No "double plurals" in Dominican Spanish: an optimality-theoretic account *.
Colina, Sonia
Abstract
In addition to standard methods of pluralization, Dominican Spanish has an alternative plural formation mechanism, normally referred to as
the "double plural," in which -(e)se [(e)se] is adjoined to
the base, libro > librose 'book-books', mujer > mujerese
'woman-women' (Jimenez Sabater 1976; Nunez-Cedeno 1980; Harris
1980; Terrell 1986; Nunez-Cedeno 2003). Extant analyses of double plural
formation in Dominican Spanish suffer from framework-specific problems
and more general ones, having to do with overgeneration of forms and
inability to reveal the true nature of the process. Most analyses
postulate a separate plural morpheme for Dominican "double
plurals"/(e)se/.
This article proposes an optimality-theoretic analysis that
demonstrates that there is no "double plural" in Dominican in
the sense that the "double plural" is based on the regular
plural There is only one plural morpheme, the traditional /s/. The
apparently redundant attachment of the plural morpheme results from
general restrictions on coda obstruents in combination with the need for
overt realization of morphemes in prominent positions. The difference
between the regular plural and the double plural (focused) is that in
the latter case, a highly-ranked constraint realize morpheme-focus
requires that the plural in focused positions have morphological
exponence. In more general terms, [se] is the output realization of
plural /s/ followed by epenthetic [e] in intonationally prominent
(focus) positions. The present analysis also shows that the plural form is in an output-to-output relation to the singular. Epenthesis of [e] in
the plural reflects the emergence of the unmarked (McCarthy and Prince
1994), with respect to the constraint against coda consonants (*Coda),
whose effects can be seen in the output-to-output phonology of the
plural, but not in the language as a whole, for example, mujer vs.
mujere.
1. Introduction
Dominican Spanish has what appears to be quite an odd method of
plural formation. More specifically, in addition to regular
pluralization mechanisms, Dominican Spanish resorts to what has been
dubbed in the literature as the "double plural" in which it
appears that the plural morpheme has been attached twice by adding the
additional allomorph es after the traditional (e)s: mujer, mujer-es
[mu.he.re], mujerese /muher-s-s/ [mu.he.re.se] 'woman/women'
(coda /s/ is deleted). All existent analyses of the phenomenon resort to
some form of stipulative mechanism: separate, underlyingly-specified
morphemes, prespecifled templates, language-specific, or stipulatory rules, etc. Harris (1980), for instance, proposes a language-specific
prosodic template [[...] VCV] (realized as [[ese] or [se]) in addition
to that of the regular plural; Nunez-Cedeno (1980) resorts to an
optional, language-specific rule that inserts an epenthetic [e] after
plural [s] in word-final position; and Nunez-Cedeno (2003) argues for
two separate underlying allomorphs, one for the regular plural and one
for the double plural. I show that all these language-specific,
stipulative mechanisms are unnecessary; furthermore, their stipulative
nature obscures the more general nature of the process.
This article demonstrates that there is no real "double
plural" in Dominican Spanish in the sense that the "double
plural" is based on the regular plural. There is only one plural
morpheme, the traditional /s/. The apparently redundant attachment of
the plural morpheme results from general restrictions on coda obstruents
in combination with the need for overt realization of morphemes in
prominent positions. The optimalitytheoretic (OT) analysis proposed
reveals that the difference between the regular plural and the double
plural (focused) is that in the latter case, a highly-ranked constraint
realize morpheme-focus (RM/FOC) requires that the plural in focused
positions have morphological exponence. In more general terms, [se] is
the output realization of plural /s/ followed by epenthetic [e] in
intonationally prominent (focus) positions.
An important contribution of the present analysis is that it shows
that the plural form is in an output-to-output (OO) relation to the
singular. More specifically, epenthesis of [e] in the plural reflects
the emergence of the unmarked (McCarthy and Prince 1994), with respect
to the constraint against coda consonants (*Coda), whose effects can be
seen in the OO phonology of the plural, but not in the language as a
whole, for example, mujer vs. mujere. This has important implications
for the debate on the nature of [e] epenthesis and plural epenthesis in
Spanish (phonological vs. morphological [Harris 1999]).
From a theoretical standpoint, the analysis proposed here
highlights the superiority of a correspondence-theoretic account over
serial analyses. An OO account reveals the natural and universals
aspects of Dominican plural formation generally obscured by derivational formalisms. Theoretical implications can also be drawn with regard to a
theory of prominence (Beckman 1997).
The article is organized as follows. After the presentation of the
data in Section 2, Section 3 reviews and evaluates the existing analyses
of "double plurals" in Dominican. The analysis proposed
appears in Section 4: Section 4.1 summarizes the main points of the
proposal in pretheoretical terms, in preparation for the formal
optimality-theoretic analysis in Section 4.2. The analysis in Section
4.2 focuses on nonplural coda obstruents (Section 4.2.1), realize
morpheme and OO-constraints (Section 4.2.2), plurals of V-final bases
(Section 4.2.3), plurals of C-final bases (Section 4.2.4), and stress
restrictions on pluralization (Section 4.2.5). Some conclusions are
drawn in Section 5.
2. Data
Standard plural formation in Spanish seemingly consists of adding
-s [s] to nonverbs ending in unstressed vowels, as in libro, libros
'book, books', and -es [es] to those ending in consonants,
mujer, mujeres 'woman, women'. (1) Existing exceptions to this
surface generalization, for example, crisis, crisis 'crisis,
crises' (vs. regular lapiz[s], lapic[s]es 'pencil,
pencils'), are reported in the standard sources (Foley 1967;
Saltarelli 1970; Contreras 1977; Harris 1980).
Dominican Spanish, however, has developed an alternative plural
formarion mechanism, normally referred to as the "double
plural," in which -(e)se [(e)se], supposedly /s-s/, is adjoined to
the nonverb base, libro, librose 'book, books', mujer,
mujerese 'woman, women' (Jimenez Sabater 1975; Nunez-Cedeno
1980; Harris 1980; Terrell 1986; and Nunez-Cedeno 2003). This can be
seen in (1) and in (3)-(4) below. Double-plural usage is found in
informationally prominent positions, but is socially conditioned, being
more prevalent amongst those with little formal education.
