1991. Tripartite model of anxiety
and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316-226.
Cohn, Nancy B. and Donald S. Strassberg. 1983. Self-disclosure
reciprocity among preadolescents. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 9, 97-102.
Cole, David A., Ruth Truglio and Lachlan Peeke. 1997. Relation
between symptoms of anxiety and depression in children: A
multitrait-multimethod-multigroup assessment. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 65, 110-119.
Compas, Bruce E., Lesley A. Slavin, Barry M. Wagner and Kathryn
Vannatta. 1986. Relationship of life events and social support with
psychological dysfunction among adolescents. Adolescence, 15, 205-221.
Compas, Bruce E., Sydney Ey and Kathryn E. Grant. 1993. Taxonomy,
assessment, and diagnosis of depression during adolescence.
Psychological Bulletin, 114, 323-344.
Cooper, Catherine R. and Susan Ayers-Lopez. 1985. Family and peer
systems in adolescence: New Models of the role of relationships in
development. Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 9-21.
Coyne, James C. 1976. Toward an interactional description of
depression. Psychiatry, 39, 28-40.
Cross, Susan E. and Laura Madson. 1997. Models of the self:
Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 5-37.
Daley, Shannon E. and Constance Hammen. 2002. Depressive symptoms
and close relationships during the transition to adulthood: Perspectives
from dysphoric women, their best friends, and their romantic partners.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 129-141.
Davies, Mark. 1982. Epidemiology of depressive mood in adolescents.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 1205-1212.
Derlega, Valerian J. and Janusz Grzelak. 1979. Appropriateness of
self-disclosure in social relationships. Journal of Social Issues, 33,
102-115.
Dindia, Kathryn and Mike Allen. 1992. Sex differences in
self-disclosure: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 106-124.
Domitrovich, Celine E. and Mark T. Greenberg. 2000. The study of
implementation: Current findings from effective programs that prevent
mental disorders in school-aged children. Journal of Educational and
Psychological Consultation, 11(2), 193-221.
Edison, Janis D. and Henry E. Adams. 1992. Depression, self-focus,
and social interaction. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 14, 1-19.
Elias, Maurice, Michael Ubriaco, Ann M. Reese, Michael A. Gara,
Peggy Rothbaum and Martha Haviland. 1992. A measure of adaptation to
problematic academic and interpersonal tasks of middle school. Journal
of School Psychology, 30, 41-57.
Flanery, Randall C. (1990). Methodological and psychometric
considerations in child reports. In A.M. La Greca (Ed.), Through the
eyes of a child: Obtaining self-reports from children and adolescents
(pp. 57-82). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Friedlander, Steven, Daniel S. Weiss and John Traylor. 1986.
Assessing the influence of maternal depression on the validity of the
Child Behavior Checklist. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14,
123-133.
Furman, Wyndol and Duane Buhrmester. 1992. Age and sex differences
in perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Development,
63, 103-115.
Glorisa Canino, Patrick E. Shrout, Maritza Rubio-Stipec, Hector R.
Bird, Milagros Bravo, Rafael Ramirez, Ligia Chavez, Margarita Alegria,
Jose J. Bauermeister, Ann Hohmann, Julio Ribera, Pedro Garcia, and
Alfonso Martinez-Taboas. (2004). The DSM-IV rates of child and
adolescent disorders in Puerto Rico: Prevalence, correlates, service
use, and the effects of impairment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61,
85-93.
Goleman, Daniel. 1995. Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam
Books.
Goodwin, C. James. 2008. A History of Modern Psychology (3rd
ed^.Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Goodwin, C. James. 2008. A history of modern psychology, 3rd
edition. New York: Wiley & Sons.
Gore, Susan, Robert H. Aseltine, Jr. and Mary Ellen Colten. 1993.
Gender, social-relational involvement, and depression. Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 3, 101-125.
Gotlib, Ian H. and Anne L. Robinson. 1982. Responses to depressed
individuals: Discrepancies between self-report and observer-rated
behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 231-240.
Gotlib, Ian H. and Constance L. Hammen. 1992. Psychological aspects
of depression: Toward a cognitive interpersonal integration. London:
Wiley.
Gotlib, Ian H. and Mary Ellen Beatty. 1985. Negative responses to
depression: The role of attributional style. Cognitive Therapy and
Research, 9, 91-103.
