Competition between syllabic and metrical constraints in two Bunun dialects *.
Huang, Hui-Chuan J.
Abstract
This article shows how syllabic and metrical constraints interact
differently in two dialects of Bunun, despite their similarity in that
both dialects exhibit modifications of vowel clusters in response to the
ONSET constraint. In Isbukun Bunun, foot forms are constrained both
syllabically and moraically. Stress shifts from the unmarked penultimate syllable to final position under duress to satisfy both ONSET and the
requirement that heavy syllables must not stand in a prosodically weak
position. In contrast, in Takituduh Bunun, metrical wellformedness is
considered more important, so the preferred disyllabic foot forms are
maintained at the cost of creating onsetless syllables. The analysis is
formalized within Optimality Theory by ranking syllabic and metrical
constraints differently in the two dialects. OT is advantageous in
analyzing the data for two additional reasons: 1) the correlation
between stress assignment in nonsuffixed and suffixed words in Isbukun
can be directly captured by an output constraint, and 2) the metrical
influence on syllabification in Takituduh can be readily handled by
allowing syllabic and metrical constraints to interact in the same
hierarchy. An implication of the Isbukun data for the properties of
surface glides is that pre-peak glides can be moraic or not, depending
on whether they follow a tautosyllabic consonant.
1. Introduction
Bunun is an Austronesian language spoken by about 30,000 people in
central and southern Taiwan. According to the classification in Li
(1988), Bunun can be divided into five dialects: the northern dialects
Takituduh and Takibakha, the central dialects Takbanuad and Takivatan,
and the southern dialect Isbukun. Given that there have been relatively
few studies on the phonology of this language, the objectives of this
article are to describe and analyze the prosodies of Takituduh and
Isbukun based on first-hand data. Specifically, the article will show
how syllabic and metrical considerations interact differently in the two
dialects: in Isbukun, syllabic wellformedness constraints may force a
marked foot form, but in Takituduh, foot form constraints may force an
otherwise illegitimate syllable.
Geographically, Isbukun is spoken in the widest area among the five
dialects, including Nantou, Gaoxiong, Hualian and Taidong Counties; in
contrast, Takituduh is spoken only in the Renai township of Nantou
County (Li 1988). There have been only a few studies on Takituduh (Li
1988, 1992; Jeng 1999), but a number of works on Isbukun Bunun,
including He et al. (1986), Li (1988, 1997), Huang (1997), Lin (1996),
Nojima (1996), Lin et al. (2001), and Zeitoun (2000). (1) Many
phonological descriptions about Isbukun are based on the variety in
Nantou County (e.g., Li 1997; Lin 1996), except He et al. (1986) and Lin
et al. (2001), which are based on speakers from Gaoxiong County. A
noticeable subdialectal difference is that while the Isbukun dialect in
Nantou exhibits final stress (Li 1997: 306), Gaoxiong Isbukun in general
stresses the penultimate syllable (He et al. 1986: 7).
The Isbukun data in the article are based on speakers from Gaoxiong
County. (2) The findings indicate that the stress patterns of Gaoxiong
Isbukun (hereafter, Isbukun) are more intricate than straightforward
unmarked penultimate stress in that the weight of the final syllable
plays a role. All words that end with a heavy syllable have final
stress, except for a few forms of reduplicated shapes and onomatopoetic words, which stress the penultimate despite the final heavy syllables.
Words ending in a light syllable generally have penultimate stress, but
some nonsuffixed forms ending in a light syllable exceptionally carry
final stress, many of which are historically contracted forms. The fact
that Isbukun words avoid unmarked penultimate stress when the final
syllable is heavy suggests that the language constructs a
quantity-sensitive trochaic (left-prominent) foot at the fight edge of
words. The weight of the final syllables in suffixed forms is dependent
upon a number of modifications at the morpheme boundaries, which take
place due to the restriction that syllables must have onset consonants.
The article will show how these syllable-related processes determine the
metrical structure of Isbukun.
Takituduh Bunun exhibits very different interactions between
syllabic and metrical requirements. The placement of stress is
insensitive to syllable quantity, and furthermore, the syllabic
constraint ONSET can be violated under some circumstances. The
interactions between the syllabic and metrical considerations in the two
Bunun dialects are analyzed within the framework of Optimality Theory
(McCarthy and Prince 1993; Prince and Smolensky 1993), which is well
suited to capturing dialectal variations via constraint rankings. It
will be shown that the prosodic differences between Isbukun and
Takituduh arise from ranking syllabic constraints and foot form
constraints differently in the two dialects.
The article is organized as follows. Section 2 presents the data
and an analysis of Isbukun Bunun, showing how the onset requirement in
the dialect affects the placement of stress. Section 3 discusses the
case of Takituduh Bunun. Section 4 summarizes and compares the prosodic
similarities and differences of the two dialects. Section 5 discusses
the theoretical implications of the analysis and concludes the article.
2. Isbukun Bunun
In order to show how syllabic considerations affect the metrical
structure of Isbukun, this section will first present an overview of
Isbukun phonemes and syllables (Section 2.1), show the ways that the
constraint ONSET forces heteromorphemic vowel clusters to undergo
changes (Section 2.2), and illustrate the stress patterns of nonsuffixed
forms (Section 2.3) and those of suffixed forms (Section 2.4). Section
2.5 gives an optimality-theoretic analysis of the stress patterns.
2.1. Isbukun phonemes and syllables
The phonemic inventory of Isbukun Bunun includes fourteen
consonants /p t k ? b d s h [>>] l m n q/and three vowels/i u
a/(He et al. 1986; Li 1997). Surface glides [j w] are derived from
underlying vowels/i u/, as will become evident in the following
discussion. A palatalization rule affects/t s/that appear before a high
front configuration [ij] and gives rise to the alveolo-palatal
allophones [t[??][??]], respectively. (3)
A maximal Isbukun syllable is CGVC or CVGC, both of which come from
underlying CVVC sequences; complex syllable margins are not allowed.
High vowels in an underlying VV sequence undergo gliding to avoid
onsetless syllables, such as /ua au ia ai/ strings being realized as
surface [wa aw ja aj], respectively; the surface manifestation of two
adjacent high vowels /iu ui/, however, varies as to which vowel
constitutes the nucleus. If we recognize the crosslinguistic
generalization that more sonorous vowels tend to occupy the nucleus
position, variation as to either/i/or/u/remaining syllabic is expected
due to their comparable sonority.
Notice that the term "glide" and the symbols j and w here
simply refer to the vocalic elements that on the surface do not
exclusively occupy the nucleus of a syllable and do not exhibit steady
vowel formants phonetically; the terminology does not entail whether
these segments are moraic or not. (4) Data from stress assignment, which
will be discussed in later subsections, suggest that glided vowels in
Isbukun are moraic except when they occur in syllable-initial position.
Patterns of static distribution of segments within syllables can
all be handled on the same proposition: whether the glided vowels are
moraic or not depends on their structural affiliation within a syllable.
In Isbukun, there are no CGVG syllables, despite the fact that they have
the same number of segments as legitimate CGVC/CVGC syllables. The lack
of CGVG syllables can be readily understood if we adopt the following
assumptions: coda consonants are nonmoraic, glided vowels are moraic,
and syllables are maximally bimoraic. Under these assumptions, CGVG
syllables are not allowed because they violate the bimoraic upper limit
of a syllable by having three moras, while CGVC/CVGC syllables obey the
constraint by having only two moras. The lack of CGVG syllables cannot
be attributed to a ban on the linear sequence of a vowel flanked on both
sides by glides, because GVGC syllables are allowed. The existence of
GVGC syllables, in contrast with the absence of CGVG, further suggests
that when there is no preceding consonant to fulfill the onset
requirement, syllable-initial glides are forced into the onset position
and become nonmoraic, thus allowing the syllable to obey the bimoraicity
constraint. The variable structural status of Isbukun prepeak glides,
which depend on the presence of a preceding tautosyllabic consonant,
resembles the case of Spanish discussed in Harris and Kaisse (1999).
While a maximal syllable contains a bimoraic sequence with at most
one consonant on either side, a minimal syllable in Isbukun is CV,
because onsets are obligatory and coda consonants are optional. An
examination of Isbukun content words shows that there are in general no
words containing onsetless syllables.
2.2. The onset requirement in Isbukun Bunun morphophonemics
The need to avoid onsetless syllables in Isbukun not only affects
intramorphemic vowel sequences but also vowel clusters that arise
through morpheme concatenation. When a vowel-final stem takes a
vowel-initial suffix, the vowel sequences undergo gliding or
coalescence, depending on the qualities of the adjacent vowels. If the
vowels are of different height, gliding takes place, similar to the
scenario in an intramorphemic vowel sequence.
(1) Vowel sequences across morpheme boundaries: unlike vowels (The
suffix -un is a patient voice marker, and -av marks nonagent voice in
imperatives. The agent voice marker is ma-, m-, or null in the following
data.)
Agent voice forms Suffixed forms
a. /sadu/ [sadu] /sadu-av/ [sadwav]
b. /silili/ [[??]ilili] /[??]ilili-av/ [[??]ililjav]
c. /tupa/ [tupa] /tupa-un/ [tupawn]
d. /ta[??]a[??]a/ [ta[??]a[??]a] /ta[??]a[??]un/ [ta[??}a[??]awn]
Agent voice forms Gloss
a. /sadu/ [sadu] /' see'
b. /silili/ [[??]ilili] /'imitate'
c. /tupa/ [tupa] /'tell'
d. /ta[??]a[??]a/ [ta[??]a[??]a] /'listen to'
If the vowels are identical, the article argues that the two vowels
coalesce into a bimoraic vowel, for reasons to be explained in the
following subsections:
(2) Vowel sequences across morpheme boundaries: like vowels (The
suffix -a marks agent voice in imperatives.)