(1) Base Double plural Gloss (2)
gallina gallinase 'hens'
lata latase 'cans'
pintura pinturase 'paints'
esto estose 'these'
eso esose 'those'
muchacho muchachose 'boys'
arriba arribase 'ups'
mujer mujerese 'women'
pan panese 'breads'
papel papelese 'papers'
vudu vuduse 'voodoos'
aji ajise 'peppers'
Dominican Spanish does not normally allow coda obstruents (Jimenez
Sabater 1975; Pineros 2003). These are often realized as zero (usted,
ustedes [u.te] [u.te.De] 'you-polite, sg. and pl.')
(aspiration of /s/ is also possible although less common in popular
speech). Consequently, [s] is affected by coda restrictions in
word-internal and word-final positions (Jimenez Sabater 1975; Pineros
2003). (3)
(2) mosca [moka] 'fly'
basta [bata] 'enough'
asno [ano] 'donkey'
gas [ga] 'gas'
virus [bi.ru] 'virus'
In accordance with the ill-formedness of coda [s], plural /s/ is
realized as zero in popular speech, thus surfacing as zero [o] in
vowel-final forms (3) and [e] in consonant-final ones (4). (3) and (4)
also contain the corresponding double plurals.
(3) Bases ending in unstressed final vowels:
Singular Plural Double plural Gloss
gallina gallina[o] gallinase 'hens'
lata lata[o] latase 'cans'
pintura pintura[o] pinturase 'paints'
e(s)to e(s)to[o] e(s)tose 'these'
eso eso[o] esose 'those'
muchacho muchacho[o] muchachose 'boys'
arriba arriba[o] arribase 'ups'
(4) Bases ending in consonants or stressed vowels:
Singular Plural Double plural Gloss
mujer mujere[o] mujerese 'women'
pan pane[o] panese 'breads'
papel papele[o] papelese 'papers'
vudu vudu[o] vuduse 'voodoos'
aji aji[o] ajise 'peppers'
Nunez-Cedeno (2003) introduces data that indicate that
double-plural selection (vs. [o] or [e]) is governed by pragmatic
factors, being selected in informationally-prominent, focus positions.
The double plural is realized only on the lexical head of a NP in focus
position, indicated here by the use of CAPITALS, for example, la
AMARILLASE vs. * lase AMARILLASE (double plural on the entire NP). The
examples in (5) from Nunez-Cedeno (2003) illustrate the use of double
plurals in context (the corresponding nonfocus forms are marked in [5]
by the use of italics).
(5) (Ramon, male, age 28)
Researcher: Ramon, bueno y que va a hacer con esas pinturas?
'Well, Ramon, what are you going to do with those paints?'
Ramon: Pue la AMARILLASE la subo pa la azotea. No la BLANCASE;
ESASE se quedan aqui porque hay que pintal toa la PAREDESE, y usar la
pintura. Si yo le digo que dona Remi na ma me da a mi pa comprar lata de
PINTURASE y fijese lo grande que son la parede, eso no se pinta con tan
chin pintura. 'I am bringing the yellow ones up to the roof. Not
the white ones; those will stay here because I got to paint all walls
and use the paints. I tell you, dona Remi just gives me enough to buy
two cans of paint and look how big these walls are, they cannot be
painted with so little paint.'
(6) Researcher: Me imagino que se comen la poca grama que queda,
esta muy seco, pero tambien hay gallina. 'I think they are eating
up the little there is, it is dry but there are some hens as well.'
Don Otilio: No, GALLiNASE no, esa son PALOMASE. 'No,
they're not hens, those are pigeons.'
Finally, Nunez-Cedeno (2003) also notes stress-related
restrictions. The double plural is not documented in proparoxytonic
words: platano, *platanose 'plantain', where it would result
in preantepenultimate stress (not permitted in Spanish); no double
plural exists either in the case of C-final paroxytonic forms, util,
*utilese 'useful', virgen, *virgenese 'virgin',
agil, *agilese, 'limber' (regular nonfocused plurals are,
however, possible, utile, virgene, agile). Stress shift in a double
plural does not seem to be a viable possibility.
3. Previous analyses
Harris (1980: 19) proposes that Spanish plurals require that the
autosegmental prosodic template of noun and adjective plurals conform to the templatic ending [[[...] VC].sub.[alpha]] where [alpha] = noun or
adjective, and to a rule that supplies phonological materials for noun
and adjective plurals ("In noun and adjective plurals the segment s
is associated with the rightmost C position in the prosodic
template"). For instance,
(7) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Harris explains that before the application of the morphological
rule, the V position of the prosodic template is associated with the
class marker o, in accordance with the constraint [[[...]
VC].sub.[alpha]], and the C position has no segment associated with it.
The plural rule associates an s with this position. In the case of
consonant final nouns and/or adjectives, a phonological epenthesis rule
inserts e into the empty V of the prosodic template, as in (8).
(8) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Within the context of this proposal, Harris accounts for Dominican
double plurals (e.g. muchacho, muchachose) by means of an additional
prosodic template--[[[...] V C V].sub.[alpha]]. The derivation would
proceed as in (9).
(9) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
This proposal can be seen as a significant improvement over earlier
accounts under the assumption that templates are independently justified
in phonological systems; however, as Harris himself admits, his analysis
also suffers from one important weakness: "the account just
sketched gives no genuine explanation.... Since we simply stipulated the
existence of the alternate template ..." (Harris 1980: 24).
Older analyses (Nunez-Cedeno 1980) propose a series of
language-specific rules to account for [e] in plural formation. In
example (10), (10a) accounts for [e] in standard plural formation ([es]
allomorph) and (10b) for [e] in the double plural; in both cases [e] is
considered epenthetic. (10b) is an optional rule.