Greenberg, Mark T., Carol A. Kusche, Elizabeth T. Cook and Julie P.
Quamma. 1995. Promoting emotional competence in school-aged children:
The effects of the PATHS curriculum. Development and Psychopathology, 7,
117-136.
Hammen, Constance and Karen D. Rudolph. 199). Childhood depression.
In E.J. Mash and R.A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (pp.
13-195). New York: Guilford Press.
Hankin, Benjamin L., Lyn Y. Abramson, Terrie E. Moffitt, Rob McGee,
Phil A. Silva and Kathryn E. Angell. 1998. Development of depression
from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a
10year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128-140.
Harrington, Richard, Diana Bredenkamp, Christine Groothues, Michael
Rutter, Hazel Fudge and Andrew Pickles. 1994. Adult outcomes of
childhood and adolescent depression: III. Links with suicidal
behaviours. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1309-1319.
Hartup, Willard W. 1996. The company they keep" Friendships
and their development significance. Child Development, 67, 1-13.
Hartup, Willard W. 2000. Middle childhood: Socialization and social
context. In A. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology. Washington, DC
and NY: American Psychological Association and Oxford University Press.
Hecht, Debra B., Heidi M. Inderbitzen and Anita L. Bukowski. 1998.
The relationship between peer status and depressive symptoms in children
and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 26, 153-160.
Heller, Michelle C. and Junko Tanaka-Matsumi. 1999. A sequential
analysis of depressive behaviors within adolescents peer interactions.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 21, 249-273.
Hogue, Aaron and Laurence Steinberg, L. 1995. Homophily of
internalized distress in adolescent peer groups. Developmental
Psychology, 31, 897-906.
Hussong, Andrea M. 2000. Perceived peer context and adolescent
adjustment. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10, 391-415.
Jacobson, Neil S. and Erling A. Anderson. 1982. Interpersonal skill
deficits and depression in college students: A sequential analyses of
the timing of self-disclosure. Behavior Therapy, 13, 271-282.
Jacques, Heather A.K. and Eric J. Mash. 2004. A test of the
tripartite model of anxiety and depression in elementary and high school
boys and girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 13-25.
Jensen, Peter S., John Traylor, Stephen N. Xenakis and Harry Davis.
1988. Child psychopathology rating scales and interrater agreement: I.
Parents' gender and psychiatric symptoms. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 442-450.
Joiner, Thomas E. 1999. A test of interpersonal theory of
depression in youth psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 1 , 77-85.
Joiner, Jr., Thomas E. and Gerald I. Metalsky. 2001. Excessive
reassurance seeking: Delineating a risk factor involved in the
development of depressive symptoms. Psychological Science, 12, 371-378.
Joiner, Jr., Thomas E. and John Barnett. 1994. A test of
interpersonal theory of depression in children and adolescents using a
projective technique. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 595-609.
Joiner, Jr., Thomas E., Gerald I. Metalsky, Jennifer Katz and
Steven R.H. Beach. 1999. Depression and excessive reassurance-seeking.
Psychological Inquiry, 10, 269-278.
Joiner, Thomas E. 1994. Contagious depression: Existence,
specificity to depressed symptoms, and the role of reassurance seeking.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 28-296.
Joiner, Thomas E. and Norman B. Schmidt. 1998. Excessive
reassurance-seeking predicts depressive but not anxious reactions to
acute stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 533-537.
Joiner, Thomas E., Mark S. Alfano and Gerald I. Metalsky. 1992.
When depression breeds contempt: Reassurance-seeking, self-esteem, and
rejection of depressed college students by their roommates. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 101, 165-173.
Jones, Warren H and Bruce N. Carpenter. 1986. Shyness, social
behavior, and relationships. In W.H. Jones J.M. Cheek, & S. Briggs
(Eds.), Shyness: Perspectives on research and treatment (pp. 227-238).
New York: Plenum.
Jourad, Sidney M. 1959. Cognition, cathexis and the "dyadic
effect" in men's self-disclosing behavior. Merrill Palmer
Quarterly, 6, 179-186.
Judd, Lewis L., Hagop S. Akiskal, Jack D. Maser, Pamela J. Zeller,
Jean Endicott and William Coryell. 1998. A prospective 12-year study of
subsyndromal and syndromal depressive symptoms in unipolar major
depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 694-700.