Agent voice forms Suffixed forms
a. /matutu/ [matutu] /tutu-un/ [tutun]
b. /ta[??]a[??]a/ [ta[??]a[??]a] /ta[??]a[??]a-a/ [ta[??]a[??]a]
Agent voice forms Gloss
a. /matutu/ [matutu] 'pour out'
b. /ta[??]a[??]a/ [ta[??]a[??]a] 'listen to'
The above data show that underlying vowel sequences undergo glide
formation or vowel coalescence in order to satisfy the requirement that
syllables must have onsets. When the two vowels are of different
heights, the high vowel loses its syllabicity; when the two vowels are
identical, they coalesce into one vowel. Either gliding or coalescence
prevents the occurrence of surface onsetless syllables, which would have
arisen from underlying vowel sequences upon morpheme concatenation. (5)
2.3. Stress assignment in nonsuffixed forms
Nonsuffixed forms in Isbukun exhibit both final and penultimate
stress patterns. Although minimal pairs contrasting final and
penultimate stress can be found, such as [tapa] 'shelf' versus
[tapa] 'blanket', the unmarked position of stress is clearly
the penultimate syllable, because words with penultimate stress greatly
outnumber those that carry final stress. It is therefore proposed that
penultimate stress is the norm and that words with final stress are
marked. The unmarked penultimate stress suggests that the last two
syllables are in general parsed into a trochaic foot at the right edge
of words in Isbukun.
If we regard the distribution of final or penultimate stress as
completely unpredictable in nonsuffixed forms, however, there is a
mysterious gap in the distribution of stress when the relationship
between stress assignment and syllable quantity is taken into
consideration. Assuming that CVV(C) syllables are heavy and CV(C)
syllables are light, there is an asymmetry that may shed light on the
nature of stress assignment in the language: among words that carry
final stress, the final syllable may be heavy or light, but for words
with penultimate stress, the final syllable is almost always light,
except for words with reduplicated shapes, such as /muliahliah/
[muljahljah] 'tear apart' and/sinaunau/ [sinawnaw] 'make
a face', and onomatopoeic words such as /tiptiah/ [tsiptsjah]
'thunder, explode' (based on the data collected so far). The
data (3) below illustrate the tendency for nonsuffixed forms with final
CVV(C) syllables not to exhibit unmarked penultimate stress patterns;
instead, they overwhelmingly show final stress.
(3) Stress assignment in nonsuffixed forms with final CVV(C) syllables
a. [kavja[??]] 'friend'
b. [mahdjal] 'slippery'
c. [mapit[??]ja] 'cook'
d. [mahan[??]jap] 'understand'
e. [tanja] 'ear'
f. [mindja] 'pick up'
g. [tahaj] '(male name)'
h. [savaj] 'win'
i. [mat[??]i[??]baj] 'escape'
j. [??]alwa[??]] 'rat'
k. [salinhanwa[??]] 'dragonfly'
l. [mindudwa[??]] 'young'
m. [tanakaw] '(male name)'
n. [minbulaw] 'blister'
o. [takjus] 'slender'
p. [ha[??]umuj] 'testicles'
The sample here excludes nonsuffixed forms with final CVV(C)
syllables such as [ma[??]jal] 'good', [makwan]]
'bad', [makwi[??]] 'narrow/slender', [madajn]
'big', [minpja] 'divide into how many', and [kasuj]
'make money', in which there is a clear prefix (ma-, min-,
ka-) immediately preceding the final CVV(C) syllables. These words are
excluded because the final stress can be attributed to the inability of
the preceding affix to carry stress, rather than to any quantity effect.
The paucity of penultimately stressed words with final heavy
syllables suggests that Isbukun obeys the Weight-to-Stress principle
(WSP) (Prince 1983, 1990; Prince and Smolensky 1993), which dictates
that heavy syllables cannot stand in prosodically weak positions. This
quantity-sensitive stress system builds a monosyllabic foot on the final
bimoraic syllable or a disyllabic left-headed foot when the final
syllable is not heavy. The variable penultimate or final stress patterns
in Isbukun nonsuffixed words are therefore phonologically predictable
for the great majority of the vocabulary, as long as we recognize WSP
and adopt the assumptions that CVV(C) is heavy and CV(C) is light.
2.4. Stress assignment in suffixed forms: consonant-final versus
vowel-final stems
The effect of syllable quantity on stress placement is clearly
manifested in suffixed forms. Before we proceed to the presentation of
stress patterns of suffixed forms, a brief introduction to Bunun
suffixes is needed.
Isbukun suffixes can be classified into two types based on whether
they induce stress shift upon suffixation, a common scenario for
languages across the world. A STRESS-SHIFTING SUFFIX will cause the
stress to move rightward; a NON-STRESS-SHIFTING suffix, in contrast, has
no effect on the stress position of the stem. Stress-shifting suffixes
are illustrated by -a '(imperative agent voice marker)', -av
'(imperative nonagent voice [NAV] marker)', -un '(patient
voice marker)', and -an '(locative voice marker)'.
Non-stress-shifting suffixes include -in '(perfect marker)',
-an '(durative aspect marker)', and nominatives -a/-tia, and
-an/-tan. The bound forms (clitics) of personal pronouns in the language
behave like non-stressshifting suffixes; they include -ik '(1st
person singular agent voice)', -im '(lst person plural exclusive agent voice)', -as '(2nd person singular agent
voice)', and -am '(2nd person plural agent voice)'. The
oblique case marker mas can optionally integrate with the preceding word
through contraction, especially in connected speech, e.g., /saiv-an mas/
[sajvanmas] 'give-NAV Obl.' becoming [sajvas] (Li 1997: 338),
and it has no effect on the stress location of the preceding word no
matter whether contraction occurs or not.
The following data illustrate the different behaviors of the two
types of suffixes:
(4) Stress-shifting suffixes:
Agent voice forms Suffixed forms
a. /masabah/ [masabah] /masabah-a/ [masabaha]
b. /tisdadan/ [t[??]i[??]dadan] /tisdadan-a/ [t[??]i[??]dadana]
c. /haiap/ [hajap] /haiap-un/ [hajapun]
d. /ma[??]uman/ [ma[??]uman] /[??]uman-av/ [[??]umanav]
e. /masaiv/ [masajv] /saiv-an/ [sajvan]
Agent voice forms Gloss
a. /masabah/ [masabah] 'sleep'
b. /tisdadan/ [t[??]i[??]dadan] 'run'
c. /haiap/ [hajap] 'know'
d. /ma[??]uman/ [ma[??]uman] 'remove'
e. /masaiv/ [masajv] 'give'
(5) Non-stress-shifting suffixes:
Agent voice forms Suffixed forms
a. /makuan/ [makwan] /makuan-in/ [makwanin]
b. /minkailas/ [minkajlas] /minkailas-in/ [minkajlaSin]
c. /supah/ [supah] /supah-in/ [supahin]
d. /mapata[??]/ [mapata[??]] /mapata[??]-in/ [mapata[??]in]
Agent voice forms Gloss
a. /makuan/ [makwan] 'bad'
b. /minkailas/ [minkajlas] 'getup'
c. /supah/ [supah] 'many'
d. /mapata[??]/ [mapata[??]] 'kill'
The data in (4) show that no matter whether stress falls on the
penultimate or final syllable, stress moves to the unmarked penultimate
position when taking a stress-shifting suffix. In contrast, a
non-stress-shifting suffix in (5) does not change the stress location of
its stem, no matter where the stress falls in the stem. Therefore,
stress may fall on the antepenult of a suffix form, such as (5b), (5c),
(5d), constituting an apparent counterexample to the generalization of a
penultimate stress pattern. Since stress assignment is determined by its
location in the stem in the case of a nonstress-shifting suffix (or a
function word such as mas), rather than by phonological environments,
the following discussion will focus on the data on stress-shifting
suffixes.
All the examples given in data (4) end with a consonant, and they
show uniformly that stress falls on the penultimate syllable of the
suffixed form, the unmarked position for stress. Complexity arises when
considering stems that end with vowels. All the stress-shifting suffixes
here are vowel-initial, and they would potentially create illegitimate
onsetless syllables when attaching to a vowel-ending stem. As mentioned
in Section 2.2, Isbukun does not allow onsetless syllables on the
surface; vowel hiatus is avoided by gliding or coalescence. As a result,
when one of these stress-shifting suffixes follows a vowelending stem,
gliding or coalescence takes place and stress falls on the final
syllable of the suffixed form, in contrast to the penultimate stress
pattern for consonant-ending stems as shown in (4). In other words,
consonant-ending and vowel-ending stems exhibit different stress
patterns in order to conform to the syllabification requirement of the
language.
The following data (6)-(9) illustrate how consonant-ending versus
vowel-ending stems exhibit different stress patterns upon suffixation
due to pressure to satisfy the onset requirement. Examples of
consonantending stems are given in (6) (as well as in [4] above) and (7)
below: although there is a contrast between penultimate and final stress
in nonsuffixed forms, stress neutralizes to the penultimate position of
the suffixed form. As to vowel-ending stems, the suffixed form uniformly
shows final stress, no matter whether the stem carries penultimate
stress, as in (8), or final stress, as in (9).(6)
(6) Consonant-ending stems with penultimate stress
a. /maludah/ [maludah] /ludah-av/ [ludahav] 'hit'
b. /mindana[??]/ [mindana[??]] /[??]ind- [?]?inda-
ana[??]-av/ na[??]av] 'help'
c. /kalat/ [kalat] /kalat-un/ [kalatun] 'bite'
(7) Consonant-ending stems with final stress
a. /minbas/ [minbas] /[??]inbas-av/ [??]inbasav] 'revenge'
b. /minsa- [minsa- /pinsan- [pinsan 'become
nama[??]/ nama[??]] ama[??]-un/ ama[??]un] (nothing)'
c. /kusbai/ [kusbaj] /kusbai-a/ [kusbaja] 'fly'
(8) Vowel-ending stems with penultimate stress
a. /tupa/ [tupa] /tupa-un/ [tupawn] 'tell'
b. /tahu/ [tahu] /tahu-an/ [tahwan] 'talk'
c. /sadu/ [sadu] /sadu-an/ [sadwan] 'see'
d. /min[??]uni/ [min[??]uni] /min[??]uni-a/ [min[??]- 'become'
unja]
(9) Vowel-ending stems with final stress
a. /piskaubu/ [pi[??]kawbu] /piskaubu- [pi[??]kawbwav] 'cheat'
av/
The data above show that when consonant-ending stems are suffixed,
the suffixed forms exhibit the unmarked penultimate stress pattern. The
final stress pattern in the suffixed forms of vowel-ending stems is
reminiscent of the fact that in nonsuffixed vocabulary, final heavy
syllables attract stress due to WSP. Based on the evidence from stress,
therefore, the article argues that a glided vowel nonetheless
contributes to the weight of the syllable where it occurs. That is to
say, words such as [tu.(pawn)](7) carry final stress because the final
syllable is a bimoraic heavy syllable; stress does not fall on the
penult *[(tu.pawn)] in that the heavy syllable cannot stand in the
nonhead position of the trochaic foot. In this way, by upholding the
principle of WSP in Isbukun, we can connect the phenomenon of suffixed
forms of vowel-final stems exhibiting marked final stress with the lack
of nonsuffixed forms with both penultimate stress and final heavy
syllables.