(10) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
Sample derivation
/pintura/ plural /papel/ plural /tarea/ plural
/pintura s papel s tarea s plural adjunction
-- papal e s -- (10a)
pintura s e papel e s e tarea se (10b)
[pinturase] [papelese] [tarease]
The proposal in (10) faces difficulties similar to those of Harris
(1980, 1986) due to the stipulative nature of the rules as well as to
their language-specific character. Harris (1980) and Nunez-Cedeno (1980)
also miss an important generalization: that the form of double plural
results from the interaction of Dominican phonology (in particular,
syllabification) with standard plural formation mechanisms common to
most dialects. This will become clear in the context of the current
proposal. (4)
Nunez-Cedeno (2003) applies Harris' (1999)
distributed-morphology account of Spanish plurals to Dominican Spanish.
In his analysis the plain plural for words ending in consonants (class
IIIa in Harris 1999) is /e/ or /es/; all other forms take the
zero-morpheme. The double plurals are [ese] for C-final words and [se]
for V-final ones as shown in (11).
(11) [root] Af [root] Af
morphology
Mujer ese gallina se
phonology
Mu.je.re.se ga.lli.na.se syllabification
This analysis also fails to capture the connection between double
plurals and syllabification mechanisms in Dominican Spanish, as the
presence of [e] after [s] is related to the ill-formedness of coda /s/.
In addition, I argue that there is no need for postulating two separate
underlying allomorphs as Nunez-Cedeno (2003) does.
In sum, extant analyses of double plural formation in Dominican
Spanish suffer from framework-specific problems (e.g. language-specific
nature of rules, stipulatory nature of rules and templates) and more
general ones, such as their inability to show that double plurals result
from the interaction of standard plural formation with the phonology of
Dominican, thus obviating the need for special mechanisms such as
language-specific templates or rules. In this article I propose an OT
analysis that solves the difficulties encountered by previous ones; in
addition, the OT account proposed here provides for a more adequate
understanding of plural formation in Dominican, by showing that double
plurals are not a language-specific oddity. Furthermore, in addition to
offering an adequate formal account that ensures good phonological
output, the current analysis incorporates pragmatic factors, thus
accounting for the selection of the "double plural" versus
standard [e]/zero realization.
4. Analysis
4.1. Basic proposal
I start by presenting the basics of the proposal in pretheoretical
terms (derivationally-sounding terminology--e.g. repair, epenthesis,
deletion--is used out of convenience and should not be read as an
endorsement of a serial analysis of the data).
Attachment of the plural morpheme results in a coda [s] in a
dialect that does not normally allow coda obstruents. Given that the
general repair mechanism for coda obstruents in Dominican is deletion
(usted, ustedes [u.te] [u.te.De] 'you-polite, sg. and pl.'),
plural 's' is realized as zero (from [s]) or [e] (from [es]).
Despite the non-overt realization of a grammatical morpheme in
pluralization, deletion is still the preferred mechanism as the plural
meaning can be recovered either from [e] in consonant-final bases or
through other marks (verb agreement, article allomorph, etc.). A
phonological zero, however, is not always the preferred outcome for
plural [s]. Since focus position is a prominent, strong position,
deletion is no longer the best strategy for the realization of the
plural morpheme on focused NP heads; as a result, coda [s] is avoided
through epenthesis, [se]. Consideration of the pragmatics and semantics
of double plural selection thus reveals the presence of the alternation [o] ~ [s], which in turn serves as evidence in favor of /s/ as the
underlying plural allomorph in Dominican.
(12) Plural allomorph output forms in Dominican:
Standard plural Plural in focus position
[o] [e] [se] [ese]
casa casa casase
mujer mujere mujerese
The examples in (12) demonstrate the existence of this alternation:
as explained above, plural /s/ surfaces as zero in regular pluralization
and as [se] when followed by epenthetic [e] in focused positions. The
plural allomorph is often zero because /s/ will normally be in coda
position and therefore deleted like other coda obstruents; yet when
deletion is no longer acceptable, and epenthesis is preferred, [e]
allows [s] to be syllabified in an onset position and thus to be
realized as [s].
In this analysis, I claim that the plural allomorph is /s/ and that
[-es] can be derived from /s/. Most analyses of pluralization in Spanish
consider plural [e] predictable and thus not underlying. Since the focus
of this article is not regular pluralization, but the so-called
"double plurals" in Dominican, I will not specifically argue
for this particular position, but simply adopt the standard view that
the V in [es] is derived (Saltarelli 1970; Contreras 1977; Harris 1980;
Moyna and Wiltshire 2000) and show how this works in conjunction with
the analysis proposed for "double plurals." (5) I do, however,
agree with recent proposals (Harris 1999) that the motivation for -e is
not "phonological" in a traditional sense, as speakers of
Dominican, as well of other dialects, do in fact produce clusters such
as [rs] as in, for instance, hypercorrect forms (e.g. una flo[rs]
'a flower') (see Harris [1999] for the proposal that -e
epenthesis is morphological). The analysis presented in this article
offers a solution to the problem of the "phonological vs.
morphological" nature of plural epenthesis: plural epenthesis
reveals the emergence of the unmarked (CV syllables) in the OO
phonology. In other words, -e is inserted in plurals to provide a
vocalic terminal element thus replacing a consonant-final syllable with
its preferred vowel final correspondent. This is not the case for the
phonology in general (IO) where faithfulness constraints (DEP-IO)
dominate markedness (*Coda).
An important theoretical implication of the data on "double
plural" realization and the account proposed here has to do with
the role of pragmatic factors, often assumed to be outside the purview of a formal theory of phonology. As Nunez-Cedeno (2003) shows,
"double plural" selection is not optional or random, but it is
governed by informational prominence (focus). Any formal account of the
data that aims at explanatory adequacy needs to capture these facts.
Existing alternatives, such as two underlying plural morphemes (/s/ and
/se/) or double attachment of the same morpheme, are neither descriptive
nor explanatorily adequate as they cannot explain what motivates
selection of one form versus the other. However, it is unclear whether,
in order to account for the facts, a theory of phonology needs to
incorporate pragmatic factors as such or whether it would suffice to
refer to the phonetic and/or phonological features that identify
informationally-prominent positions, thus excluding reference to
pragmatics in formal phonology. Despite much recent work on the topic
(Truckenbrodt 1995; Selkirk 2002 and references therein), the role and
formalization of focus in phonology remains to be understood.
