An exploratory investigation of identity negotiation and tattoo removal.
Shelton, Jeremy A. ; Peters, Cara
Consumption choices affect how others view the individual consumer,
which influences her decision-making. Less obvious, but no less
important, is that consumption choices also affect how the consumer
views herself (Shrauger and Schoeneman 1979), another factor that
influences decision-making (e.g., Belk 1988; Schouten 1991; Solomon
1983). Recent research in consumer behavior has begun to explore the
effect of product disposition on the consumer's self-concept (e.g.,
Kates 2001; Price, Arnould, and Curasi 2000; Young 1991). McAlexander
(1991), for example, found that during a divorce people will dispose of
objects associated with their former partner as a method of
"breaking free." The present study explores how
consumers' disposition decisions are tied to their self-concepts
within the domain of tattoo removal.
Tattoo removal provides a relevant context for exploring how
identity negotiation motivates disposition. Because a tattoo is part of
a person's physical body, it may have a stronger influence on the
self-concept when compared to the typical external possession (cf,
McClelland 1951; Prelinger 1959). Furthermore, an originally designed
tattoo represents a possession in which the consumer invests significant
time and effort in creating the design. Such personal investments may
translate into a deeper connection between the possession and the
self-concept (cf. McClelland 1951; Prelinger 1959; Sartre 1943). In
addition, the consumer may make an even greater personal investment
(i.e., financially and psychologically) to remove a tattoo when compared
to the disposal of a typical external possession because tattoo removal
is expensive, painful, and may not be completely successful (i.e.,
scars). Finally, tattoo removal is a relatively permanent form of
disposition because it involves intentional destruction compared to the
more porous selling or giving away of a possession (e.g., Kates 2001;
Lastovicka and Fernandez 2005; Price et al. 2000; Young 1991).
Therefore, tattoo removal serves as fertile ground for examining the
identity-related motivations for disposition.
The purpose of the present investigation is to examine disposition
as a means of identity negotiation using Brewer's (1991) optimal
distinctiveness theory as the conceptual foundation. This study unpacks
the relationship between disposition and the different aspects of the
self-concept. After a brief review of research identifying the reasons
consumers have for acquiring and removing tattoos, this paper discusses
optimal distinctiveness theory, followed by an overview of the method,
key findings, and a discussion of the study's implications.
CONCEPTUAL FOUNDATION
Motives for tattoo acquisition and removal. Historically in the
United States tattooing is perceived as a mark of social deviance
associated with bikers, sailors, convicts, gang members and prostitutes
(DeMello 2000; Rubin 1988; Sanders 1989). Within the last two decades,
however, the meaning of tattoos has begun to morph as the activity has
spread into mainstream society. Tattooing now represents a legitimate
art form, practiced by trained artists on members of middle-class
society (DeMello 2000).
Research finds that mainstream consumers have a wide variety of
reasons for being tattooed. Table 1 presents a summary of motives for
tattoo acquisition that research has uncovered. Collectively, the
reasons for tattoo acquisition seem to be methods of influencing the
self-concept (Sanders 1989). Furthermore, these motivations appear
consistent with those from research on defining the self via symbolic
consumption (e.g., Belk 1988).
The most commonly reported reason to obtain a tattoo is to enhance
one's sense of individuality (DeMello 2000). For example, some
people use their tattoo as a personal narrative to commemorate an event
or significant period in their life (Velliquette, Murray, and Creyer
1998), such as celebrating an accomplishment of the bearer (Gritton
1988). Others use a tattoo as a symbol representing some aspect of their
personal identity, such as a nickname, an animal, or a hobby (Sanders
1989; Velliquette et al. 1998). Furthermore, a person may choose to
individuate herself as an act of rebellion.
According to DeMello (2000) tattoos also symbolize a personal
connection to higher powers. For example, popular tribal tattoos are
often self-designed by the consumer. The process of design, the tribal
symbolism, and the acquisition process, come together to represent a
personal ritual that in turn develops a deeper connection to the
spiritual sense of self. Similarly, some consumers view their tattoo as
an outward expression of the personal control they have over their body,
while others cite empowerment and personal growth as chief motivations
for obtaining tattoos. In these cases, the tattoo serves as a symbol of
the person's feeling of a heightened sense of control over the self
and may even accompany positive changes in lifestyle (e.g., being drug
free; DeMello 2000). A tattoo may also enhance a person's body
aesthetic. Besides cosmetic tattooing, which applies ink as a form of
permanent makeup, tattoos themselves may be used to hide a scar or
simply draw attention to a particular body part (Sanders 1989).
Tattoos may also communicate social aspects of a person's
identity. Tattoos can be a marker of group membership, such as a
fraternity (Sanders 1989). In other cases the individual may acquire a
tattoo as a prerequisite for group membership, as is the case of group
initiation rituals. In fact, as the tattoo culture becomes more
mainstream (DeMello 1995), a person may get a tattoo as an act of
conformity. Finally, personal relationships also serve as a common
reason to get a tattoo. These tattoos are usually the name of a
person's romantic partner and serve as a symbol of the
wearer's devotion to the relationship (Sanders 1989), and are more
common among women (Watson 1998).
Despite the increasing ubiquity of tattooing, the acquisition
process does contain risk. There is physiological risk because tattooing
is somewhat painful and there is a slight risk of contracting a disease
(e.g., Long and Rickman 1994). There is also psychological risk because
the tattoo could result in negative social reactions or not be what the
customer desired (e.g., low quality). Finally, there is financial risk
because tattooing is rather expensive. Regardless of the risks, many
consumers make impulsive decisions to be tattooed without much knowledge
about the process or the ramifications of their decision (Sanders 1988).
An impulsive decision could lead to buyer's remorse. Although
several researchers have investigated consumer's reasons for
acquiring a tattoo, little research explores consumer regret after
obtaining an initial tattoo.
Sanders (1985) found approximately 33% of tattoo consumers (n =
163) experienced regret after their purchase. Only two studies have
explored why some consumers ultimately had their tattoo removed.