Notice that the term "consonant-ending" here refers to
stems that end with a nonsyllabic element on the surface, including
glides, which are actually underlying vowels in Bunun. A surface
stem-final glide becomes the onset of the following syllable upon
suffixation, and the syllable-initial glide in the suffixed form
patterns with true consonants in inducing penultimate stress, as shown
in (7). The data from stress assignment, therefore, provide additional
evidence that syllable-initial onset glides are nonmoraic, as mentioned
in Section 2.1.
From a derivational point of view, stress falls on the final
syllable of a suffixed form in vowel-ending stems because
gliding/coalescence takes place after stress is assigned to the
second-to-last vowel. A rule-based analysis of the patterns is given
below.
(10) A rule-based analysis
a. Rules:
Intervocalic gliding: [V, +high] [right arrow] [-syllabic] / V_V
Stress assignment: Stress the penultimate syllable (except in
the case of lexical exceptions)
Gliding: [V, +high, a place] [right arrow]
{_[V, a place
[-syllabic] / [V, -a place]_}
Coalescence: Adjacent identical vowels coalesce.
b. Derivations:
/tupa-un/ /tutu-un/ /kusbai/ /kusbai-a/ /haiap/
Syllabi- tu.pa.un tu.tu.un kus.ba.i kus.ba.i.a ha.i.ap
fication
Inter- -- -- -- kus.ba.ja (8) ha.jap
vocalic
gliding
Stress tu.pa.un tu.tu.un kus.ba.i kus.ba.ja ha.jap
Gliding/ tu.pawn tu.tun kus.baj -- --
coa- [tupawnl [tutun] [kusbaj] [kusbaja] [hajap]
lescence
The intermediate forms/tu.pa.un/,/tu.tu.un/, and/kus.ba.i/show that
the penultimate stress generalization holds true at a level before
Gliding/ Coalescence takes place. To put it another way, the
gliding/coalescence rule that applies later obscures the penultimate
stress pattern and gives rise to final stress. The derivation of/kusbai-a/[kusbaja] illustrates that the Intervocalic Gliding rule
must apply before the stress rule; otherwise, stress would be
incorrectly placed on the second-to-last input vowel */kusbaia/if no
additional adjustments are made.
Although the rule-ordering analysis is straightforward, the article
will adopt a constraint-based analysis of the data in the following
section and argue that such an approach provides more insight into the
interaction between stress assignment and syllabification in the
language.
2.5. An Optimality-theoretic analysis of the Isbukun data
The stress patterns in Isbukun nonsuffixed forms are correlated
with the ways stress assignment interacts with syllabification in
suffixed forms, and such a correlation can be better captured by a
constraint-based approach such as Optimality Theory rather than by rules
because the framework of OT allows the correlation to be directly
embodied by an output markedness constraint that prohibits heavy
syllables in prosodically weak positions. Specifically, the constraint
WSP is responsible for the paucity of nonsuffixed forms with penultimate
stress and final heavy syllables as well as for the final stress pattern
in suffixed forms of vowelending stems.
The predictable pattern of penultimate or final stress is captured
by ranking FtBIN[sigma], which dictates that a foot must be disyllabic,
below WSP and a foot type constraint FTTYPE(trochaic), (9) which prefers
leftheaded feet. When no final heavy syllable is present, WSP is not
relevant, so that FTTYPE(trochaic) and [FTBIN.sub.[??]] lead to
penultimate stress, as seen in the unmarked stress patterns of
nonsuffixed forms and in suffixed forms of consonant-ending stems. When
there are final heavy syllables, it is impossible to satisfy WSP,
FTTYPE(trochaic), and [FTBIN.sub.[??]] at the same time. The dilemma is
resolved by resorting to violation of the lower ranked [FTBIN.sub.[??]]
constraint, creating a final stress pattern on the surface.
Notice that [FTBIN.sub.[??]] is actually one version of the
FooTBINARITY (FTBIN) constraint, which refers to either syllables or
moras in the literature. The article recognizes two separate FTBIN
constraints, one referring to syllables ([FTBIN.sub.[??]]) and the other
to moras ([FTBIN.sub.u]), because they are better treated as separate
constraints and ranked in different places in Isbukun. (10)
[FTBIN.sub.u] is ranked below WSP and FTTYPE(trochaic), so a
monosyllabic foot form is forced to appear in the presence of a final
heavy syllable. The constraint [FTBIN.sub.u], however, is relatively
highranked. Except for lexically marked final-stressed words such as
[piskawbu], [FTBIN.sub.u] is not only obeyed in words with penultimate
stress but also in words which exhibit final stress. Moreover, the
bimoraic requirement of a foot is supported by the observation that
monosyllabic words are long although they are short if affixed, e.g.,
/dan/ [da:n] 'road' versus/ka-dan/[kadan] 'to build
roads' (Lin 1996:42-44). (11) The lengthening of vowels can be
attributed to the bimoraic minimum of a content word, which is derived
from the Prosodic Hierarchy (Selkirk 1980) when taken together with Foot
Binarity (Prince 1980; McCarthy and Prince 1993). That is, a content
word, being a prosodic word, must consist of at least one foot, which in
turn consists of two moras. Vowels in Isbukun monosyllabic roots
lengthen in order to form a bimoraic foot and fulfill the minimal word
requirement. Since there is no positive evidence showing that
[FTBIN.sub.u] is violated on the surface except in the case of lexically
marked stress, (12) the article assumes that the constraint is not
crucially ranked below WSP and FTTYPE(trochaic) as FTBIN[sigma] is, in
order to highlight the bimoraic property of a foot in the language.
Consequently, the constraints [FTBIN.sub.[??]] and [FTBIN.sub.u] are
ranked in different places.
The constraints most relevant to the set of data here are
summarized below, including the three constraints WSP, [FTBIN.sub.[??]],
and FTTYPE(trochaic) already mentioned above:
(11) a. WSP: Heavy syllables are stressed.
b. [FTBIN.sub.u]: A foot must be disyllabic.
c. FTTYPE(trochaic): Feet have initial prominence. (13)
d. ALL-FT-RIGHT: Every foot stands at the right edge of the
Prosodic Word.
e. PARSE-SYL: Syllables are parsed by feet.
f. ONSET: Syllables must have onsets.
g. V-NUCLEUS (V-Nuc): Every [-consonantal] segment must be
linked to the nucleus (Rubach 2000: 274) without sharing it
with other elements.
h. UNIFORMITY-IO: No element of the output has multiple
correspondents in the input (McCarthy and Prince 1995a).
i. Max-IO-V: Input vowels must have a correspondent in the
output (McCarthy and Prince 1995a).
j. MAx-IO-u: The mora associated with an input segment must
be preserved in its output correspondent.
k. OCP-PLACE: glide-vowel or vowel-glide sequences of identical
place features, such as [wu uw ji ij], are disallowed.
The constraint ALL-FT-RIGHT crucially ranks above a number of
constraints and is presumably undominated in the language. The ranking
of ALL-FT-RIGHT above PARSE-SYL accounts for the fact that stress is
noniterative (Kager 1999) in Bunun. Only a single foot is formed at the
right edge because any other foot structure would incur additional
violations of the higher-ranked ALL-FT-RIGHT. The constraint
ALL-FT-RIGHT must also rank above WSP in order to account for the
possible occurrences of unstressed heavy syllables in pretonic
positions, such as /piskaubu-av/ [pis.kaw.(bwav)].(14) If ALL-FT-RIGHT
is dominated by WSP, [pis.kaw .(bwav)] would incorrectly lose to
*[pi[??].(kaw).(bwav)]. Given the established rankings ALL-FT-RIGHT
>> WSP and WSP >> [FTBIN.sub.u], by transitivity,
ALL-FT-RIGHT also ranks above FTBIN[sigma], which accounts for the fact
that forming a monosyllabic foot at the right edge is more optimal than
forming a disyllabic nonfinal foot, e.g. [min.[??].(nja)] rather than
*[(min.[??]u).nja]. The two constraints ALL-FT-RIGHT and PARSE-SYL will
not be included in the following discussion in order to simplify the
presentation.
The constraint ONSET motivates gliding or coalescence in vowel
clusters. The suffixed forms of vowel-final stems exhibit a final stress
pattern, because ONSET forces a final heavy syllable; the suffixed forms
of consonant-final stems carry penultimate stress, because ONSET can be
satisfied without creating a final heavy syllable. In Isbukun, the
syllabic constraint ONSET must crucially rank above the foot form
constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]]; otherwise the correct form [sa.(dwav)]
would lose to *[sa.(du.av)]. The rankings of both ONSET and WSP above
[FTBIN.sub.[??]] lead to the different stress patterns in the suffixed
forms of vowel-final versus consonant-final stems.
The article adopts the ranking ONSET >> V-Nuc to represent
the changes of /i u/to [j w], which needs further discussion because of
the complexities associated with the differences between
syllable-initial and noninitial glides. The constraint V-Nuc as
formulated in Rubach (2000) penalizes glides in the onset or in the coda
since they are [-consonantal] elements that are not linked to the
syllable nucleus. Strictly speaking, although syllable-initial glides in
Isbukun violate V-NUC, nonsyllable-initial glides satisfy V-NUC because
they may share, and thus link to, the Nucleus node with another vowel.