Furthermore, the complexity of the matter places it well beyond the
scope of this article. Therefore, and for the purposes of the analysis
proposed in Section 4.2, I use "focus" as a cover term for
phonetic/ phonological markers of focused NP heads (FOC constraint).
An alternative to the analysis proposed here would be to assume, as
most of the literature does, that there is no underlying /s/ in plain
Dominican plurals and that [se] is inserted in plurals in focus position
(/(e)/ ~ /se/). This account needs to specify an additional morpheme for
focused plurals /se/ (or [s] epenthesis, otherwise nonexistent in the
language) along with nonfocused /e/ for C-final words. On the other
hand, the /s/-plus-epenthesis proposal can account for the facts through
a single plural morpheme /s/ and general mechanisms of syllabification
already existing in the language; it also captures the connection
between [s] and plural and [e] as the default epenthetic vowel in
Spanish (yugoslavo vs. eslavo). Note also that this type of analysis is
at least partly imposed by the limitations of a serial account. A
rule-based account cannot obtain both the plain and double plural
outputs from a unique plural morpheme /s/. In such a model, trying to
derive both [oreha] and [OREHASE] from /oreha + s/ would require a rule
of epenthesis and another of deletion. No matter the order of
application of the rules, it is impossible to obtain both output forms:
(13) UR /oreha + s/ /oreha + s/
deletion [oreha] epenthesis [orehase]
epenthesis -- deletion --
output [oreha] [orehase]
As a result, a rule-based analysis needs to specify a separate
underlying morpheme for double plurals.
No underlying /s/ is also the standard analysis for Dominican coda
's' in nonplural forms like mas /ma/ 'more' (see
also [16] and [17]) (Terrell 1986). Terrell argues that there is no
evidence for an underlying coda /s/, given the lack of alternations and
the fact that [s] does not normally surface in coda position. Terrell
further argues that when [s] does surface, it is in cases of
hypercorrection, often appearing in forms with no /s/, e.g. [fisno] ~
Standard Spanish fino [fino], a fact that would remain unexplained under
a deletion analysis. Although Terrell (1986: 126) includes plurals
amongst the forms for which he postulates no underlying /s/, for
example, /kasa/ 'casa, casas', it is easy to see that the
restructuring of coda /s/ as zero affects only lexical coda /s/
(underived forms) and not plural /s/ (plural /s/ does in fact have
alternations, [o] ~ [se], plain and double plural). In other words, the
plural /s/ account presented here is entirely compatible with the
absence of underlying coda 's'. The apparent duplication in
the analysis is not problematic at all beating in mind the different
nature of [s], lexical in one case, underlyingly-specified plural
allomorph, in the other.
4.2. Formalization: an OT analysis
In this section I show that the analysis presented in Section 4.1
can be expressed in a natural way in an optimality-theoretic framework.
In addition, the OT account has the following advantages: (i)
universality of constraints (vs. language specific rules or templates);
(ii) greater empirical coverage, as it explains the "apparent"
optionality of the so-called "double plurals" and the stress
facts mentioned by Nunez-Cedeno (2003).
4.2.1. Lexical (nonplural) coda obstruents. Coda obstruent deletion
is the result of the domination of *CodaObst over the faithfulness
constraint MAX-IO, as seen in (14b). That epenthesis is not the
preferred strategy indicates that only MAX-IO, not DEP-IO, is dominated
by *CodaObst.
(14) a. *CodaObs: No obstruents in coda position (McCarthy 2002:
106).
MAX-IO: Every segment present in the input must have a
correspondent in the output (Benua 1995; McCarthy 1995).
DEP-IO: Every segment present in the output must have a
correspondent in the input (Benua 1995; McCarthy 1995).
b. *CodaObs, DEP-IO >> MAX-IO
The markedness constraint *CodaObs refers to coda obstruents to
capture the fact that in Dominican different coda consonants behave
differently: thus while obstruents tend to be deleted, sonorants are
faithfully retained (or vocalized in some dialects [Pineros 2003]).
*CodaSon (here referred to as *Coda, see Note 11 and [32]-[34]; see also
McCarthy [2002: 106]) would therefore be dominated by MAX-IO (not shown
in the tableaux).
The tableaux in (15) contains candidate evaluation for coda
obstruent /d/. Candidates (15a) (final epenthesis) and (15b) (coda
obstruent) violate DEP-IO and *CodaObs respectively, both more highly
ranked than MAX-IO. (15c), the candidate with coda obstruent deletion,
is preferred because it only incurs a violation of MAX-IO. In addition,
all candidates in (15) incur a violation of MAX--indicated by the use of
parentheses --due to the deletion of /s/ (alternative candidates
involving /s/ are not considered for presentational reasons).
(15) /usted/ [ute]
* CodaObs DEP-IO MAX-IC
a. utede * ! (*)
b. uted * ! (*)
[??] c. ute (*)
(16) and (17) examine /s/-final forms. They show evaluation of
candidates for two possible input forms for [ga] 'gas', one in which
the input contains a lexical /s/ (16) and one in which it does not (17).
(16) /gas/ [ga] (preliminary)
* CodaObs DEP-IO MAX-IO
a. gase * !
b. gas * !
[??] c. ga
(17) /ga/ [ga] (final)
* Coda0bs DEP-IO MAX-IO
a. gase ** !
b. gas * ! *
[??] c. ga
While both input forms lead to the same correct output, the
preferred input by lexicon optimization is /ga/. Lexicon optimization is
a learning strategy that assists the language learner in setting up an
underlying representation when the data provide no evidence for it
through alternation (Prince and Smolensky 1993). As McCarthy (2002: 77)
puts it, learners "[c]hoose the underling representation that gives
the most harmonic mapping." As (16) and (17) show, the phonetic
output for /gas/ and /ga/ is the same. Nonetheless, /ga/ is selected as
the input since there is a more harmonic mapping for the output [ga]
from /ga/ (no constraint violation marks as shown in [17c]) than from
/gas/ (one violation mark as seen in [16a]).