Armstrong et al. (1996) surveyed 105 participants and the most common
reasons given were: to help me feel better about myself, tired of the
tattoo; be more credible with friends; prevent people judging based on
the tattoo; separate from previous life experiences; and remove a label
associated with belonging to a bad group. A similar survey of 68
patients at a private clinic indicated that improvement of self-esteem;
social reasons; family pressure; improving potential for employment; and
change of partner were the most common reasons for tattoo removal (Varma
and Lanigan 1999). Table 1 also summarizes reasons for tattoo removal.
As with tattoo acquisition, consumers' reasons for removal are tied
to their self-concepts.
Optimal Distinctiveness Theory. The list of reasons for tattoo
acquisition and removal can be dichotomized as attempts to influence a
sense of differentiation from or assimilation to others. Enhancing a
sense of individuality, commemorating a life event, expressing an aspect
of one's personality, rebelling, spirituality, a sense of control
and improving body aesthetics are actions that further differentiate a
person. These motivations are people's attempts to further define
who they are by distinguishing themselves from others. In contrast,
marking a group membership, conforming to normative influence, and
displaying romantic devotion to another are actions that further
assimilate a person. These motivations represent attempts to integrate
within a larger collective.
In her optimal distinctiveness theory, Brewer (1991) suggests that
the need to be an individual (i.e., differentiation) and the need to
belong to social groups (i.e., assimilation) are the two primary
motivations that comprise a person's self-concept. Brewer finds
that an individual is constantly negotiating the need to individuate the
self from others and integrate the self within social groups as she
searches for and defines her identity over time. For example, students
of the same university have a shared collective identity that satisfies
an assimilation need, while at the same time they see themselves as
distinct from another university's students to satisfy a
differentiation need. Sometimes, however, a group may become too large,
creating a greater sense of assimilation in its members as just another
face in the crowd. In response members may further subdivide into more
distinct groups to enhance feelings of differentiation. Thus students at
a large university can feel too assimilated and therefore further
subdivide into more distinct groups, such as sororities or sports clubs.
Another important aspect of Brewer's optimal distinctiveness
theory is the distinction between group memberships and social
identities. Brewer argues that a person's social identities are not
the same as his or her memberships in groups or social categories.
Membership in social categories may not always be voluntary, as in the
case of a person's ethnicity, whereas an individual chooses her
social identities. Although a person may belong to a variety of social
categories, she chooses to identify with those groups and social
categories that allow her to successfully negotiate a symbiosis between
her needs for assimilation and differentiation.
In 1996, Brewer and Gardner extended optimal distinctiveness theory
to include a tripartite representation of the self-concept. Specifically
they find that the self is composed of individual, relational, and
collective components. These components are "interactive" and
"coexist" within the same individual (Sedikedes and Brewer
2001, p. 2). The individual component represents unique traits and
characteristics a person possesses. Contrasting these traits and
characteristics with those of others creates a sense of individual-self.
An action that further differentiates a person from others will enhance
a sense of individual-self (Sedikedes and Brewer 2001). The relational
component represents the role a person plays in meaningful dyadic personal relationships with others (e.g., parent-child, friendships, and
romantic involvements). Reflected appraisal from relationship partners
creates a sense of relational-self. Actions that further assimilate a
person with these significant others will enhance a sense of
relational-self. The collective component represents larger social group
memberships that do not require the same type of close relationships
that comprise the relational-self (Sedikedes and Brewer 2001). Factors
that separate in-group membership from out-group membership create a
sense of collective-self. Any actions that further assimilate a person
into these large social groups will enhance a sense of collective-self
(Sedikedes and Brewer 2001).
Prior disposition research suggests that motivations for
disposition potentially stem from the three components of the self. For
example, Kates' (2001) found that people dying of AIDS used the
disposition of their possessions to enhance familial bonds via gift
giving, or distance disliked family members via exclusion during
disposition. These findings are indicative of consumers using
disposition to influence the relational-self. The collective-self may
also motivate disposition such as a person changing her hairstyle after
joining a new social organization (McAlexander and Schouten 1989) or
giving up an old car associated with partying to mark the transition
from "young rebel" to "mature adult" (Young 1991, p.
36). Finally, the individual-self also seems to motivate disposition.
For instance, the elderly will bequeath a favorite possession as a means
of achieving symbolic immortality (Price, Arnould, and Curasi 2000).
The focus of the present study is to utilize the conceptual
framework of optimal distinctiveness theory to better understand
disposition as a means of identity negotiation. Prior literature in
marketing primarily explores disposition within specific settings such
as garage sales (Lastovicka and Fernandez 2005), death (Kates 2001) or
divorce (McAlexander, Schouten, and Roberts 1993). Optimal
distinctiveness theory should provide further insight into when people
are likely to engage in disposition. Few of these prior works attempt to
understand the factors that precipitate disposition at a broader
theoretical level. To that end, this paper turns toward a discussion of
the method.
RESEARCH DESIGN
Data collection followed the general approach of psychological
phenomenological interviewing (Creswell 1998; Moustakas 1994; Thompson,
Locander, and Pollio 1989). Although the study was phenomenological in
nature, the authors participated in several ethnographic activities
prior to formally collecting the interview data. During a two-year
period, the authors sought to develop a deeper understanding of the
tattoo subculture, progressively narrowing their focus on removal. At
first, the authors read books on the history of tattooing (e.g., Gilbert
2000; Mifflin 2001; Schiffmacher and Riemschneider 2001). Next, the
authors purchased several tattoo magazines (Flash, Tattoo, and Skin Art)
from a local bookstore and reviewed their contents. After realizing that
these sources focused more on acquisition and thus provided little
information on removal, the authors informally interviewed two tattoo
artists while working in parlors; visited one web site that discussed
tattoo removal; inquired with three offices housing plastic surgeons
that conducted tattoo removal; and had two informal conversations with
consumers who had tattoos and have considered removal. These activities
provided a richer understanding of tattoo acquisition and its tie to
removal.