Kessler, Ronald C. and Jane D. McLeod. 1984. Sex differences in
vulnerability to undesirable life events. American Sociological Review,
49, 620-631.
Koch, Sigmund. (1984, September). Psychology Versus the
Psychological Studies. In Symposium: Psychology in the Future (p. 175).
Paper read at XXIII International Congress of Psychology, Acapulco,
Mexico.
Koch, Sigmund. September, 1984. Psychology Versus the Psychological
Studies. In Symposium: Psychology in the Future (p. 175). Paper read at
XXIII International Congress of Psychology, Acapulco, Mexico.
Kovacs, Maria and Bernie Devlin. 1998. Internalizing disorders in
childhood. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 47-63.
Kovacs, Maria. 1980. Rating scales to assess depression in
school-aged children. Acta Paedopsychiatry, 46, 305315.
Leary, Mark R., Paul D. Knight and Kelly A. Johnson. 1987. Social
anxiety and dyadic conversation: A verbal response analysis. Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 34-50.
Lewinsohn, Peter M., Ari Solomon, John R. Seeley and Antonette
Zeiss. 2000. Clinical implications of 'subthreshold'
depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 345-351.
Lewinsohn, Peter M., Paul Rhode, John R. Seeley, Daniel N. Klein
and Ian H. Gotlib. 2003. Psychosocial functioning of young adults who
have experienced and recovered from major depressive disorder during
adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 353-363.
Lorion, Raymond P. Tracy G. Myers, Carol Bartels and Alisa Dennis.
1994. Preventive intervention research: Pathways for extending knowledge
of child/adolescent health and pathology. In Advances in clinical child
psychology. Vol. 16. T.H. Ollendick and R.J. Prinz, eds. New York:
Plenum Press, pp. 109-39.
Maccoby, Eleanor E. 1990. Gender and relationships: A developmental
account. American Psychologist, 45, 513-520.
March, John S., James Parker, Kevin Sullivan, Patricia Stallings,
and Keith Conners. 1997. The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children
(MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 554-565.
Mariano, Kristin A. and Helen C. Harton. 2005. Similarities in
aggression, inattention/ hyperactivity, depression, and anxiety in
middle childhood friendships. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
24, 471-496.
McAdams, Dan P. 1984. Human motives and personal relationships. In
V.J. Derlega (Ed.), Communication, intimacy, and close relationships
(pp. 41-70). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Meleshko, Kenneth George Andrew and Lynn E. Alden. 1993. Anxiety
and self-disclosure: Toward a motivational model. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 64, 1000-1009.
Michael, Kurt D. and Kenneth W. Merrell. 1998. Reliability of
children's self-reported internalizing symptoms over short to
medium-length time intervals. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 194-201.
Miller, Lynn C. And David A. Kenny. 1986. Reciprocity of
self-disclosure at the individual and dyadic levels: A social relations
analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 713-719.
Millon, Theodore M. 1981. Disorders of personality: DSMIII: Axis
II. New York: Wiley.
Mineka, Susan, David Watson and Lee Anna Clark. 1998. Comorbidity
of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 49,
377-412.
Muris, Peter, Henk Schmidt, Harald Merckelbach and Erik Schouten.
2001. The structure of negative emotions in adolescents. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 331-337.
Najman, Jake M., Gail M. Williams, Jane Nikles, Sue Spence, William
Bor, Michael O'Callaghan, Robyn LeBrocque, Margaret J. Andersen and
G.J. Shuttlewood. 2001. Bias influencing maternal reports of child
behaviour and emotional state. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology, 36, 186-194.
Newcomb, Andrew F. and Catherine L. Bagwell. 1996. The
developmental significance of children's friendship relations (pp.
289-321). In W.M. Bukowski, A.F. Newcomb, and W.W. Hartup (Eds.), The
company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Nolen-Hoeksema, Susan, Joan S. Girgus and Martin E. P. Seligman,
M.E.P. 1992. Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive
symptoms: A five-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 101, 405-422.
Oland, Alyssa A. and Daniel S. Shaw. 2005. Pure versus co-occurring
externalizing and internalizing symptoms in children: The potential role
of socio-developmental milestones. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
Review, 8, 247-270.