4.2.2. Realize morpheme and output-to-output constraints.
Pluralization consists of the attachment of the plural allomorph /s/ to
a singular base. A question that arises is that if there is only a
single plural morpheme /s/, how is it that it is realized differently
depending on whether it is in an informationally prominent position,
e.g. [muherese] or not, e.g. [muhere]? In this context the crucial
constraints are two morphemerealization constraints--RM and RM/FOC, (18)
and (19)--regarding the relationship between the plural morpheme and
overt phonological realization. In the OT literature
morpheme-realization constraints have been conceptualized in a narrow
fashion (MORPH-REAL) in which case satisfaction is dependent on whether
a particular morpheme surfaces or not (Samek-Lodovici 1993; McCarthy
2000: 124) or in a more general way (realize morpheme [RM]) in which any
change on the form of a base could be interpreted as the realization of
a morpheme and thus satisfaction of the RM constraint in (18) (Kurisu
2001). In this article, I adopt the RM conceptualization of morpheme
realization due to its broader empirical application. (6)
(18) RM: All morphemes must be realized. A morpheme is realized iff
the outcome has some phonological property which distinguishes it from
the base (Kurisu 2001)
Kurisu (2001) shows that the RM formalization has broad empirical
coverage in that it can account for both concatenative and
nonconcatenative morphology. Under this approach, morpheme realization
in concatenative morphology is a consequence of faithfulness constraints
(and their ranking) on the underlying form of the affix. More
specifically, concatenative morphology is an epiphenomenon of the
ranking RM >> DEP-IO, where an affix is inserted in order to
realize a morpheme. On the other hand, in nonconcatenative morphology
the ranking DEP-IO >> RM >> IDENT. B(ase) (7) captures the
lack of any lexically-specified affix and the preference for realizing
morphemes by means of phonological modifications to the base. Thus,
while Kurisu's model incorporates the possibility of satisfying RM
through base modification (nonconcatenative morphology), it also
accounts for morphological processes in which morphemes can only be
realized through affixation (not through modification of the base). This
distinction is important for the current analysis dealing with
concatenative morphology.
In addition to a general RM constraint, I propose a second RM
constraint--RM/FOC (19)--that requires that morphemes be realized
overtly in prosodically emphatic, prominent positions, such as a focus.
RM/FOC accounts for plural realization (double plurals) in focus
position in Dominican (see [28]-[30] and [34]).
(19) RM/FOC: All morphemes must be realized overtly in focus
position (intonationally or otherwise strong position).
RM/FOC can be easily motivated on universal grounds as prominence
is intimately connected with explicit marking (e.g. intonation,
duration, stress, etc.). In this respect it is worth noting that
intonationally prominent positions favor epenthesis and not deletion
(cf. Martinez-Gil [1997], who shows that optional epenthesis in Galician
is favored in metrical heads of intonational phrases). Additional
justification can be found if one views this constraint as part of a
universal hierarchy that relates linguistic prominence and overt
phonological expression (and/or faithfulness). It is well-known that
within the phonological realm, some positions (onset, metrical heads,
etc.) are more resistant to epenthesis/deletion than others (coda,
nonheads of metrical feet). Within an OT framework, Beckman (1997)
proposes a universal hierarchy of constraints (positional faithfulness)
in which faithfulness constraints on strong positions (e.g. faith/onset)
are ranked higher than faithfulness to weak positions (faith/ coda).
Similarly, in the realm of morphophonology, the requirement of morpheme
realization can be seen as the result of faithfulness constraints on
morphemes being ranked higher than faithfulness to prosodic positions
which are not morphemes (e.g. faith/morpheme >> faith/prosodic
position). Faithfulness constraints affecting morphemes which are also
in focus position (i.e. intonationally prominent, marked by different
pitch or duration) would occupy an even higher place in the hierarchy. A
prominence hierarchy in which different types of linguistic prominence
are related to phonological expression deserves much more detailed study
than permitted by the confines of the current study, and therefore (20)
is included only as an illustration of what this type of hierarchy might
look like. For the purposes of this article, a general constraint
relating overt expression (thus no deletion) with morphemes in strong
positions such as focus (19) (RM/FOC) will suffice.
(20) Faithfulness to strong, prominent positions:
Faith/morpheme/Foc >> Faith/morpheme
In addition to the ranking IDENT. B(ase), RM >> DEP-IO,
proper of concatenative morphology, an adequate account of Spanish
plural formation needs to refer to OO faithfulness constraints (Benua
1995; McCarthy 1995). As is well-known, the domain of application of
pluralization in Spanish is the morphological word, as demonstrated by
the fact that the plural /s/ is attached after all other derivational
and inflectional morphemes, including terminal elements, -a, -o, and -e
(cas-a-s 'house[s]', camin-o-s 'path[s]',
chocolat-e-s, 'chocolate[s]'). That the plural morpheme is
attached to the morphological word means that OO faithfulness
constraints, which evaluate correspondence relations between two output
forms, become relevant in the evaluation of plural outputs. For the sake
of convenience, I use the abbreviations DEP-OO and MAX-OO to refer to
the OO faithfulness constraints specific to pluralization, that is,
DEPSgPl and MAX-SgPl. Spanish plural forms contain by definition a
violation of DEP-IO with respect to the input of the singular on account
of a segmentally specified plural morpheme /s/ (concatenative
morphology) [given their definitional nature and for reasons of
presentation, these violations of DEP-IO in the plural forms will not be
the focus of attention and will not be shown in the tableaux]. In
addition and more importantly, under OO correspondence, [s] and [e] in
the plural outputs violate DEPOO (see [26a] and [26b]), since [s] and
[e] are not present in the output of the singular. [e] does not incur a
DEP-IO violation given that the relevant correspondence relation is
output to output (the plural is formed on the output of the singular).