The sampling procedure for the study was based on the criterion of
wanting or having a tattoo removed (Miles and Huberman 1994). Informants
for the study were recruited from three newspaper advertisements. One ad
was placed in the primary newspaper in a large, southern city. Two
similar ads were placed in a local newspaper and a college campus
newspaper within a medium sized, southern city. All of the
advertisements offered a small incentive for an interview with consumers
who have had a tattoo removed or intended to have a tattoo removed.
The authors obtained twenty-two informants who ranged in age from
nineteen to forty three. The sample consisted of both males and females
(although more women than men responded to the ad) and represented a
diverse set of educational backgrounds and occupations. For example, the
sample varied from students, who held part time jobs, to highly educated
professionals within the fields of health care, real estate, and
services. Table 2 summarizes the demographic information of the
respondents. It is important to note that several of the respondents
felt that tattoo removal was a deeply personal topic, and they asked
that they not be identified in the final report. Thus, to ensure
confidentiality, all the names reported in this article are pseudonyms.
Furthermore, the authors have intentionally omitted some detail to
respect the privacy of the informants.
The sixty-minute, unstructured, depth interviews were audio taped
and transcribed. The data were analyzed and interpreted according to the
protocol for phenomenology suggested by Moustakas' (1994). This
protocol consists of four steps: "epoche, phenomenological
reduction, imaginative variation, and synthesis" (Moustakas 1994,
p. 84). Epoche involves setting aside "prejudgments and
biases" in order to focus on the topic under investigation (p. 85).
Phenomenological reduction involves "describing in textural
language just what one sees ... revealing the qualities of the
experience" (p. 90). Imaginative variation is the act of using
"varying frames of references, employing polarities and reversals,
and approaching the phenomenon from divergent perspectives ... to arrive
at the underlying precipitating factors" that account for the
experience (p. 98). Synthesis is the "integration of the
fundamental textural and structural descriptions into a unified
statement of the essences of the experience" (p. 100).
The authors sought to analyze and interpret the data in the spirit
of Moustakas' (2004) approach. First, the authors bracketed their
biases and prejudgments. Next, turning to the interview data, the
authors moved back and forth between the transcripts and the complete
set of data to identify significant statements (i.e., textural language)
from each of the informants' reported experiences. Using various
perspectives and constantly questioning the analysis, these significant
statements were combined into "meaning units" (Creswell 1998,
p. 150) that represent the experience (i.e., imaginative variation).
Third, the authors developed a thick description and identified specific
quotes to elaborate on the informants' experiences (i.e.,
synthesis). In the final step, the authors developed an interpretive description of each meaning unit in light of existing theory, attempting
to capture the essence of the tattoo removal experience as it relates to
larger theoretical considerations.
Two methods of validation were used to ensure accurate
representation and reliability of the data: respondent validation and
the constant comparative method. To achieve respondent validation, the
first author went back to several subjects with tentative results to
refine and confirm the findings. In addition, using the constant
comparative method (repeated to and fro between data parts), the
investigators inspected and compared all the data fragments within the
phenomenon under study. For reporting purposes, the authors chose to
present several of the respondents' experiences with an
interpretation woven into each of the individual stories. In contrast to
reporting respondents' experiences in a piecemeal fashion via
thematic categories, the authors believe that the individual story
format most clearly illustrates the conceptual framework for
understanding identity negotiation via tattoo removal. For completeness,
table 3 reports both a summary of informants' meaning statements
for why they obtained the tattoo and wanted or had the tattoo removed,
as well as how long they considered tattoo acquisition. Within table 3
informants are also grouped according to which aspect of the self the
informant is attempting to influence by their tattoo removal.
FINDINGS
The data were analyzed and interpreted in light of Brewer's
optimal distinctiveness theory. Optimal distinctiveness theory suggests
that an individual is constantly negotiating the need to individuate the
self from others and integrate the self within relational and collective
social groups as she searches for and defines her identity over time.
Thus, the authors sought to unpack the identity negotiation strategies
of the informants as they relate to tattoo removal.
For many consumers the process of deciding to acquire and remove a
tattoo is multi-layered and complex. A variety of factors in the
consumer's life, such as past events, others' reactions to the
tattoo, the wearer's social identities, alterations of the social
environment, and even possible future events may affect the
tattoo's identity ramifications for the consumer. A tattoo's
relationship to all of these personal factors makes the decision to
acquire or remove a tattoo deeply embedded within the consumer's
life as a whole. Therefore, the authors chose to present cases that
provided vivid detail regarding the consumer's life situation at
the time of acquisition and removal. Although the authors report
interview data for only a subset of the sample, table 3 suggests that
virtually all respondents, regardless of how simple or complex the life
story, acquired and removed (or planned to) their tattoo while
negotiating feelings of differentiation or assimilation.
Maggie-Trying to Be a Mature Individual with a Rebellious Tattoo
Maggie got her two tattoos when she was in high school. Her
boyfriend, who had tattoos of his own, was part of a rather deviant
group with which Maggie ended up spending a lot of time.
I had a boyfriend who was into heavy metal music and was a
rebellious outsider and he had tattoos. I thought it was cool, being
rebellious against my age and society. I was seventeen and I wasn't
supposed to have them, legally. The tattoo was a demon holding a
crystal ball as if it was telling a fortune. It was something that
was just on the wall. It was kind of satanic. It ran through my mind
at the time, maybe I should get something more feminine or girly,
that tattoo is kind of evil. I guess I was kind of influenced by my
boyfriend being there and from what he had. That was kind of his
thing. I seriously don't know what made me decide on that tattoo
other than the influence or pressure of getting something that went
with the type of people we hung out with, the clothes, and the music
we listened to. I felt that I was kind of leaning toward what they
thought was cool.