Consequently, the ranking ONSET >> V-NUC is not sufficient to
express the changes of/i u/ to [j w] in the data. One way to solve the
problem is to assume that noninitial glided vowels in Isbukun are
motivated by the ranking of ONSET above NoDIPH (No DIPHTHONGS,
Rosenthall 1997a: 142) while syllable-initial glides are motivated by
ONSET >> V-Nuc. Since it is argued in the article that postvocalic
vowel-glide or glide-vowel sequences are in fact bimoraic, we may term
such sequences diphthongs and treat them formally as diphthong formation
rather than glide formation. Another solution is to modify the
definition of V-Nuc so that the same constraint is violated by both
syllable-initial and noninitial glides: every [-consonantal] segment
must be linked to the nucleus without sharing it with other elements. In
the tableaux given below, the constraint V-Nuc, with its revised
definition, is adopted. (15)
The three constraints V-Nuc, UNIFORMITY-IO, and Max-IO-V are
included to illustrate the various possible strategies used to repair an
ONSET violation. V-Nuc proscribes the gliding of vowels, UNIFORMITYIO
prevents coalescence, and Max-IO-V prohibits vowel deletion. While ONSET
>> V-Nuc leads to gliding, the ranking ONSET >> UNIFORMITYIO
accounts for vowel coalescence. The constraint V-Nuc should rank below
UNIFORMITY-IO, because gliding affects vowel sequences in a wider
variety of contexts. Max-IO-V is ranked higher than both V-Nuc and
UNIFORMITY-IO, SO that vowel deletion is not observed here.
The fact that glided and coalesced vowels retain their underlying
moras is captured by the high ranking of MAx-IO-u. MAX-IO-u ensures that
a final heavy syllable is created in the suffixed forms of vowel-ending
stems no matter whether gliding or coalescence occurs. The advantages of
adopting a high ranking MAX-IO-u, rather than IDENT-IO-u, will be
discussed shortly along with the illustrating tableaux.
The output constraint OCP-PLACE disallows vowel-glide or
glide-vowel sequences that are identical in terms of their place
features. In this article, OCP-PLACE functions to block gliding in the
case of identical vowel sequences, leading to coalescence on the
surface.
The crucial rankings among these constraints are summarized below:
(12)
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
The tableau in (13) illustrates how constraint interactions account
for the unmarked penultimate stress pattern in the suffixed forms of
consonantending stems. A consonant-ending stem allows the formation of a
final light syllable in the suffixed forms, so that [FTBIN.sub.??] can
be satisfied without violating other high-ranked constraints, resulting
in penultimate stress.
Candidate (13a) shows that when the stem ends with a consonant, it
is possible to satisfy the constraints ONSET, WSP, FTTYPE (trochaic),
and FTBIN?? at the same time. The stem-final consonant is assigned to
the onset of the following syllable, and the formation of a disyllabic
trochaic foot at the fight edge does not lead to a violation of WSP.
Placing stress in the final syllable violates [FTBIN.sub.??] if a
monosyllabic foot is formed, as illustrated by candidate (13b), and the
possibility of final stress is ruled out by FTTYPE (trochaic) if
disyllabicity is satisfied at the cost of forming an iambic foot, as
illustrated by (13c). Whether a stem carries penultimate or final stress
does not make a difference in the stress placement of suffixed forms;
therefore, only one constraint table is given to illustrate
consonant-ending stems.
As to vowel-ending stems, the vowel hiatus created through
suffixation would lead to violation of the high-ranked ONSET constraint,
which must be avoided through the violation of some lower ranked
constraint. The tableau in (14) illustrates how gliding occurs in
response to the syllabic constraint ONSET, and how stress placement is
affected accordingly.
Candidates (a) through (e) show that underlying vowels become
glides to satisfy ONSET, at the cost of violating the lower ranked
V-NUC. Candidates (a), (b), and (c) all place stress on the desired
final syllable, but only (a) is the optimal output because (b) forms an
iambic foot, violating FTTYPE (trochaic), and (c) is ruled out by
MAx-IO-u due to the loss of an input mora. Candidates (d) and (e) place
stress on the penultimate syllable. Candidate (d) violates WSP because
the final bimoraic syllable is in the weak position of the foot.
Candidate (e) avoids a WSP violation by turning the underlying vowel
into a nonmoraic glide in the output, but this move again violates the
equally high ranked constraint MAx-IO-u. Candidate (f) shows that given
the need to obey ONSET, forming a disyllabic foot on the last two vowels
of the suffixed form is impossible in the case of vowel-ending stems. A
comparison of (a) and (f) suggests that the need to satisfy the onset
requirement is more important than maintaining the unmarked disyllabic
foot form. Candidates (g) and (h) illustrate the cases in which an ONSET
violation is repaired through coalescence. The fact that candidate (g)
loses to (a) shows that V-Nut is ranked lower than UNIFORMITY-IO.
Candidate (h) shows that penultimate stress is impossible even if
coalescence takes place, because MAX-IO-u demands that coalesced vowels
preserve their input quantity. Candidate (i) satisfies the ONSET
constraint by deleting one of the vowels in hiatus; although this form
avoids violation of MAX-IO-u by saving the mora of the deleted vowel, it
is ruled out by the constraint MAX-IO-V, which prevents deletion of
underlying segments.
Tableau (15) shows that when the stem-final vowel and the
suffix-initial vowel are identical, the ONSET constraint is satisfied
through coalescence rather than gliding. The placement of stress in the
final syllable is accounted for by assuming that coalescence, just as
gliding, preserves the moras of the input vowels.
Candidates (a) through (i) in tableau (15) correspond to those in
tableau (14) in terms of stress placement and the overall makeup of
syllable shapes; the only difference is that the two vowels at the
morpheme boundary in tableau (15) are identical whereas they are
different in tableau (14). In tableau (15), candidate (g), which resorts
to coalescence, wins over candidate (a), which employs glide formation,
because given the sequence of two identical high vowels, gliding would
create a configuration that is blocked by the high ranked OCP-PLACE.
If the two identical vowels are low vowels, gliding is not a
possible strategy to repair hiatus, because low vowels lack
corresponding glides. Therefore, coalescence is observed.
The fact that IDENT-IO-u cannot substitute MAX-IO-u to account for
the moraic status of glided and coalesced vowels is illustrated in
tableaux (15) and (16). For example, the mora-deleting candidate (16c)
*[([tu.sub.u]*pa (1,2) uV)] satisfies IDENT-IO-u because either the
preceding or the following vowel corresponds to a vowel associated with
one mora in the output, and the optimal (16b)
[tu.sub.u]*([pa.sup.(1,2).sub.uu]V)] would incorrectly lose to (16c) by
violating the IDENT-IO-u constraint because each of the coalescing
vowels is associated with two moras. The constraint MAx-IO-u. better
captures the generalization in the data that input moras must survive in
the output despite the type of segmental modification that occurs.
One might question the validity of the assumption that the correct
output representation for the final syllable is a bimoraic vowel, since
the evidence for assuming such a representation has so far come from
stress placement, which is also what is being accounted for here.
Another piece of evidence that independently supports the bimoraic
analysis comes from dialectal comparison. We have mentioned in Section
2.3 that a few lexical items in Isbukun Bunun exceptionally place stress
on final syllables. Some of these final stressed vowels in fact
correspond to two vowels flanking an intervening glottal in Takituduh;
for example [tapa] 'blanket' and [[??]uva[??]]
'child' correspond to [tapaha] and [[??]uva[??]a[??]],
respectively, in Takituduh. It is possible that diachronically, the loss
of glottals [h] and [??] in Isbukun (see Li 1988) is followed by the
merger of adjacent vowels, and that final stress results because the
adjacent two vowels both contribute a mora to the final syllable. The
advantage of assuming a bimoraic analysis is that it not only renders
transparent the final stress pattern in the suffixed forms of
vowel-ending stems, but also accounts for the exceptional final stress
in many nonsuffixed forms from a diachronic point of view. If we assume
outright deletion of one of the identical vowels together with its mora,
the placement of stress in [tutun] and [tupav] would pose a type of
metrical opacity, which is an unnecessarily complicated analysis for the
Bunun data. (16)
To summarize, the unmarked penultimate stress in Isbukun is
captured through the constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]]. In the suffixed forms
of vowel-ending stems, the ranking of [FTBIN.sub.[??]] below ONSET and
other relevant constraints forces the creation of final stress. The
marked final stress pattern is not observed in the suffixed forms of
consonant-ending stems, because in these cases [FTBIN.sub.[??]] can be
satisfied without violating other higher-ranked constraints. In Isbukun
Bunun, the syllabic constraint ONSET is crucially ranked above the
constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]], a metrical constraint demanding unmarked
foot forms. The syllabic influence on metrical structure in Isbukun
contrasts with the case of Takituduh Bunun, in which metrical influence
on syllabification is observed.
3. Takituduh Bunun
In contrast to Isbukun Bunun, in which ONSET and WSP rank above
[FTBIN.sub.[??]], this section will show that in the dialect of
Takituduh, the rankings between the two sets of constraints are
reversed. That is to say, the [FTBIN.sub.[??]] constraint that demands
unmarked disyllabic foot forms is more important than the syllabic
constraint ONSET, and [FTBIN.sub.[??]] also ranks above WSP. Section 3.1
presents a brief introduction of Takituduh phonemes, syllables and
stress. Section 3.2 will show how the vowel clusters in Takituduh are
modified in response to the ONSET constraint. Section 3.3 argues that
ONSET ranks below [FTBIN.sub.[??]], based on evidence from minimal
words. The OT analysis of the Takituduh data is given in Section 3.4.
3.1. Takituduh phonemes, syllables and stress
The phonemic inventory of Takituduh Bunun includes sixteen
consonants /p t k q [??] b d ts s h v [??] l m n/ and three vowels/i u
a/(Li 1988). Compared with Isbukun, Takituduh has two additional
consonant phonemes: the voiceless uvular stop/q/ and the voiceless
alveolar affricate/ts/. Surface glides are derived from the underlying
vowels /i u/ as in Isbukun. The palatalization rule affects/ts s/that
appear before a high front articulation [i j] and gives rise to the
allophones [t[??] [??]], respectively.
Similar to Isbukun, a maximal syllable in Takituduh is CGVC or
CVGC. Usually syllables are minimally composed of CV, but onsetless
syllables may appear on the surface under some specific circumstances,
as discussed in the following subsections.
In Takituduh Bunun, stress in general falls on the final syllable.
Stress moves to the final syllables of affixed forms when
stress-shifting suffixes are attached. Syllable quantity in Takituduh
does not play a role in predicting stress patterns as it does in
Isbukun.