DEP-IO is vacuously satisfied. Domination of *Coda over DEP-OO makes
plural epenthesis possible, while word-final epenthesis is ruled out by
DEP-IO >> *Coda (see Section 4.2.4).
Finally, morphological epenthesis (/s/, the DEP-IO violations
incurred to satisfy RM) introduces a new set of IO correspondence
relations--[IO.sub.Morpheme]. These are the correspondence relations
established between the underlyingly specified form of the morpheme /s/
and its output correspondent, usually zero in Dominican Spanish. One way
of integrating this into the constraints and constraint-ranking that is
formally appropriate, but also convenient on presentational grounds, is
to define RM in concatenative morphology as consisting of two conjoined constraints RM & [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme]. [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme]
requires that an underlyingly specified morpheme have an output
correspondent ([MAX-IO.sub.Morpheme]) and vice versa ([DEP-IO.sub.Morpheme]) (see [21]). It also requires preservation of the
featural specification of the input ([IDENT-IO.sub.Morpheme]). For RM to
be satisfied, [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme] also must be satisfied; in other
words, RM must be satisfied through the surface correspondent of the
underlying-specified morpheme, and not through the violation of IDENT.
B(ase) or LINEARITY (constraint against metathesis). For instance, a
plural like [muhere] (singular [muher]), despite the change to the base,
does not satisfy RM in a concatenative language like Spanish (IDENT.
B(ase), RM >> DEP-IO) because the underlyingly specified plural
morpheme /s/ is not realized, violating [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme]
([MAX-IO.sub.Morpheme] or [IDENT-IO.sub.Morpheme], for a candidate in
which the plural morpheme is in correspondence with [e]). Alternatively,
RM satisfaction could be formalized as the combined effect of separate,
nonconflicting RM and [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme], constraints.
(21) [MAX-IO.sub.Morpheme]: A morpheme present in the input must
have a correspondent in the output (adapted from Benua 1995; McCarthy
1995).
[DEP-IO.sub.Morpheme]: A morpheme present in the output must have a
correspondent in the input (adapted from Benua 1995; McCarthy 1995).
[IDENT-IO.sub.Morpheme]: The featural specification of an input
morpheme must be preserved in the output (adapted from Benua 1995;
McCarthy 1995).
Although unranked with respect to RM, [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme],
constraints exhibit the same ranking as RM with regard to other
constraints.
(22) ... >> RM, [MAX-IO.sub.Morpheme], [DEP-IO.sub.Morpheme],
[IDENT-IO.sub.Morpheme] >>...
I will use the cover term RM to refer to satisfaction of RM and
[Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme], regardless of whether this is formalized as
conjoined constraints or unranked separate constraints. Under the
separate constraint account a candidate could violate RM on the basis of
a violation of [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme] and not necessarily of RM. Under
the conjoined account a violation of [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme] means a
violation of RM. The distinction has no bearing upon the data being
considered here. Both types of outcomes will be represented in the
tableaux as RM violations.
4.2.3. Pluralization of V-final words. The system of correspondence
relations proposed for regular V-final plurals is shown schematically in
(23).
(23) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
(23) shows correspondence relations for [oreha], the optimal
candidate for /oreha/ + /s/ as seen in (26c). *CodaObs must dominate RM
because the plural morpheme surfaces as zero in (23) and (26c). Thus
(26c) incurs a violation of RM (through [Max-IO.sub.Morpheme]). (26b)
violates DEP-OO, since [s] is not present in the output of the singular,
as well as the more highly ranked markedness constraint *CodaObs.
Therefore, (26b) (one *CodaObs mark) loses to (26c) (one RM violation).
An additional constraint, Align Plural in (24), accounts for the
ill-formedness of a candidate with epenthesis: Align Plural must
dominate RM, since a violation of RM is preferred to the misaligned candidate orehase (26a). Align plural is motivated by the fact that the
plural morpheme is attached after all other derivational and
inflectional morphemes. The data examined so far offer no evidence to
indicate that *CodaObs and Align Plural are crucially ranked with
respect to each other. However, the "double plural" data (e.g.
muherese) will demonstrate that *CodaObs dominates Align Plural (see
Section 4.2.4).
(24) Align (Pl, R, Wd, R) (Align Plural): the right edge of the
plural morpheme must be aligned with the right edge of the word.
(25) *CodaObs >> Align Plural >> RM
(26) /oreha + s/
& [oreha] [oreha] (8)
* CodaObs Align Plural RM
a. orehase * !
b. orehas * !
[??] c. oreha * (MAX-
[IO.sub.Morpheme]
DEP-OO
a. orehase * (s) * (e)
b. orehas * (s)
[??] c. oreha
An additional plural candidate [orehae] would violate DEP-OO in
addition to RM. The correspondence relations obtaining for the losing
candidates (26a) and (26b) are shown schematically in (27a) and (27b).
(27a) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
(27b) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
For "double plurals," such as OREJASE (CAPS = focused
element), the constraint RM/FOC in (19) becomes relevant. Recall that
RM/FOC requires that morphemes be realized overtly in prosodically
emphatic, prominent positions, such as a focus. (9)
RM/FOC dominates Align Plural because (28a) OREHASE is preferred to
OREHA (28c). As shown in (25) in *Coda Obs dominates Align Plural;
furthermore this is shown by the selection of (28a) over (28b). Thus,
domination of RM/FOC over Align Plural results in Align Plural and
DEP-OO violations (a.k.a. epenthesis), even though epenthesis is not the
strategy normally employed in Dominican to avoid coda obstruents. Note
that nonplural codas are still affected by deletion in emphatic
positions, thus the relevant constraint must refer to the morpheme.
(28) /OREHA + s/ [OREHASE] &
[oreha]
RM/FOC * CodaObs Align RM DEP-
Plural OO
[??] a. OREHASE * * (s) * (e)
b. OREHAS * ! * (s)
c. OREHA * ! *
The tableaux in (29) shows evaluation for a form with lexical coda [s]
(nonplural [s]) in focus position; (30) shows the plural of this same
form.