Ollendick, Thomas H. 1983. Reliability and validity of the Revised
Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSSC-R). Behavior Research and
Therapy, 21, 685-692.
Papsdorf, Michael and Lynn Alden. 1998. Mediators of social
rejection in social anxiety: Similarity, self-disclosure, and overt
signs of anxiety. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 351-369.
Parker, Jeffrey G., Kenneth H. Rubin, Stephen Earth, Julie C.
Wojslawowicz and Allison A. Buskirk. (2006). Peer relationships, child
development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathology
perspective. In D. Cicchetti (Ed), Developmental Psychopathology: Vol.
1: Theory and method. New York: Wiley.
Philips, Beth M., Christopher J. Lonigan, Kimberly Driscoll and
Eric S. Hooe. 2002. Positive and negative affectivity in children: A
multitrait-multimethod investigation. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 31, 465-179.
Pickles, Andrew, Richard Rowe, Emily Simonoff, Debra Foley, Michael
Rutter and Judy Silberg. 2001. Child psychiatric symptoms and
psychosocial impairment: Relationship and prognostic significance.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 230-235.
Pine, Daniel S., Elizabeth Cohen, Patricia Cohen and Judith Brook.
1999. Adolescent depressive symptoms as predictors of adult depression:
Moodiness or mood disorder? American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 133135.
Potthoff, John G., Charles J. Holahan, C.J. and Thomas E. Joiner.
1995. Reassurance seeking, stress generation, and depressive symptoms:
An integrative model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
664-670.
Prinstein, Mitchell J., Jessica L. Borelli, Charissa S.L. Cheah,
Valerie A. Simon and Julie Wargo Aikins. 2005. Adolescent girls'
interpersonal vulnerability to depressive symptoms: A longitudinal
examination of reassurance-seeking and peer relationships. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 114, 676-688.
Radloff, Lenore. 1977. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression
scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological
Measurement, 1, 385-401.
Rathus, Spencer A. 2011. Childhood and adolescence: Voyages in
development, 4th edition. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Cengage
Learning.
Reynolds, William M. 1992. Depression in children and adolescents.
In W. M. Reynolds (Ed.), Internalizing disorders in children and
adolescents (pp. 149-253). New York: John Wiley.
Reynolds, Cecil R. and Bert O. Richmond. 1985. Revised
Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) manual. Los Angeles:
Western Psychological Services.
Reynolds, William M. 1986. Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale.
Odessa, TX: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Rose, Amanda J. 2002. Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and
boys. Child Development, 73, 1830-1843.
Rose, Amanda J., and Karen D. Rudolph. 2006. A review of sex
differences in peer relationship processes: Potential tradeoffs for the
emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological
Bulletin, 132, 98131.
Rose, Amanda J., Wendy Carlson and Erika M. Waller. 2007.
Prospective associations of co-rumination with friendship and emotional
adjustment: Considering the socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination.
Developmental Psychology, 43, 1019-1031.
Rotenberg, Ken J. 1995. Disclosure processes: An introduction. In
K.J. Rotenberg (Ed.), Disclosure processes in children and adolescents
(pp. 1-9. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rotenberg, Ken J. and Chase, Nancy. 1992. Development of the
reciprocity of self-disclosure. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153,
75-86.
Rotenberg, Ken J., and Dave Sliz. 1998. Children's restrictive
disclosure to friends. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 203-215.
Rubin, Kenneth H., Julie C. Wojslawowicz, Linda Rose-Krasnor,
Cathryn Booth-LaForce and Kim B. Burgess. 2006. The best friendships of
shy/withdrawn children: Prevalence, stability, and relationship quality.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 143-157.
Rubin, Kenneth H., Kathleen M. Dwyer, Cathryn L. Booth-LaForce,
Angel H. Kim, Kim B. Burgess and Linda
Rose-Krasnor. 2004. Attachment, friendship, and psychosocial
functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 24,
326-356.
Rudolph, Karen D. 2002. Gender differences in emotional responses
to interpersonal stress during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 30, 3-13.
Rudolph, Karen D., and Colleen S. Conley. 2005. The socioemotional
costs and benefits of social-evaluative concerns: Do girls care too
much? Journal of Personality, 73, 115-137.