Maggie was trying to conform to her group of friends when she
decided which tattoo to acquire. This is a classic example of tattoo
acquisition to influence the collective-self and be more accepted by a
group. Thus, Maggie's tattoo enhanced her feelings of affiliation
with the group with which she interacted with regularly.
After Maggie finished college, however, her life started to change
in a number of important ways.
I finished college and then decided I needed to get a real job. That
was a wake-up call, so to speak. I was maturing. I had my own place
and really had some time to myself thinking and really looking
inside myself and discovering this (the tattoo) is not me. I'm not
listening to the same music, not partying the amount that I was,
didn't hang around with the same people. I had changed pretty much
everything: the way I wore makeup, the way I wore my hair, the way I
dressed, the way I carried myself, who I hung out with, everything.
I was just really discovering who I was, or who I wanted to be, and
what I wanted out of life. A lot of things were going through my
mind; that this (the tattoo) really does not fit my lifestyle or
what I want or choose to be. I got to thinking about that, and it
just was no longer appealing to me to see myself in another way.
When I got out of the shower and looking in the mirror and I see
that (the tattoo), it just reminded me that maybe I'm not what I'm
trying to be.
When examining her story in light of optimal distinctiveness
theory, the inconsistency between Maggie's tattoos and her new
lifestyle should make Maggie feel differentiated. In other words, she
should feel very different from they type of person she wants to be in
her own mind. This strong sense of differentiation should motivate
Maggie to try and achieve a greater sense of assimilation with her new
mature self-concept. Therefore, optimal distinctiveness theory predicts
that Maggie will attempt to change her individual-self to feel more
connected with how she conceptualizes her new self-concept by removing
the tattoos. Indeed, after her tattoos were removed Maggie reported
feeling much less conflicted about her sense of self.
I felt relieved, freedom. I am not worrying, and I've actually shed
that part of my rebellious life. I can live on. I carried this
baggage of my rebellious life around with me and having them [the
tattoos] removed, I'm just relieved.
Amy--A Vow Tattoo Disrupts the Relational-Self
Ever since she was fifteen, Amy wanted to get a tattoo after seeing
a temporary butterfly tattoo that she really liked. Once she reached age
twenty, Amy's parents considered her old enough to do as she
wished, so Amy acquired a butterfly tattoo that she designed herself
over a four month period. On her twenty-first birthday Amy acquired
another tattoo of a dragon. That same year Amy began a dating
relationship which would lead to her third tattoo.
I started dating a man who was actually quite older than I was. I
decided that relationship wasn't going anywhere, but I still really
felt close to him. He was just scaring me to death because he was
talking about marriage after like two months. I was really attached
to him and cared for him a lot, but he freaked me out with the whole
marriage thing, so I decided to break it off. So I got the ankle
tattoo to remember him by. The tattoo is in Chinese and it means
'always in my heart.' It basically meant I would always have a place
in my heart for him.
Obviously Amy's vow tattoo affected her relational-self by
emphasizing her feelings for her ex-boyfriend. Unfortunately, Amy's
tattoo would soon take on a new and unwanted significance.
[After breaking up] I told him we could still communicate and be
friends, but then he showed up at my house uninvited one night when
I had another male visitor over and he just demanded to be let in
and then he started stalking me. I contacted the police about it.
The guy that was over at my house at the time knew someone in the
police department and got his buddy to mention to this guy to leave
me alone or go to jail for harassment. It stopped after that.
Although the stalking incident bothered Amy, it was not enough to
warrant removal of her ankle tattoo. After all, the tattoo was in
Chinese and did not actually feature the name of her ex-boyfriend, so
Amy was able to put the tattoo out of her mind and avoid any disruptive
effects the tattoo might have on her relational-self. Some time later
Amy married another man and had a baby boy. Following the birth of her
child, Amy started to seriously consider having the ankle tattoo
removed.
I'm married now and it just feels funny--having that as a reminder
of a previous relationship. My husband just says, "Well you could
tell people it's for the baby; that the baby will always be in your
heart. " But I'll know why I really got it. You can't lie to
yourself. You know, kids are curious and will ask about anything. I
knew he (her child) would probably ask me about it. He would notice
it and I would tell him and he would ask why did you get it. I
wouldn't want to lie to my child. That's not something I would ever
want to do unless it's for his own good. So I wouldn't want to have
to tell him about this man I dated before your daddy.
With the birth of her child Amy's tattoo is now causing a
problem for her relational-self. Specifically, Amy's view of her
tattoo as a symbol of deception towards her son is straining their
relationship. In the language of optimal distinctiveness theory, the
negative effect of Amy's ankle tattoo is differentiating her
relational-self to an uncomfortable degree. This causes Amy to feel a
need for greater assimilation with her son. Amy feels the only way to
fulfill this need is to have her ankle tattoo removed.
Lana-Trying to be Part of the Young Crowd Backfires
After being married for a few years, Lana decided to get a vow
tattoo that featured a rainbow with her husband's name on the
bottom. Although the tattoo was a declaration of her relationship, it
served more important functions for Lana.
It was something expressive to do and kind of a bonding thing with
my husband at the time. My husband was six years younger than me.
The age difference between us was weird, and he even seems younger
than his real age, so that added more to it. I think that might have
been part of it, just trying to stay younger. I always looked young
because most people thought we were the same age, but I just felt I
had to keep up in some ways to be young and not get in the way older
people think. I felt like even though I looked young I knew I
couldn't change my age, so I was thinking this would be something to
sort of impress them [his friends] because they all had tattoos.
They were always hanging around and coming over. I think I was
trying to be part of his friends, and more of them were doing it
[getting tattoos] at the time. I guess I wanted to fit in with
younger people. It looked like a way to be different or expressive.
I felt like I was going through a sort of midlife crisis at the
time.