(29) /GA/ &[GA] (singular)
RM/FOC * CodaObs DEP-IO MAX-IO
a. GASE * (s) * (e)!
b. GAS * ! * (s)
[??] c. GA
(30) /GA + s/ [GASE] &[GA]
Align
RM/FOC * CodaObs Plural RM DEP-00
[??] a. GASE * * (s) * (e)
b. GAS * ! * (s)
c. GA * ! *
In (29), given that the final [s] is not part of the plural
morpheme, RM/ FOC is vacuously satisfied. Whereas (29a) and (29b)
violate DEP-IO and *CodaObs, (29c) incurs no marks and is therefore the
winner. In (30), however, pluralization makes RM/FOC relevant, ruling
out (30c). (30a) is selected as the output because it only violates
Align Plural (vs. a *CodaObs violation for [30b]).
4.2.4. C-final bases. The last set of data in need of consideration
consists of C-final bases that end in consonants other than /s/. As
explained above, I claim, in agreement with most of the literature, that
in nonfocus positions the plural morpheme is /s/; /s/ has two
allomorphs, [s] -[es] in standard varieties, and [o] [e] in Dominican.
Thus in regular (nonfocus) plurals, (3lc) [muhere] is preferred over
(31a) [muherse] due to the high ranking of Align Plural and over
[muheres] (31b) because (31b) violates *CodaObs. (31c) violates RM
through a violation of [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme] ([Max-IO.sub.Morpheme] or
[IDENT.sub.Morpheme] if [e] is in correspondence with /s/).
(31) /muher + s/ [muhere] & [muher]
* CodaObs Align Plural RM
a. muherse * !
b. muheres * !
[??] c. muhere * (Faith-
[IO.sub.Morpheme])
DEP-OO
a. muherse * (s) * (e)
b. muheres * (s) * (e)
[??] c. muhere * (e)
Yet (31) cannot be complete as inclusion of the additional candidate
[muher] produces incorrect results, as seen in (32).
(32) /muher + s/ [muhere] (preliminary) (10)
* CodaObs Align Plural RM DEP-OO
a. muherse * ! * (s) * (e)
b. muheres * ! * (s) * (e)
c. muhere * * (e)
[??] d. muher *
However, one crucial constraint is missing in (32). Although a
high-ranked constraint on coda obstruents (*CodaObs) is necessary to
account for deletion of obstruents, a *Coda constraint (= *CodaSonorant)
is still necessary, as coda sonorants are retained in the dialect under
consideration. (11) *Coda dominates DEP-OO. Thus while sonorant codas
are not repaired by deletion or epenthesis in the singular (muher *muhe
*muhere 'woman'), indicating that DEP-IO, MAX-IO >>
*Coda, this is not the case for plural forms, where the constraints are
*Coda >> DEP-OO. (33c) and (33d) are therefore tied with respect
to RM, passing the decision onto *Coda, which correctly chooses (30c)
[muhere]. *Coda is not crucially ranked with respect to RM.
(33) /muher + s/ [muhere] (final) & [muher]
* CodaObs Align Plural RM * Coda DEP-00
[??] c. muhere * *
d. muher * * !
This account reveals the relevance of OO constraints in plural
formation in Spanish and also serves to explain plural epenthesis, seen
now as the result of favoring CV structure (or in morphological terms,
"words are V-final in Spanish") in a morphologically derived,
word-level context (output-to-output). In other words, epenthesis of [e]
in the plural reflects the emergence of the unmarked (McCarthy and
Prince 1994): *Coda effects are seen in the OO phonology of the plural,
but not in the language as a whole, as an effect of the ranking DEP-IO
>> *Coda >> DEP-OO. The issue of the morphological versus
phonological status of plural epenthesis in Spanish appears now much
clearer. While plural epenthesis may still be considered phonological,
it does not belong to the input-to-output phonology, but to the OO
phonology. This accounts for plural epenthesis after codas that would be
well-formed in nonplural outputs. At the same time, the connection with
the morphological structure of Spanish becomes obvious as the morphology
reveals a bias toward CV structure evidenced by the preference for
vowel-final words (word markers).
Evaluation for focused forms proceeds as in (34). Candidates (34c)
and (34d) are eliminated on account of RM/FOC violations, while (34b)
violates the highly ranked *CodaObs. Despite the ties on Align Plural
and DEP-IO, (34e), MUHERESE, is better than (34b), MUHERES, because of
the additional *Coda violation incurred by the latter. (34e) is thus
selected as the optimal candidate. (12) An additional candidate MUHESE
(not shown in the tableaux) is also ruled out, indicating that the
antideletion constraint MAX-OO dominates *Coda.
(34) /MUHER + S/ [MUHERESE] & [muher]
RM/ * CodaObs Align RM * Coda DEP
FOC Plural -OO
a. * * ! * (s)
MUHERSE * (e)
b. * ! * * (s)
MUHERES * (e)
c. MUHERE * ! * * (e)
d. MUHER * ! * *
[??] e. * * (s)
MUHERESE * (e)
* (e)
4.2.5. C-final paroxytones. An OO analysis like the one proposed
here finds further support in that it explains the ill-formedness of
"double plurals" with C-final paroxytones, e.g. agil, agile,
?agilese, 'quick'; util, utile, ?utilese 'useful'
(Nunez-Cedeno 2003). The data reveal that double plurals in which the
output would have to bear stress past the antepenultimate syllable
(notice that [se] results in one additional syllable) are not possible.
An OO account can explain this through the domination over RM/FOC of the
stress constraint(s) that penalize the placement of stress to the left
of the antepenultimate syllable.
(35) 3 [sigma] win: Stress cannot be placed to the left of the
antepenultimate syllable (cover term for stress alignment constraints)
(Harris 1983). [For reasons of simplicity, the relevant stress
constraints are referred to by the cover term three-syllable window (3
[sigma] win.)] Stress-OO: The head of a foot in the output must be
preserved in the output strings it stands in correspondence with.