3.2. The onset constraint in Takituduh Bunun
Recall that in the case of Isbukun, the onset constraint is
strictly obeyed: vowel sequences are resolved by either gliding or
coalescence, depending on whether the adjacent vowels are alike. In
Takituduh, however, although gliding is also a strategy used to avoid
onsetless syllables, vowel hiatus can be observed on the surface in some
cases.
Similar to Isbukun, gliding takes place in Takituduh as a result of
the relatively high-ranked ONSET constraint, as manifested in the
formation of glides in tautomorphemic and heteromorphemic vowel
sequences.
(17) Glide formation in tautomorphemic vowel sequences
a. /madain/ [aj] 'big'
b. /ma[??]aiv/ [aj] 'give'
c. /kuntsais/ [aj] 'change'
d. /mindia[??]/ [ja] 'pick up'
e. /madia[??]/ [ja] 'many'
f. /laksial/ [ja] 'slippery'
g. /tusauts/[aw] 'sing'
h. /mahau[??]/[aw] 'angry'
i. /maldadauk/ [aw] 'still'
j. /matsua[??]/ [wa] 'cultivate'
k. /silaluan/ [wa] 'cheat'
The data below suggest that in the case of heteromorphemic
nonidentical two-vowel sequences, the second vowel turns into a glide if
it is a high vowel.
(18) Glide formation in heteromorphemic vowel sequences
a. [tan[??]a] /tan[??]a-i/ (17) [aj] 'listen' (imp. patient
voice)
b. [[??]asa] /[??]asa-i/ [aj] 'like' (imp. patient
voice)
c. [tan[??]a] /tan[??]a-un/ [aw] 'listen' (patient voice)
d. [[??]asa] /[??]asa-un/ [aw] 'like' (patient voice)
e. [[??]ilulu] /silulu-i/ [uj] 'pull' (imp. patient
voice)
f. [pislulu] /pislulu-i/ [uj] 'cause to know' (imp.
patient voice)
g. [pin[??]uni] /pin[??]uni-un/ [iw] 'cause to become'
(patient voice)
If the second vowel in a heteromorphemic nonidentical two-vowel
sequence is nonhigh, glide formation is not observed, resulting in vowel
hiatus on the surface. Huang (2002) attributes the nonoccurrence of
gliding in the first member of the sequence to the ranking of ONSET
below an output-to-output constraint, which demands that the stressed
vowel in the base must remain syllabic in affixed forms
(IDENT-BA-[??]).(18)
(19) Vowel hiatus on the surface: unlike vowels across morpheme
boundaries
a. [[??]i[??]ili] /sisili-a/ [ia] 'imitate' (imp. agent voice)
b. [kitlalavi] /kitlalavi-a/ [ia] 'follow' (imp. agent voice)
c. [[??]ilulu] /silulu-a/ [ua] 'pull' (imp. agent voice)
d. [matsalpu] /kajtsalpu-an/ [ua] 'sad' (locative voice)
If the two vowels across morpheme boundaries are identical,
coalescence does not take place as in the case of Isbukun Bunun.
Instead, vowel hiatus is observed:
(20) Vowel hiatus on the surface: like vowels across morpheme
boundaries
a. [kitlalavi] kitlalavi-i [ii] 'follow' (imp. patient voice)
b. [tan[??]a] tan[??]a-a [aa] 'listen' (imp. agent voice)
The lack of coalescence in (20) implies that the UNIFORMITY-IO
constraint ranks higher than ONSET in Takituduh.
Regardless of the appearance of vowel hiatus in Takituduh, the two
dialects are similar in that they both resort to gliding as a way of
avoiding onsetless syllables, suggesting the ranking of ONSET above
V-NUC in both dialects. The two dialects are different in the ways ONSET
is ranked with respect to other constraints. In Takituduh, ONSET is
dominated by the output-to-output constraint IDENT-BA-[??] and by
UNIFORMITY-IO, leading to vowel hiatus on the surface. In Isbukun,
however, onsetless syllables are disallowed because the syllabic
constraint ONSET must be strictly obeyed.
3.3. Minimal word requirement and the violation of the ONSET
constraint
The patterns of resolving vowel clusters in Takituduh suggest that
ONSET is dominated by some constraints, and the fact that Takituduh
stress is quantity-insensitive suggests that FTBIN[??] is ranked above
WSP. The ranking relationship between the metrical constraint FTBIN[??]
and the syllabic constraint ONSET remains to be determined. The data
from the minimal word phenomenon may shed light on how the dialect deals
with the conflict between syllabic and metrical considerations. In
Takituduh, tautomorphemic nonidentical vowel sequences fail to undergo
glide formation when there are just two vowels in the input, creating
violations of ONSET on the surface.
(21) Nonoccurrence of glide formation in #CVV(C)# words (19)
a. /tai[??]/ [ai] 'taro'
b. /haip/ [ai] 'now'
c. /pia[??]/ [ia] 'how many'
d. /via[??]/ [ia] 'knife'
e. /vau[??]/ [au] 'eight'
f. /qau[??]/ [au] 'pillar'
g. /naun/ [au] 'cat'
h. /kaut/ [au] 'female name'
i. /buan/ [ua] 'moon'
j. /puaq/ [ua] 'flower'
k. /tsui[??]/ [ui] 'money'
l. /quin/ [ui] (20) 'body hair'
m. /niun/ [iu] 'female name'
In addition, a vowel in Takituduh must lengthen if it is the only
vocalic element in the input:
(22) Vowel lengthening in Takituduh
a. /sak/ [saak] 'smell'
/sak-a/ [saka] 'smell!' (imp. patient voice)
/sak-un/ [sakun] 'smell' (patient voice)
b. /spat/ [paat] 'four'
/spat-in/ [patin] 'four' (imp.)
/RED-spat/ [saspat] 'four persons'
Given that Bunun does not have phonemic long vowels, the data (22)
must involve a vowel lengthening rather than shortening rule, based on
the morphophonemic alternations shown. These phonetically long vowels
involve one Root node linked up to two timing slots, with the second
timing slot heading an onsetless syllable (see tableau [29] below).
The disyllabic forms in (21) and (22) suggest that the ranking of
[FTBIN.sub.[??]] above ONSET forces the creation of onsetless syllables,
under the assumptions that a grammatical word is a prosodic word, that
the prosodic word must consist of at least one foot, and that the foot
must be disyllabic. When there are only two adjacent vowels in the
input, glide formation fails to take place, because its occurrence would
create a grammatical word that consists of a single monosyllabic foot.
When there is only one vocalic element in the input, lengthening occurs
in order to form a disyllabic foot, even though an onsetless syllable is
thus created. The generalization that a minimal word in Takituduh must
be disyllabic explains the lack of glide formation in #CVV(C)# words as
well as the lengthening of vowels in #CV(C)# words. (21)
Recall that in the discussion of the FTBIN constraint (Section
2.5), lengthening in Isbukun is also reported in monosyllabic words such
as /dan/[da:n] 'road,' which takes place to satisfy the
bimoraicity requirement. Because the lengthening effect is much more
prominent in Takituduh than in Isbukun based on the data collected, it
is proposed in the article that a minimal word is monosyllabic and
bimoraic in Isbukun but disyllabic and bimoraic in Takituduh. Isbukun
ranks ONSET higher than [FTBIN.sub.[??]], so minimal words are
monosyllabic and obey the onset requirement. Takituduh ranks
[FTBIN.sub.[??]] higher than ONSET, SO onsetless syllables are created
in minimal words.(22) The proposal here conforms to the hypothesis that
the minimal word is bimoraic in quantity-sensitive languages and
disyllabic in quantity-insensitive languages (McCarthy and Prince 1995b:
321-322).
3.4. An Optimality-theoretic analysis of the Takituduh data
The rankings between the following pairs of constraints have
already been mentioned in the above discussion of the Takituduh data:
(23)
(23) a. ONSET >> V-NUC
b. UNIFORMITY-IO >> ONSET
c. [FTBIN.sub.[??]] WSP
d. [FTBtN.sub.[??]] >> ONSET
The ranking ONSET >> V-Nut indicates that gliding is the
strategy used to avoid onsetless syllables, but gliding is prohibited if
it would create a configuration that violates other higher ranked
constraints such as [FTBIN.sub.[??]]. The ranking UNIFORMITY-IO >>
ONSET means that the language would not resort to coalescence to avoid
an onsetless syllable. The ranking of [FTBIN.sub.[??]] above WSP
reflects the quantity-insensitive character of Takituduh; that is, foot
forms are not constrained by the moraic composition of a syllable. The
metrical structure of Takituduh is not constrained syllabically either,
since [FTBIN.sub.[??]] ranks higher than ONSET. In contrast with
Isbukun, in which syllabic considerations may affect foot forms,
Takituduh exhibits a metrical influence on syllabification.
The ranking relationship among the four pairs of constraints is
shown in the diagram below, together with other relevant constraints:
(24) Constraint rankings for Takituduh Bunun
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Candidates (c), (d), (e) show that the language employs neither
vowel deletion nor consonant insertion nor coalescence, respectively, to
avoid onsetless syllables, because MAx-IO-V, DEP-IO-C and UNIFORMITY-IO
rank above ONSET and V-Nuc. Notice that in tableau (25), the metrical
constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]] does not play a role in winnowing out the
correct output, because the input contains more than two vocalic
elements. The gliding of one vowel still leaves enough vowels to form a
disyllabic foot.
If adjacent vowels are identical, vowel hiatus is observed on the
surface, because UNIFORMITY-IO prohibits the coalescence of the two
vowels.
If the two adjacent identical vowels are high, the candidate with
an onsetless syllable still wins, despite the possibility of gliding one
of the high vowels and thus violating the lowest ranked V-NUC, because
glide formation in this case would create a configuration violating the
higher ranked OCP-PLACE.
Candidate (b) loses to the optimal (a) because glide formation
creates a vowel-glide/glide-vowel sequence with identical place
features, violating the constraint OCP-PLACE. In a word, the constraint
tables (25)-(27) show that although the syllabic constraint ONSET forces
glide formation to take place in some cases, Takituduh exhibits several
cases of vowel hiatus due to the interaction of ONSET with other higher
ranked constraints. The constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]] does not have any
effect in these cases, because there are more than two vowels in the
input.