Ruscher, Susan M. and Ian H. Gotlib. 1988. Marital interaction
patterns of couples with an without a depressed partner. Behavior
Therapy, 19, 455-470.
Rutter, Michael. 1991. Age changes in depressive disorders: Some
developmental considerations (pp. 274-300). In J. Garber and K.A. Dodge
(Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Salovey, Peter and John D. Mayer. 1990. Emotional intelligence.
Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185211.
Segrin, Chris and Jeanne Flora. 1998. Depression and verbal
behavior in conversation with friends and strangers. Journal of Language
and Social Psychology, 17, 492-503.
Segrin, Chris and Lyn Y. Abramson. 1994. Negative reactions to
depressive behaviors: A communication theories analysis. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 103, 655-668.
Segrin, Chris. 1990. A meta-analytic review of social skill
deficits in depression. Communication Monographs, 57, 292-308.
Smith, Rhiannon L., and Amanda J. Rose. 2011. The "cost of
caring" in youths' friendships: Considering associations among
social perspective taking, co-rumination, and empathic distress.
Developmental Psychology, 47, 1792-1803.
Sorenson, Susan B., Carolyn M. Rutter and Carol S. Aneshensel.
1991. Depression in the community: An investigation into age of onset.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 541-546.
Speilberger, , Charles D. 1973. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory for Children. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Starr, Lisa R. and Joanne Davila. 2009. Clarifying co-rumination:
Associations with internalizing symptoms and romantic involvement among
adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 19-37.
Stone, Lindsey B., Benjamin L Hankin., Brandon E. Gibb and John
R.Z. Abela. 2011. Co-rumination predicts the onset of depressive
disorders during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120,
752-757.
Strack, Stephen and James C. Coyne. 1983. Social confirmation of
dysphoria: Shared and private reactions to depression. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 798-806.
Sullivan, Henry S. 1953. The interpersonal theory of psychiatry.
New York: Norton.
Susman, Elizabeth J., Lorah D. Dorn and George P. Chrousos. 1991.
Negative affect and hormone levels in young adolescents: Concurrent and
predictive perspectives. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 167-190.
Tannen, Deborah. 1990. Gender differences in topical coherence:
Creating involvement in best friends' talk. Discourse Processes,
13, 73-90.
Thomas, Amanda McCombs, Rex Forehand, Lisa Armistead, Michelle
Wierson, and Rob Fauer. 1990. Crossinformant consistency in
externalizing and internalizing problems in early adolescence. Journal
of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 12, 255-262.
Vernberg, Eric M. 1990. Psychological adjustment and experiences
with peers during early adolescence: Reciprocal, incidental, or
unidirectional relationships? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18,
187-198.
Watson, David, Lee Anna Clark, Kris Weber, Jana Smith Assenheimer,
Milton E. Strauss and Richard A. McCormick. 1995. Testing a tripartite
model: II. Exploring the symptoms structure of anxiety and depression in
student, adult, and patient samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
104, 15-25.
Weissman, Myrna M., Susan Wolk, Rise B. Goldstein, Donna Moreau,
Philip Adams, Steven Greenwald, Claudia M. Klier, Neal D. Ryan, Ronald
E. Dahl and Priya Wickramaratne. 1999. Depressed adolescents grown up.
JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 18, 1707-1713.
Weissman, Myrna, Helen Orvashel and Nancy Padian. 1980.
Children's symptom and social functioning self-report scales:
Comparison of mothers' and children's reports. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 168, 736740.
Wolfe, David A., and Peter G. Jaffe. 1999. Emerging strategies in
the prevention of domestic violence. Domestic Violence and Children, 9,
133-144.
Worthy, Morgan, Albert L. Gary and Gay M. Kahn. 1969.
Self-disclosure as an exchange process. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 13, 59-63.
Youniss, James. 1980. Parents and peers in social development: A
Sullivan-Piaget perspective. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zahn-Waxler, Carolyn, Bonnie Klimes-Dougan and Marcia J. Slattery.
2000. Internalizing problems of childhood and adolescence: Prospects,
pitfalls, and progress in understanding the development of anxiety and
depression. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 443-466.
Allison Buskirk-Cohen, Assistant Professor, Counseling Psychology,
Delaware Valley College