In addition to affecting her relational-self, Lana's tattoo
enhanced feelings of assimilation to her husband's group of young
friends for her collective-self. Eventually, however, two things
happened to alter how Lana viewed her tattoo. The first was that
Lana's relationship with her husband ended in divorce. At this
point the tattoo of his name was obviously no longer a defining aspect
of her relational-self, however, that was not the main reason she had
acquired the tattoo. Therefore, Lana did not really consider removing
her tattoo until she noticed a change in the tattooing trend.
At first it was more of a novelty, then as time went on, it just
seemed like everybody had one. So it wasn't a big deal anymore and
it kind of lost that ability to make me feel young. My niece, who's
in college now, got one and my mother didn't really react to that at
all. She just shrugged it off. So I started realizing pretty soon it
[tattooing] was just going to become standard, like the way everyone
had their ears pierced. I think I just got sick of it, and I often
wondered how other people can stand to look at theirs all the time.
I remember thinking this is just old, it's something I felt real
good about then, but it was something at the time that was new and
now it's not. It has lost its interest and nobody else cares about
it anymore either.
After Lana noticed everybody else getting tattooed, her tattoo
became a liability for her collective-self. Originally Lana acquired the
tattoo to feel more assimilated with a younger crowd. Now that everybody
was getting tattoos, Lana's tattoo no longer possessed its
youth-restorative properties by being associated exclusively with a
younger crowd. In terms of optimal distinctiveness theory, this change
in the tattooing trend enormously increased the size of Lana's
"in-group" (i.e. people possessing tattoos), which resulted in
feelings of extreme assimilation for Lana and made her uncomfortable.
I really didn't like the fact that I was conforming to something
that everyone was doing because that's not something I do. I just
wanted to do something different, like I did it more for myself
rather than to declare anything, but it had become more mainstream.
Finally Lana decided to remove her tattoo. Lana's reflection
on her tattoo experience reveals how removing the tattoo allowed her to
negotiate through her identity conflict by replacing her feelings of
assimilation with a new sense of differentiation.
It's weird because I've sort of felt all this is kind of
silly, everything people do to their bbodies, like earrings or nose
rings or whatever. It almost seems like such a waste of time. As I got
into my forties I started to think things we do don't really make
any sense. I had my ears pierced when I was twelve and I'm not sure
why I did that except just to go along with what was trendy or in
fashion. I've started thinking maybe the body should just be pure
and shouldn't have all of these extra things pierced and tattooed.
In some ways it kind of gives me a sick feeling. I was just trying to
return to a time when my body was left alone in its natural state.
DISCUSSION
In addition to providing descriptive reasons for tattoo removal,
this study's findings illustrate that tattoo acquisition and
removal are undertaken as a means of identity negotiation. The
informants initially acquired a tattoo to either enhance feelings of
affiliation (e.g., conform to a group, follow a trend, friend's
influence, relationship vow) or feelings of individuation (e.g., assert
independence, rebellion, personal expression). Tattoos were typically
acquired under the implicit assumption that factors influencing the
tattoo's identity ramifications would remain stable over time. For
many consumers this assumption proved false as lifestyle transitions
such as moving, changing friends, marriage, and divorce all changed the
tattoo's identity ramifications. In some cases these changes
created an identity conflict between the tattooed aspect of the
consumer's identity and the identity associated with a new social
role or group of friends. In other cases these changes made the tattoo
obsolete or meaningless to the consumer. Regardless of which change
occurred, when identity conflict arose, consumers sought tattoo removal
services.
This study's findings provide insight into the link between
the self-concept and disposition. Respondents who felt overly
individuated in a frequently activated social identity group (e.g.,
occupation), compensated for this conflict by enhancing feelings of
affiliation in another frequently activated social identity (e.g., group
of close friends). Even impending new social identities that had yet to
be established (e.g., future occupation) conflicted with a currently
existing social identity (e.g., friends). This finding illustrates that
even repeated cognitive, as opposed to situational, activation is
sufficient to create a new, albeit imagined, social identity. In this
case, the new cognitively-based social identity was powerful enough to
warrant tattoo removal, further illustrating not all social identities
are created equal and some have greater impact on disposition.
Although a rapidly growing literature, the research to date on
disposition and its connection to the self tends to fall in one of three
domains. One domain focuses on voluntary simplicity where disposition is
practiced in an effort to cut the tie to, or separate from, an identity
(Cherrier and Murray 2007; Craig-Lees and Hill 2002; Huneke 2005; Sharma
1985). A second domain involves the involuntary disposition of
possessions due to uncontrollable, external circumstances, such as a
natural disaster (Delorme, Zinkhan, and Hagen 2004). In this case, the
consumer is fighting to maintain his/her self identity and
"struggles to preserve his/her possessions and thus symbolic
self' (Delorme, Zinkhan, and Hagen 2004, p. 188). The third domain
of disposition and the connection to the self consists of the context in
which the consumer uses disposition as a bridge to transfer his/her
identity to others, as in the case of the elderly or AIDS victims
passing on possessions to achieve symbolic immortality (Kates 2001;
Price, Arnould, and Curasi 2000).
The complexity of the identity negotiation found in the tattoo
removal process extends the literature on disposition and the
self-concept. This study's findings show that tattoo removal does
not fit neatly into one of the three domains associated with disposition
and the self (i.e., separate from an identity; transfer an identity to
others; maintain an identity). In fact, this study identified a complex,
continuous identity reconstruction process whereby the consumer is
continually negotiating the need to individuate the self from others and
integrate the self with relational and collective social groups as she
searches for and defines her self-concept over time. The informants who
were undergoing tattoo removal were not simply committing a single,
granular act of cutting ties to an identity, transferring an identity to
others, or struggling to maintain their sense of self. The data show
that the informants were experiencing a more elastic process involving
the balancing and rebalancing of multiple (i.e., individual, relational
and collective), conflicting, co-existing identities that composed the
self-concept.