In the case of exactly two vowels in the input, each of the vowels
remains syllabic in the output due to the disyllabic requirement, as
shown below:
The constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]] rules out candidates (b), (c), and
(e) because gliding, vowel deletion, and coalescence all reduce the
number of syllables from two to one. Although insertion of an epenthetic
consonant would allow the formation of a disyllabic foot and avoid
violating [FTBIN.sub.[??]], as illustrated by candidate (d), the ranking
of DEP-IO-C above ONSET favors (a) with an onsetless syllable. (25)
Notice that if [FTBIN.sub.[??]] is ranked below ONSET as in the case of
Isbukun, the incorrect output (b) would be selected instead.
When there is just one vowel in the input, vowel lengthening takes
place in order to meet the requirement of a disyllabic foot, as shown in
tableau (29) below.
Maintaining the length of the input vowel would violate the
constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]], as illustrated by candidate (c). Candidate
(d) shows that insertion of both a consonant and a vowel to satisfy
[FTBIN.sub.[??]] and ONSET is considered less optimal than violating
just the ONSET constraint. Notice that candidate (a) with vowel
lengthening wins over (b) with vowel insertion, no matter whether
DEP-IO-V, the constraint against vowel insertion, ranks above or below
DEP-IO-[u], the constraint against vowel lengthening, because violation
of DEP-IO-V implies violation of DEP-IO-[u]; in cases where the
violation of DEP-IO-[u] is necessary in order to satisfy
[FTBIN.sub.[??]], DEP-IO-V will favor a representation with a linked
structure rather than the insertion of a whole segment. (27)
The proposed OT analysis of the Takituduh data shows that although
the syllabic constraint ONSET plays an important role in the
modification of vowel clusters, ONSET is dominated by other constraints,
leading to the occurrences of onsetless syllables under some
circumstances.
4. Summary: a comparison of the two dialects
The prosodic similarities and differences between Isbukun and
Takituduh can be summarized as follows. The two Bunun dialects are
similar in that they both employ glide formation as a strategy to deal
with underlying vowel sequences, which is reflected in the ranking ONSET
>> V-NUC.
Although Isbukun and Takituduh are similar in adopting gliding as a
strategy to avoid vowel clusters, they are different in how sequences of
rising sonority and sequences of identical vowels are dealt with in
suffixed forms. In Isbukun, vowel clusters are systematically avoided
both within and across morphemes through the application of gliding
between adjacent nonidentical vowels and coalescence between identical
vowels. In Takituduh, however, modifications of vowel clusters fail to
take place when suffixation creates a sequence of rising sonority or
identical vowels even though in other combinations gliding takes place
as in the case of Isbukun. The differences are attributed to the
undominated status of ONSET in Isbukun and the ranking of ONSET below
IDENT-BA-[??] and UNIFORMITY-IO in Takituduh.
Takituduh and Isbukun are also different in their stress patterns.
Takituduh stress falls on the final syllable, and its
quantity-insensitive character is reflected in the ranking
[FtBin.sub.[??]] >> WSP. Isbukun stress is basically penultimate,
but exhibits final stress when gliding or coalescence creates a final
heavy syllable. In Isbukun, the quantity-sensitive character is captured
by the ranking WSP >> [FtBin.sub.[??]]; ONSET also needs to rank
higher than [FtBin.sub.[??]], in order to account for the final stress
pattern.
Another difference between the two dialects is how the minimal word
requirement is fulfilled. While a content word in Takituduh must be
minimally disyllabic, an Isbukun content word can be monosyllabic and
bimoraic. Based on the assumption that a content word is a prosodic
word, which consists of at least one foot, the analysis captures the
difference by proposing that [FTBIN.sub.[??]] is ranked differently with
respect to other constraints in the two dialects. Specifically,
Takituduh ranks [FTBIN.sub.[??]] above ONSET, which leads to the
appearance of onsetless syllables when the input contains no more than
two vowels, and Isbukun ranks [FTBIN.sub.[??]] below ONSET, which is
responsible for the regular occurrence of gliding even if the input
contains just two vowels as well as for the marked final stress pattern.
5. Discussion and conclusion
The article has illustrated the prosodic characteristics of the two
dialects of Bunun and formalized their similarities and differences by
means of the constraint ranking mechanism in OT. The two dialects under
comparison are distinct from each other in how syllabic and metrical
constraints rank with respect to each other, although they are similar
in that violation of ONSET is primarily resolved through glide
formation. In Isbukun, the syllabic constraint ONSET has priority over
the metrical constraint [FTBIN.sub.[??]]; when the concatenation of a
vowel-ending stem and a vowel-initial suffix renders the satisfaction of
both ONSET and [FTBIN.sub.[??]] impossible, the dialect chooses to
maintain syllable wellformedness at the cost of creating marked
monosyllabic feet. In contrast, Takituduh opts for the preferred
disyllabic foot form when the demands of syllabic and metrical
constraints are in conflict, ranking ONSET below [FTBIN.sub.[??]]. The
different results of the competition between syllabic and metrical
constraints within the same language family are captured naturally
within the framework of OT by ranking the constraints ONSET and
[FTBIN.sub.[??]] differently in the two dialects.
An output-oriented theory such as OT is advantageous in analyzing
the facts in the two Bunun dialects. First of all, the framework of OT
allows syllabic and metrical constraints to interact in the same
hierarchy, and such parallelism makes it possible to account for both
the top-down influence of foot forms on syllabification in Takituduh and
the fact that foot forms are constrained syllabically and moraically in
Isbukun. As already pointed out in previous studies (Prince and
Smolensky 1993; Rosenthall 1997b), it is difficult to capture the effect
of metrical structure on syllabification in an approach that builds
prosodic structures from the bottom up. Moreover, in Isbukun, the
relatively high-ranked constraint WSP directly expresses the correlation
between the static distribution of stress in nonsuffixed words and the
dynamic assignment of stress in morphophonemic alternations. That is,
the need to avoid heavy syllables in the nonhead position of a foot is
responsible for the lack of words with penultimate stress and final
heavy syllables in nonsuffixed words, and also for the different stress
patterns induced by consonant-ending versus vowel-ending stems in
suffixed words.
One important implication of the analysis of Isbukun is that glides
that derive from an underlying vowel may pattern together with
consonants or vowels on the surface, a counterexample to the prediction
in Levi (2003) that if a language has only derived glides but not
underlying glides, the derived glides in the language should pattern
with vowels. In Isbukun, glide-ending stems pattern with
consonant-ending stems in inducing penultimate stress in the suffixed
forms, despite the fact that surface glides are exclusively derived from
underlying vowels. The Isbukun data suggest that the behavior of a
derived glide in stress assignment is not tied to its underlying status
but is dependent on its syllabic affiliation on the surface.
Furthermore, the coalescence analysis of Isbukun vowel clusters
shows that IDENT-IO-[u] is problematic in winnowing out the correct
output; the analysis can be successful only if we adopt the constraint
MAx-IO-[u], which makes direct reference to whether the mora of an input
segment is preserved in the output. The high ranking of MAx-IO-[u] and
the low ranking of V-NUC in Isbukun reflect the fact that underlying
vowels remain moraic on the surface even though they do not exclusively
occupy the nucleus of a syllable on the surface due to the ONSET
constraint. The data from Isbukun Bunun shows that the loss of
syllabicity does not necessarily entail the loss of moraicity of a
segment.
Received 27 August 2003 Revised version received 5 October 2004
References
Bernhardt, Barbara H. and Stemberger, Joseph P. (1998). Handbook of
Phonological Development." From the Perspective of Constraint-Based
Nonlinear Phonology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Garrett, Edward (1999). Minimal words aren't minimal feet. In
Papers in Phonology 2, UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics 1, Matthew K.
Gorden (ed.), 68 105. Los Angeles: UCLA Linguistics Department.
Harris, James W. and Kaisse, Ellen M. (1999). Palatal vowels,
glides and obstruents in Argentinian Spanish. Phonology 16, 117 190.
He, Ru-fen; Zeng, Si-qi; Li, Wen-shu; and Lin, Qing-chun (1986).
Gaoshanzu Yuyan Jianzhi (Bunenyu) [An outline of the Formosan native
languages (Bunun)]. Beijing: Min-Zu.
Huang, Hui-chuan J. (2002). Zhuoshe Bunongyu de huayin xingcheng
guilu [Glide formation in Takituduh Bunun]. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese
Studies 32(2), 441-468. Hsinchu, Taiwan: National Tsing Hua University.
Huang, Lillian (1997). Gaoxiongxian de bunongyu [Isbukun Bunun in
Gaoxiong County]. In Gaoxiongxian Nandao Yuyan [Formosan Austronesian
languages in Gaoxiong County], Paul J.-K. Li (ed.), 351-410. Gaoxiong:
Gaoxiong County Government.
Jeng, Heng-hsiung (1977). Topic and Focus in Bunun. Institute of
History and Philology, Special Publication No. 72. Taipei: Academia
Sinica.
Jeng, Heng-syung (1999). Bunun tense and aspect. In Selected Papers
from the Eighth International Conference on Austronesian Linguistics,
Symposium Series of the Institute of Linguistics (Preparatory Office)
No. 1, Elizabeth Zeitoun and Paul Jen-kuei Li (eds.), 455-487. Taipei:
Academia Sinica.
Kager, Rene (1999). Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Levi, Susannah (2003). Representing underlying distinctions between
vowels and glides. Paper presented at the 77th annual meeting of the
Linguistic Society of America, Atlanta.
Li, Paul J.-K. (1987). The preglottalised stops in Bunun. In A Worm
of Language: Papers Presented to Prof S. A. Wurm on His 65th Birthday,
Pacific Linguistics 100, Donald C. Laycock and Werner Winter (eds.), 381
387. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
--(1988). A comparative study of Bunun dialects. Bulletin of the
Institute of History and Philology 59(2), 479 508.
--(1992). Taiwan Nandao Yuyan de Yuvin Fuhao Xitong [Orthographic
systems for Formo- san languages]. Taipei: Educational Research
Committee, Ministry of Education, Taiwan.
--(1997). Nantouxian Xinyixiang de bunong nanbu fangyan [Isbukun
Bunun in Xinyi Township, Nantou County]. In Gaoxiongxian Nandao Yuyan
[Formosan Austronesian languages in Gaoxiong County], Paul J.-K. Li
(ed.), 300-350. Gaoxiong: Gaoxiong County Government.
Lin, Hsiu-hsu (1996). Isbukun phonology: a study of its segments,
syllable structure and phonological processes. Unpublished Master's
thesis, National Tsing Hua University.
Lin, Tai; Zeng, Si-qi; Li, Wen-shu; and Ismahasan, Bukun (2001).
Isbukun Bunongyu Goucifit Yanjiu [A study on the morphology of Isbukun
Bunun]. Taipei: Ducewenhua.
McCarthy, John and Prince, Alan (1993). Prosodic morphology I:
constraint interaction and satisfaction. Unpublished manuscript,
University of Massachusetts, Amherst and Rutgers University.
--and Prince, Alan (1995a). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Papers in Optimality Theory, University of Massachusetts
Occasional Papers 18, Jill N. Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, and Suzanne
Urbanczyk (eds.), 249-384. Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student
Association.
--and Prince, Alan (1995b). Prosodic morphology. In The Handbook of
Phonological Theory, John Goldsmith (ed.), 318 366. Cambridge, MA:
Blackwell.
Nojima, Motoyasu (1996). Lexical prefixes of Bunun verbs. Gengo
Kenkyu 110, 1-27. Prince, Alan (1980). A metrical theory for Estonian
quantity. Linguistic Inquiry 11, 511-562.
--(1983). Relating to the grid. Linguistic Inquiry 14, 19-100.
--(1990). Quantitative consequences of rhythmic organization.
Chicago Linguistic Society (26)2, 355-398.
--and Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint
interaction in generative grammar. Unpublished manuscript, Rutgers
University and University of Colorado.
Rosenthall, Sam (1997a). Vowel/Glide Alternation in a Theory of
Constraint Interaction. New York: Garland.
-- (1997b). Metrical influence on syllable structure in Lenakel.
Linguistic Inquiry 28(1), 187193.
Rowicka, Grazyna (1999). Prosodic optimality and prefixation in
Polish. In The Prosody Morphology Interface, Rene Kager, Harry van der
Hulst, and Wim Zonneveld (eds.), 367-389. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Rubach, Jerzy (2000). Glide and glottal stop insertion in Slavic
languages: a DOT analysis. Linguistic Inquiry 31(2), 271-317.
Selkirk, Elisabeth (1980). The roles of prosodic categories in
English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry 11,563-605.
Zeitoun, Elizabeth (2000). Bunongyu Cankao Yufa [A reference
grammar of Bunun]. Taipei: Yuanliu.
Notes
* The article was presented at the 15th North American Conference
on Chinese Linguistics. I thank the audience at the conference for
helpful comments. Sincere gratitude also goes to the major Isbukun
consultants Bukun Ismahasan and Lamata Istanda, and to the Takituduh
consultants Maduvaian Savung and Talum Qalavangan. The research leading
to this article was supported by NSC90-2411-H-007-025 in Taiwan.
Correspondence address: Graduate Institute of Linguistics, National
Tsing Hua University, 101, Section 2, Kuangfu Road, Hsinchu, Taiwan,
300. E-mail: hcjhuang@mx.nthu.edu.tw.
(1.) Also see Li (1987, 1988) for a cross-dialect perspective, and
Jeng (1977) on Takbanua[??] Bunun.
(2.) Unless noted otherwise, all the data presented in the article
are based on the author's own continuing fieldwork, which is
comprised of sentence elicitations and short narratives so far. The
generalizations regarding stress placement as presented in the article
are consistent with either type of data, but the article does not
address all the observed processes that affect underlying vowel
sequences; see Note 5.
(3.) The palatalization rule may optionally change/s/to [[??]]
after [ij].
(4.) This is different from the usage of "glide" as a
mora-less vowel in standard moraic theory, as pointed out by an
anonymous reviewer. As will become evident in the following discussion,
the low-sonority high vowels in a vowel sequence are forced by the ONSET
constraint to occupy the onset position and become nonmoraic, and when
such vowels occupy nonsyllable-initial position, they remain moraic on
the surface. Because both types of segments are derived by the onset
requirement, the article uses one term "glide" to refer to
them, despite the fact that they are different in moraicity.
(5.) A reviewer points out that there are data involving the
additional changes of/ai, au/ to [i] and [[??]]: the change of /au/to
[[??]] is observed especially in connected speech, and the change of/ai/
to [i] occurs within a word (e.g., maisna/misna 'from') as
well as across words (e.g., minsuma-in/minsumin 'to have
appeared'). Although the monophthongization of /au/to [[??]] and
the change of /ai/to [i] are also processes that affect vowel sequences,
they are not discussed here for the following reasons. The reduction
nature of monophthongization is not directly related to the onset
requirement, and its phonetic realization is gradient, manifesting much
intra- and interspeaker variations. As far as the change of/ai/to [i] is
concerned, it is unclear how many roots are affected by this optional
rule (Li 1997: 306), and it seems that there are also variations in how
speakers use the forms with the reduced [i]. The/-in/ suffix as in
/minsumain/ is nonstress-shifting whereas the article includes only
stress-shifting suffixes (see Section 2.4). Notice that no matter
whether/au/monophthongizes to [[??]] and whether /ai/ changes to [i],
they do not affect the generalization in the article that Isbukun avoids
the unmarked penultimate stress when the final syllable is heavy. The
apparent violations in cases such as /minsuma-in/ [minsumajn] do not
constitute a counterexample because they involve different types of
suffixes.
(6.) As the data given in (1), (2), (4), and (5), the data (6)-(9)
contain an agent voice form in the left and a suffixed form to its
right. The agent voice forms in the left column carry the agent voice
marker ma-, m-, or null, which might not show up in the suffixed forms
if there is a conflict in voice. The apparent alternations involving the
beginning consonants in examples (6b), (7a), and (7b) are due to a
separate process deleting the initial consonants in forms with the agent
voice marker m-.
(7.) The dots indicate syllable boundaries, and the parentheses
represent foot structures.
(8.) In a narrow transcription,/kusbai-a/and/haiap/are pronounced
as [kus.baj.ja] and [haj.jap], respectively, in which the
syllable-initial glides are also perceived as part of the preceding
syllables.
(9.) Kager (1999: 172) uses the constraints RhType=T and RhType=l
to state whether a rhythmic type should be trochaic or iambic.
(10.) Rowicka (1999: 373) also proposes that there should be
separate [FTBIN.sub.[??]] and [FTBIN.sub.[u]] constraints.
(11.) The data in Lin (1996) are based on the Isbukun dialect
spoken in Nantou County. The lengthening phenomenon is observed in
Gaoxiong Isbukun too, as illustrated by /spat/ [pa:t] 'four'.
(12.) Presumably [FTBIN.sub.[??]] is ranked only below the
faithfulness constraint that demands underlying stress to be preserved.
(13). The article assumes that a foot consisting of a single
syllable does not violate FTTYPE (trochaic); a monosyllabic foot
violates [FTBIN.sub.[??]] but not FTTYPE (trochaic).
(14.) In addition, to account for the fact that [pi[??]kaw(bwav)]
is more optimal than *[pi[??](kawbwav)], we seem to need a separate
higher-ranked WSP constraint, which prohibits only heavy syllables in
the nonhead position of a foot but not those unstressed heavy syllables
that are linked directly to the Prosodic Word node. Alternatively, a
constraint that prefers the stressed syllable to be right-aligned would
rule out the incorrect *[pi[??](kawbwav)].
(15.) Notice that the ranking ONSET >> IDENT-IO-[u] (e.g.,
Rosenthall 1997b) alone cannot capture the changes of /i u/to [j w] in
Isbukun either, because the constraint IDENTIO-[u] is violated by
syllable-initial glides but satisfied by noninitial glides in Isbukun.
(16.) Whether bimoraic vowels such as those in the final syllables
of [tutun] and [tupav] are consistently longer than their monomoraic
counterparts is an issue requiring a phonetic study. If these syllables
are not phonetically longer, stress placement still provides
phonological evidence for the abstract bimoraic representation. If these
bimoraic syllables are significantly longer, we cannot infer the
bimoraic representation from the duration alone because the length
effect could simply be the result of being the head of a foot.
Therefore, although the final heavy syllables in words such as [tutfin]
and [tupav] sometimes sound longer, they are not marked for length
throughout the article.
(17.) While the nonagent voice marker in imperatives is /-av/ in
Isbukun, it is /-i/ in Takituduh.
(18.) Huang (2002: 453) also notes that these onsetless syllables
might not be as obvious in fast speech.
(19.) The corresponding forms in Isbukun are monosyllabic, such
as/buan/[bwan] 'moon' and/pia/[pja] 'how many'.
(20.) The vowel/u/is pronounced more like [o] when it is adjacent
to the uvular/q/.
(21.) If the minimal word phenomenon is not connected to foot
structure, as argued in Garrett (1999), the disyllabicity of minimal
words does not necessarily argue for the high ranking of
[FTBIN.sub.[??]], but for the high ranking of some other constraints
which lead to the minimality requirement, such as the BE-LONG constraint
proposed in Garrett. Although many languages surveyed in Garrett (1999)
exhibit a lack of connection between foot structure and minimality, the
data from Takituduh Bunun are compatible with either a foot structural
explanation or an approach based on length. In either account, the
comparison between Takituduh and Isbukun shows the different roles of
the syllabic constraint ONSET in the two dialects.
(22.) There seems to be no positive evidence suggesting how
[FtBin.sub.[u]], is ranked with respect to other constraints in
Takituduh. Although ranking [FtBin.sub.[u]] above or below
[FTBIN.sub.[??]] will give rise to different metrifications for words
that contain more than two vowels in the input (e.g., /kavia[??]/
'friend' [ka(vja[??])] or [(kavja[??])]), either of the
rankings is consistent with the final stress pattern in Takituduh.
(23.) The ranking IDENT-BA-[??] >> ONSET in Takitituduh is
omitted from the presentation of the OT analysis. Notice that ONSET must
rank above IDENT-BA-[??] in Isbukun because the onset requirement is
strictly obeyed in the dialect.
(24.) The symbol 'T' represents an epenthetic consonant.
(25.) The ranking of DEP-IO-C above ONSET predicts that insertion
of a consonant to avoid onsetless syllables is not possible in either
word-initial or word-medial position. In the Takituduh data collected so
far, I have not observed vowel-initial words.