This study's findings also complement and extend
Schouten's (1991) work on the role of consumption activities in
shaping the maintenance and development of the self-concept. His work
examines the consumption of plastic surgery during times of personal
self-transition. Schouten (1991) argues that plastic surgery is part of
a symbolic consumption process that relates to personal rites of passage
and identity reconstruction. Arguably, tattoo and plastic surgery
services are similar because they are often obtained while the self is
in transition, and they represent relatively permanent surgical changes
to the body with identity ramifications. However, our research
demonstrates that altering the body via tattoo acquisition and removal
is part of an identity negotiation, rather than a reconstruction,
process. Furthermore, that negotiation process involves not just the
reconstruction of a single social identity, but the negotiation of
multiple conflicting needs (i.e., assimilation and differentiation) as
they relate to various social identities that make up an
individual's self-concept.
Plastic surgery seems to be strongly tied to performance in key
roles, lifestyle transitions, and a resulting change in self (Schouten
1991). This study found these factors are also important to acquiring
and removing a tattoo; however, not all role transitions elicit tattoo
consumption or disposition. This study's findings indicate that a
role transition causes a person to acquire or remove a tattoo under two
primary conditions. When a consumer bases part of her self-concept on
the group or category membership that is altered by a role transition
then she may desire to acquire or remove a tattoo. In addition, a
consumer may desire tattoo removal when she views the tattoo as
inconsistent with her new social role. In this condition, the person
must perceive an incompatibility between her new social role and her
tattooed identity, such that the conflict sufficiently disrupts her
sense of self.
This study also revealed factors other than key roles and lifestyle
transitions that were part of the consumer story of tattoo removal. For
example, the informants explained that interpersonal influence, broken
relationships, negative perceptions, identity conflict, and a lack of
(or negative associations with) product meaning, also warranted tattoo
removal. More importantly, this study's findings raise an even
larger question when compared to Schouten's (1991) research.
Plastic surgery is a form of symbolic consumption used to seek a more
stable, harmonious self-concept. This study found that some people also
acquire tattoos to alter their identity, whereas, others acquire tattoos
with less intention and more impulse. Regardless of a consumer's
reasons for obtaining a tattoo, the service often results in unintended,
long-term consequences for the self-concept. This study's findings
suggest that many consumers, like Schouten's (1991) informants, may
initially seek harmony in the self-concept when they purchase a tattoo
service; but over time the tattoo ultimately disrupts, instead of
stabilizes, the consumer's identity. Instead of finding harmony,
this study's informants indicated the tattoo eventually created
further instability in their self-concepts.
One implication of this finding is that achieving a stable
self-concept via consumption may not even be possible. The consumer may
be involved in constant identity negotiation, which is fueled, resolved,
and further fueled by consumption. A person may consume tattoo services
to attain a stable self-concept, but later find the tattoo has
unforeseen identity implications that disrupt her sense of self.
Therefore, the consumer considers further consumption, via tattoo
removal, to stabilize the sense of self. The result is a cyclical process of consumption to maintain the consumer's identity
beginning with tattoo acquisition, which eventually disrupts the
identity; continuing with tattoo removal, to repair the disrupted
identity; and possibly continuing with further tattoo acquisition still
in search of harmony in the self-concept. The authors found evidence of
this continuous cycle in one of the interviews, where the informant
obtained the tattoo, removed the tattoo, and then had the removal scar
covered with yet another tattoo.
This study's findings are also applicable to identity
negotiation via consumption and disposition in other domains of consumer
behavior. For example McAlexander and Schouten's (1989) study of
college students' hairstyles found that a student uses his/her
hairstyle as a method of altering either feelings of individuation
(e.g., independence from their parents, from an ex-boyfriend) or
affiliation (e.g., to fit in with a new group). A person's
hairstyle is a salient characteristic and, particularly in adolescence,
can mark the person's membership in a particular group or
individuate that person from others (i.e., a particularly outrageous
hairstyle). Similar to tattoos, therefore, a hairstyle represents an
effective consumption (and disposition) venue for making any social
identity more salient as a means of offsetting disruptions in the
self-concept. A similar cyclical pattern, for the purposes of identity
negotiation, is also likely for other transition-related consumer
behaviors such as cosmetics and clothing consumption.
Our results suggest several directions for future research on
disposition and identity. One interesting question is the relative
influence each component of the tripartite self has on a consumer's
consumption and disposition decisions. One possible answer is whichever
aspect of the self is most currently salient will exert the most
influence on a consumer's self-concept. The constant identity
negotiation efforts many of our respondents engaged in seem to support
this interpretation. Alternatively, the conceptual overlap of the
relational-self and collective-self suggests that either may chronically
exert more influence over consumption and disposition decisions than the
other. In fact, their relative influence may interact with gender such
that women are more influenced by the relational-self and men are more
influenced by the collective-self. This interpretation is consistent
with how men and women traditionally define themselves (cf., Brewer and
Gardner 1996; Gabriel and Gardner 1999). Future research may test this
prediction in a disposition context.
At a more fundamental level, our results beg the question of the
relative effects consumption and disposition have on stabilizing a
consumer's identity. For instance, future research may find that
disposition behaviors are better able to stabilize a person's
identity because disposition is seen as more authentic compared to the
materialism of consumption (cf., Kozinets 2002). Of course this assumes
it is even possible for consumers to truly stabilize their identity via
consumption and disposition. In light of the present results, there is
certainly reason to doubt this assumption.
REFERENCES
Armstrong, Myrna L., Dorothy J. Stuppy, Donata C. Gabriel, and Rox
R. Anderson. 1996. "Motivation for tattoo removal." Archives
of Dermatology 132: 412-416.
Belk, Russell W. 1988. "Possessions and the Extended
Self." Journal of Consumer Research 14: 139-168.
Brewer, Marilynn B. 1991. "The Social Self. On Being the Same
and Different at the Same Time." Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin 17: 475-482.
Brewer, Marilynn B. and W. Gardner. 1996. "Who Is This
'We'? Levels of Collective Identity and
Self-Representations." Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 71: 83-93.