(26.) The ranking of DEP-IO-V above ONSET is justified by the
realization of /spat/ 'four' as [paat] rather than *[sVpat]
('V' represents an epenthetic vowel). If ONSET ranks above
DEP-IO-V, [paat] would incorrectly be considered less optimal than
*[sVpat]. The preference of [paat] over *[sVpat] also suggests that
DEP-IO-V must rank above Max-IO-C.
(27.) The optimal candidate in (29) contains a doubly linked
representation, which presumably violates a low-ranked constraint
DOUBLYLINKED (Bernhardt and Stemberger 1998). The low ranking of
DOUBLYLINKED raises the question of whether the correct phonological
representation of words such as /buan/ should be [buwan], with the Root
node of /u/ linked to both the nucleus of the first syllable and the
onset of the following syllable, rather than [buan] as shown in Tableau
(28). Technically, whether [buan] or the [buwan] with double linking
would win depends on the ranking between ONSET and DOUBLYLINKED:
DOUBLYLINKED >> ONSET gives rise to [buan] and the opposite
ranking results in [buwan]. However, the output form [saak] does not
provide positive evidence for the ranking between the two constraints;
it only justifies the pairs of ranking [FTBIN.sub.[??]] >> ONSET
and [FTBIN.sub.[??]] >> DOUBLYLINKED. Since onsetless syllables
must be tolerated in minimal words such as [saak], the article assumes
in (28) that the onsetless candidate [buan] wins.
HUI-CHUAN J. HUANG
National Tsing Hua University, Taiwan
(13) Tableau: consonant-ending stems
/ludah-av/ FTTYPE WSP ONS MAX- OCP- MAX-
(trochaic) IO-u PLACE IO-V
* a. lu.(da.hav)
b. lu. da.(hav)
c. lu.(da.hav) *!
/ludah-av/ [FTBIN. UNIFORMITY- V-
sub.[??]] IO NUC
* a. lu.(da.hav)
b. lu. da.(hav)
c. lu.(da.hav) *!
(14) Tableau: vowel hiatus resolved through gliding
/[sa.sub.u] FTTYPE WSP ONS MAX- OCP- MAX-
[du.sub.u]- (trochaic) IO-u PLACE IO-V
[a.sub.u]v/
* a. [sa.sub.u] *
([dw.sub.u]
[a.sub.u]v)
b. ([sa.sub.u] * *!
[dw.sub.u]
[a.sub.u]v)
c. [sa.sub.u] * *!
([dwa.sub.u]v)
d. ([sa.sub.u] * *!
[dw.sub.u.]
[a.sub.u]v)
e. ([sa.sub.u] * *!
[dwa.sub.u]v)
f. [sa.sub.u] * *!
([du.sub.u] *
[a.sub.u]v)
g. [sa.sub.u] *
h. ([sa.sub.u] * *!
[do.sub.u]v)
i. [sa.sub.u] * *!
([da.sub.uu]v)
/[sa.sub.u] [FTBIN. UNIFORMITY V-
[du.sub.u]- sub.[??]] IO NUC
[a.sub.u]v/
* a. [sa.sub.u] * * *
([dw.sub.u]
[a.sub.u]v)
b. ([sa.sub.u] * *
[dw.sub.u]
[a.sub.u]v)
c. [sa.sub.u] * * *
([dwa.sub.u]v)
d. ([sa.sub.u] * *
[dw.sub.u.]
[a.sub.u]v)
e. ([sa.sub.u] * *
[dwa.sub.u]v)
f. [sa.sub.u] *
([du.sub.u] *
[a.sub.u]v)
g. [sa.sub.u] * * *!
h. ([sa.sub.u] * *
[do.sub.u]v)
i. [sa.sub.u] * *
([da.sub.uu]v)
(15) Tableau: vowel hiatus resolved through coalescence
/[tu.sub.u][tu.sup.1.sub.u]- FTTYPE WSP ONS MAX- OCP-
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n/ (trochaic) IO-u PLACE
a. [tu.sub.u]* *!
([tw.sup.1.sub.u]
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
b. ([tu.sub.u]* *! *
[tw.sup.1.sub.u]
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
c. [tu.sub.u]* *! *
([tu.sup.1.sub.u]
[w.sup.2]n)
d. ([tu.sub.u]* *! *
[tu.sup.1.sub.u]
[w.sup.2.sub.u]n)
e. ([tu.sub.u]* *! *
[tw.sup.1]
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
f. [tu.sub.u]* *!
([tu.sup.1.sub.u]*
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
* g. [tu.sub.u]*
([tu.sup.1,2.sub.uu]n)
h. ([tu.sub.u]* *!
[tu.sup.1,2.sub.u]n)
i. [tu.sub.u]*
([tu.sup.2.sub.uu]n)
/[tu.sub.u][tu.sup.1.sub.u]- MAX- [FTBIN. UNIFOR- V-
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n/ IO-V sub.[??]] MITY NUC
a. [tu.sub.u]* * *
([tw.sup.1.sub.u]
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
b. ([tu.sub.u]* *
[tw.sup.1.sub.u]
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
c. [tu.sub.u]* * *
([tu.sup.1.sub.u]
[w.sup.2]n)
d. ([tu.sub.u] *
[tu.sup.1.sub.u]
[w.sup.2.sub.u]n)
e. ([tu.sub.u]* *
[tw.sup.1]
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
f. [tu.sub.u]*
([tu.sup.1.sub.u]*
[u.sup.2.sub.u]n)
* g. [tu.sub.u]* * *
([tu.sup.1,2.sub.uu]n)
h. ([tu.sub.u]* *
[tu.sup.1,2.sub.u]n)
i. [tu.sub.u]* *! *
([tu.sup.2.sub.uu]n)
(16) Tableau: low vowels in hiatus
/[tu.sub.u][pa.sup.1.sub.u]- FTTYPE WSP ONS MAX- OCP-
[a.sup.2.sub.u]v/ (trochaic) IO-u PLACE
a. [tu.sub.u]* *!
([pa.sup.1.sub.u]*
[a.sup.2.sub.u]v)
* b. [tu.sub.u]*
([pa.sup.1,2.sub.uu]v)
c. [tu.sub.u]* *!
([pa.sup.1,2.sub.u]v)
d. [tu.sub.u]*
([pa.sup.2.sub.uu]v)
/[tu.sub.u][pa.sup.1.sub.u]- MAX- [FTBIN. UNIFOR- V-
[a.sup.2.sub.u]v/ IO-V sub.[??]] MITY NUC
a. [tu.sub.u]*
([pa.sup.1.sub.u]*
[a.sup.2.sub.u]v)
* b. [tu.sub.u]* * *
([pa.sup.1,2.sub.uu]v)
c. [tu.sub.u]* *
([pa.sup.1,2.sub.u]v)
d. [tu.sub.u]* *! *
([pa.sup.2.sub.uu]v)
Tableau (25) shows that due to the ranking of ONSET above V-NUC,
gliding takes place to deal with vowel clusters that arise through
affixation.
(25)
/tan[??]a-un/ [FTBIN.sub.[??]] UNIFORMITY- MAX- DEP-
IO IO-V IO-C
a. tan[??]aun
* b. tan[??]awn
c. tan[??]un *!
d. tan[??]aTun (24) *!
e. tan[??]on *!
/tan[??]a-un/ DEP- ONSET DEP- V-
IO-V IO-u NUC
a. tan[??]aun *!
* b. tan[??]awn
c. tan[??]un *
d. tan[??]aTun (24)
e. tan[??]on
(26)
/tan[??][a.sup.1]-[a.sup.2]/ [FTBIN.sub.[??]] UNIFORMITY- MAX-
IO IO-V
* a. tan[??][a.sup.1][a.sup.2]
b. tan[??][a.sup.2] *!
c. tan[??][a.sup.1][Ta.sup.2]
d. tan[??][a.sup.1,2] *!
/tan[??][a.sup.1]-[a.sup.2]/ DEP- DEP- ONSET DEP- V-
IO-C IO-V IO-u NUC
* a. tan[??][a.sup.1][a.sup.2] *!
b. tan[??][a.sup.2]
c. tan[??][a.sup.1][Ta.sup.2] *!
d. tan[??][a.sup.1,2]
(27)
/[kitlalavi.sup.1]-[i.sup.2]/ [FTBIN.sub.[??]] UNIFORMITY-
IO
* a. [kitlalavi.sup.1]-[i.sup.2]
b. [kitlalavi.sup.1]-[j.sup.2]
c. [kitlalavi.sup.2]
d. [kitlalavi.sup.1][Ti.sup.2]
e. [kitlalavi.sup.1,2] *!
/[kitlalavi.sup.1]-[i.sup.2]/ MAX- DEP- DEP- OCP- ONSET
IO-V IO-C IO-V PLACE
* a. [kitlalavi.sup.1]-[i.sup.2] *
b. [kitlalavi.sup.1]-[j.sup.2] *!
c. [kitlalavi.sup.2] *!
d. [kitlalavi.sup.1][Ti.sup.2] *!
e. [kitlalavi.sup.1,2]
/[kitlalavi.sup.1]-[i.sup.2]/ DEP- V-
IO-u NUC
* a. [kitlalavi.sup.1]-[i.sup.2]
b. [kitlalavi.sup.1]-[j.sup.2] *
c. [kitlalavi.sup.2]
d. [kitlalavi.sup.1][Ti.sup.2]
e. [kitlalavi.sup.1,2]
(28)
/buan/ [FTBIN.sub.[??]] UNIFORMITY- MAX- DEP- DEP-
IO IO-V IO-C IO-V
* a. buan
b. bwan *!
c. ban *! *
d. buTan *!
e. bon *! *
/buan/ ONSET DEP- V-
IO-u NUC
* a. buan *
b. bwan *
c. ban
d. buTan
e. bon
(29)
/sak/ [FTBIN.sub.[??]] UNIFORMITY- MAX- DEP- DEP-
IO IO-V IO-C IO-V (26)
u u
RT
* a. saak
u u *!
RT RT
b. saak
c. sak *!
d. saTak *! *
/sak/ ONSET DEP- V-
IO-u NUC
u u * *
RT
* a. saak
u u * *
RT RT
b. saak
c. sak
d. saTak *