Cherrier, Helene and Jeff B. Murray. 2007. "Reflexive Dispossession and the Self. Constructing a Processual Theory of
Identity." Consumption, Markets, and Culture 10: 1-29.
Craig-Lees, Margaret and Constance Hill. 2002. "Understanding
Voluntary Simplifiers." Psychology & Marketing 19: 187-210.
Creswell, John W. 1998. Qualitative Inquiry and Research Design:
Choosing Among Five Traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
DeLorme, Denise E., George M. Zinkhan, and Scott C. Hagen. 2004.
"The Process of Consumer Reactions to Possession Threats and Losses
in a Natural Disaster." Marketing Letters 15: 185-199.
DeMello, Margo. 1995. "'Not Just for Bikers
Anymore': Popular Representations of American Tattooing."
Journal of Popular Culture 29: 37-52.
DeMello, Margo. 2000. Bodies of Inscription: A Cultural History of
the Modern Tattoo Community. Durham, NC: Duke University.
Gabriel, Shira and Wendi L. Gardner. 1999. "Are there
'His' and 'Hers' Types of Interdependence? The
Implications of Gender Differences in Collective Versus Relational
Interdependence for Affect, Behavior, and Cognition." Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 77: 642-655.
Gilbert, Steve. 2000. Tattoo History. New York, NY: Powerhouse Books.
Gritton, Joy. 1988. "Labrets and Tattooing in Native
Alaska." In Marks of Civilization. Ed. Arnold Rubin. Los Angeles,
CA: Museum of Cultural History, University of California, 181-190.
Huneke, Mary E. 2005. "The Face of the Un-Consumer: An
Empirical Examination of the Practice of Voluntary Simplicity in the
United States." Psychology & Marketing 22: 527-550.
Kates, Steven M. 2001. "Disposition of Possessions Among
Families of People Living with AIDS." Psychology and Marketing 18:
365-387.
Kozinets, Robert V. 2002. "Can Consumers Escape the Market?
Emancipatory Illuminations from Burning Man." Journal of Consumer
Research 29: 20-38.
Lastovicka, John L. and Karen V. Fernandez. 2005. "Three Paths
to Disposition: The Movement of Meaningful Possessions to
Strangers." Journal of Consumer Research 31: 813-23.
Long, G. E. and L. S. Rickman. 1994. "Infections and
Complications of Tattoos." Clinical Infectious Disease 18: 610-619.
McAlexander, James H. 1991. "Divorce, the Disposition of the
Relationship, and Everything." Advances in Consumer Research 18:
43-48.
McAlexander, James H. and John W. Schouten. 1989. "Hair Style
Changes as Transition Markers." Sociology and Social Research 74:
58-62.
McAlexander, James H., John W. Schouten, and Scott D. Roberts.
1993, "Consumer Behavior and Divorce." Research in Consumer
Behavior 6: 153-184.
McClelland, David. 1951. Personality. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart
& Winston.
Mifflin, Margot. 2001. Bodies of Subversion. New York, NY: Juno
Books.
Miles, Matthew B. and Michael A. Huberman. 1994. Qualitative Data
Analysis: A Source book for New Methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Moustakas, Clark E. 1994. Phenomenological Research Methods.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Prelinger, Ernst. 1959. "Extension and Structure of the
Self." Journal of Psychology 47: 13-23.
Price, Linda L., Eric J. Arnould, and Carolyn Folkman Curasi. 2000.
"Older Consumers Disposition of Special Possessions." Journal
of Consumer Research 27: 179-201.
Rubin, Arnold. 1988. "Tattoo Renaissance." In Marks of
Civilization. Ed. Arnold Rubin. Los Angeles, CA: Museum of Cultural
History, University of California, 233-264.
Sanders, Clinton R. 1985. "Tattoo Consumption: Risk and Regret
in the Purchase of a Socially Marginal Service." In Advances in
Consumer Research 12. Eds. Elizabeth F. Hirschman and Morris B.
Holbrook. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 17-22.
Sanders, Clinton R. 1988. "Marks of Mischief. Becoming and
Being Tattooed." Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 16: 395-431.
Sanders, Clinton R. 1989. Customizing the Body: The Art and Culture
of Tattooing. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University.
Sartre, Jean-Paul. 1943. Being and Nothingness: A Phenomenological
Essay on Ontology. New York, NY: Philosophical Library.
Schiffmacher, Henk and Burkhard Riemschneider. 2001. 1000 Tattoos.
New York, NY: Taschen.
Schouten, John W. 1991. "Selves in Transition: Symbolic
Consumption in Personal Rites of Passage and Identity
Reconstruction." Journal of Consumer Research 17: 412-425.
Sedikides, Constantine and Marilynn Brewer. 2001. Individual Self,
Relational Self, Collective Self. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
Sharma, Avraham. 1985. "The Voluntary Simplicity
Consumer." The Journal of Consumer Marketing 2: 57-63.
Shrauger, J. Sidney and Thomas J. Schoeneman. 1979. "Symbolic
Interactionist View of Self-Concept: Through the Looking Glass
Darkly." Psychological Bulletin 86: 549-573.
Solomon, Michael R. 1983. "The Role of Products as Social
Stimuli: A Symbolic Interaction Perspective." Journal of Consumer
Research 10: 319-329.
Thompson, Craig J., William B. Locander, and Howard R. Pollio.
1989. "Putting Consumer Experience Back into Consumer Research: The
Philosophy and Method of Existential-Phenomenology." Journal of
Consumer Research 16: 133-147.
Varma, Sandeep and Sean W. Lanigan. 1999. "Reasons for
requesting laser removal of unwanted Tattoos." British Journal of
Dermatology 140: 483-485.
Velliquette, Anne M., Jeff B. Murray, and Elizabeth H. Creyer.
1998. "The Tattoo Renaissance: An Ethnographic Account of Symbolic
Consumer Behavior." In Advances in Consumer Research 25. Eds.
Joseph W. Alba and J. Wesley Hutchinson. Provo, UT: Association for
Consumer Research, 461-467.
Watson, Joel. 1998. "'Why Did You Put That There?':
Gender, Materialism and Tattoo Consumption." Advances in Consumer
Research 25: 453-460.
Young, Melissa Martin. 1991. "Disposition of Possessions
during Role Transitions." In Advances in Consumer Research 18. Eds.
Rebecca Holman and Michael Solomon. Provo, UT: Association for Consumer
Research, 33-39.
(1) Derived from the literature. Arguably, these reasons are not
mutually exclusive. For example, it could be argued that impression
management could also drive acquisition.
(2) (D): Reason activating goal of differentiation. (A): Reason
activating goal of assimilation.
Jeremy A. Shelton
Lamar University
Cara Peters
Winthrop University
Jeremy A. Shelton is an Assistant Professor in the Department of
Psychology, Lamar University, Beaumont, TX 77710 (phone: 409-880-7839;
email: sheltonious@yahoo.com; fax: 409-880-1779). Cara Peters is an
Assistant Professor in the Department of Management and Marketing,
Winthrop University, Rock Hill, SC 29733 (phone: 803-323-4280; email:
petersc@winthrop.edu; fax: 803-323-3960). Correspondence may be
addressed to either author.
TABLE 1
Reasons to Acquire or Remove a Tattoo (1)
Reasons for tattoo acquisition (2)
(D) Commemorate life event
(D) Symbol of personal identity
(D) Statement of spirituality
(D) Control over the body
(D) Rebellion
(D) Body aesthetics
(A) Mark group affiliation
(A) Relationship vow
(A) Initiation rite
(A) Conformity
Reasons for tattoo removal
(D) Sever ties with previous life period
(D) Loss of art value or uniqueness
(D) Peer pressure in purchase decision
(D) Impression management
(A) Conformity
(A) Repair body aesthetics
(D) End group affiliation
(D) End of relationship
(A) Social rejection
(A) Family pressure
TABLE 2
Informant Demographics
Pseudonym Age Number of Number have/want removed
tattoos
Lana 43 1 1 Removed
Oscar 27 2 Wants 1 removed
Diane 23 3 Wants 1 removed
Chandra 23 2 Wants 2 removed
Teresa 19 1 Wants 1 removed
Barbara 23 1 Wants 1 removed
Donna 37 1 Wants 1 removed
Bob 41 4 1 Removed
Connie 23 1 1 Removed
Amy 23 3 Wants 1 removed
Carla 24 2 1 Removed
Janet 34 3 Wants 3 removed
Jill 27 1 1 Removed then covered
Kim 21 2 Wants 2 removed
Cindy 30 2 Had 1 covered
Maggie 29 2 2 Removed
Sandra 29 5 Wants 1 removed
Crystal 27 4 Wants 2 removed
George 24 2 Wants 1 removed
Susan 25 5 3 Removed
Robin 27 2 Wants 1 removed
Karen 24 2 Wants 1 removed
TABLE 3
Tattoo Decision Information
Self-
Concept Pseu-
Chance (l) donym Reasons to obtain
Individual Lana Trendy; relationship vow; conform to
group; art value/uniqueness
Individual Oscar Drinking; spontaneous; friends'
influence
Individual Diane Spontaneous; depression
Individual Chandra Friend's influence; addictive; trendy;
beauty enhancement
Individual Teresa Drinking; spontaneous; friend's
influence
Individual Barbara Assert independence
Individual Donna Trendy; friends' influence
Relational Bob Assert independence; art value/
uniqueness
Relational Connie Assert independence; friend's influence
Relational Amy Vow tattoo
Relational/ Carla Spontaneous; boredom
Collective
Relational/ Janet Personal expression, addictive
Collective
Relational/ Jill Spontaneous; conform to group
Collective
Collective Kim Trendy; conform to group; friendship
vow; friends' influence
Collective Cindy Trendy
Collective Maggie Conform to group; rebellion;
boyfriend's influence
Collective Sandra Personal expression
Collective Susan Rebellion; alter identity; conform to
group
Collective George Drinking; conform to group
Collective Crystal Husband's influence; friends'
influence; relationship vow; beauty
enhancement
Collective Robin Friend's influence; rebellion; conform
to group
Collective Karen Trendy; beauty enhancement
Self- Time
Concept conside-
Chance (l) red (2) Reasons for removal
Individual 2 Years Relationship ended; loss of unique-
ness; identity conflict
Individual None Negative reminder; identity conflict;
Individual 30 Minutes Not desired design; negative re-
minder; lacks meaning
Individual 1-2 Years Lack of uniqueness; lacks meaning;
fears negative reactions
Individual None Does not fit with character; fears
negative reactions; negative remind-
er; lacks meaning
Individual 6 Months Mistake; poor quality
Individual 3 Months Mistake; poor quality
Relational 1 Year Show of love for wife
Relational 1 Year Parents' negative reaction; lifestyle
transition
Relational 1 Month Negative reminder; fears negative
reactions; mistake
Relational/ None Parents' negative reaction; lifestyle
Collective transition
Relational/ A few More responsible; show of love for
Collective years kids and friends; fears negative
reactions
Relational/ None Lacks meaning; mother's negative
Collective reaction; change in group
Collective 1 Week Lacks meaning; change in group
Collective None Lifestyle transition; lacks meaning
Collective 2.5 Years More responsible; fears negative
reactions
Collective None Does not fit with image; lacks
meaning
Collective 1-2 Days Change in group; committed to job;
show of love for kids
Collective 5 Minutes Lifestyle transition; change in
group;
fears negative reactions; not me
Collective 1 Week-2 Divorced; fears negative reactions;
Months not desired design; poor quality
Collective None More confident; in new roles; fears
negative reactions
Collective 4 Months Lifestyle transition; does not fit her
image
(1) Level of self-concept at which act of disposition changed feelings
of differentiation and assimilation.
(2) Amount of time the individual contemplated getting their tattoo.