3 [sigma] win and Stress-OO must both dominate RM/FOC given that a
candidate without plural realization (37c) (RM/FOC violation) is
preferred to one with stress past the antepenultimate (37a) or with
stress shift (37b). (13)
(36) 3 [sigma] win, Stress-OO >> RM/FOC
(37) /ahil + s/ [aHILE] & [ahil]
3 [sigma] win. Stress-OO RM/FOC
a. aHILESE * !
b. AHI'LESE * !
[??] c. aHILE *
Candidate evaluation in (37) demonstrates that (37c) is the optimal
candidate because (37a) and (37b) incur violations of more highly ranked
constraints--3 [sigma] win. and Stress-OO.
To conclude, it is important to emphasize the analysis of the
stress data as it constitutes a strong argument in favor of the OO
approach to Dominican plurals and to pluralization in Spanish in
general. Furthermore, it shows that prosodic constraints can outrank morphological constraints such as RM/FOC.
5. Conclusions
This article presents an analysis of the so-called "double
plurals" of Dominican Spanish. An important contribution of the
proposal put forth here is the insight that there are no "double
plurals" per se in Dominican Spanish: the apparently redundant
attachment of the plural morpheme is only the result of general
restrictions on coda obstruents in combination with the need for overt
realization of morphemes in prominent positions. An optimality-theoretic
account in which an entire system of universal constraints must, in
principle, be considered is essential in reaching this conclusion.
Furthermore, that constraints are universal and always present allows
for the possibility of avoiding coda obstruents through deletion as well
as epenthesis. This is something which could not be easily derived in a
serial model in a nonstipulatory way, but which falls out naturally from
the constraints and constraint ranking in OT. In the case at hand,
deletion of coda obstruents is usually preferred due to the ranking
*CodaObst >> MAX-IO. However, when an output with a MAX violation
is not acceptable because it would also violate a more highly ranked
constraint (i.e. RM/FOC), a candidate with epenthesis (DEP-OO) violation
and no RM/FOC marks is preferred. That plural formation is an OO process
and is also crucial to account for epenthesis in regular and double
plurals.
The same analysis captures the connection between obstruent coda
deletion, plural /s/, and double plurals without having to resort to
separate rules or templates. Although the low-ranking of MAX and RM
obscure the effects of DEP-IO (/s/ deletion rather than epenthesis in
word-final position in singulars and plain plurals), such effects can be
seen in double plurals when a more highly ranked constraint, RM/FOC,
rules out a candidate with a MAX violation (deletion). In other words,
word-final epenthesis is resorted to when other alternatives (deletion)
are no longer optimal, thus revealing the actual presence of a process
which may have appeared absent in word-final position (since aspiration
and deletion are usually preferred). (14)
An additional advantage of the current proposal is the elimination
of a separate, additional underlying representation/morpheme (also
templates) postulated for the sole purpose of explaining the double
plural. This move was required due to the nature of rules and rule
ordering in a serial approach.
Finally, the present account of Dominican double plurals also
advances some new proposals that are of relevance for phonological
theory in general and for Spanish phonology. In the first place, the
analysis presented here argues for the extension of a positional
faithfulness hierarchy (Beckman 1997) beyond the syllable and the foot.
In other words, I argue that a universal hierarchy that relates
linguistic prominence and overt phonological expression (i.e.
faithfulness) needs to refer to additional prominent positions/units,
such as morphemes, intonational phrase heads, etc. This introduces the
issue of the nature of linguistic prominence and how that relates to a
theory of phonology.
Secondly, the analysis of Dominican plurals sheds new light into
the nature of pluralization and the so-called "plural
epenthesis" in Spanish. Epenthesis is allowed in the context of
pluralization because it is no longer a matter of IO faithfulness, but
of OO faithfulness. Thus, "plural epenthesis" is not simply a
matter of phonotactics as many supposedly ill-formed clusters can be
pronounced in nonplurals, but also of improving word-level
well-formedness (emergence of the unmarked in OO phonology). In
addition, this analysis brings out the different nature of syllable
repair mechanisms in ill-formed codas and coda clusters (final coda
deletion) and in the supposedly ill-formed clusters resulting from
morpheme concatenation in pluralization (epenthesis), and can therefore
explain the presence of deletion in one case, but epenthesis in the
other. This insight is also valid for analysis of pluralization in other
dialects of Spanish for which the nature (epenthetic vs. underlying) of
the mid-vowel preceding the plural allomorph has been a matter of debate
for decades. A more detailed analysis of standard Spanish pluralization
(non-aspirating varieties) as an OO process is therefore warranted
(Colina forthcoming).
Received 22 April 2003
Revised version received 8 June 2004
References
Beckman, Jill (1997). Positional faithfulness. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Massachusetts at Amherst.
Benua, Laura (1995). Identity effects in morphological truncation.
In University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18:
Papers in Optimality Theory, Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh, and Suzanne
Urbanczyk (eds.), 77-136. Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistics Student
Association.
Colina, Sonia (2003). The status of word-final [e] in Spanish.
Southwest Journal of Linguistics 22, 87-108.
--(forthcoming). Output-to-output correspondence and the emergence
of the unmarked in Spanish plural formation. In New Analyses in Romance
Linguistics. Selected Papers from the 35th LSRL, Jean-Pierre Montreuil
(ed.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Contreras, Heles (1977). Spanish epenthesis and stress. Working
Papers in Linguistics 3, 9-33.
Foley, J. (1967). Spanish plural formation. Language 43, 486-493.
Harris, James W. (1980). Nonconcatenative morphology and Spanish
plurals. Journal of Linguistic Research 1, 15-31.
--(1983). Syllable Structure and Stress in Spanish. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
--(1986). El modelo multidimensional de la fonologia y la
dialectologia caribena. In Estudios sobre la fonologia del espanol del
Caribe, Rafael A. Nunez-Cedeno, Paez Urdaneta, and Jorge M. Guitart
(eds.), 41-51. Caracas: Ediciones La Casa de Bello.
--(1999). Nasal depalatalization no, morphological wellformedness
si; the structure of Spanish word classes. MIT Working Papers in
Linguistics 33, 47-82.
Jimenez Sabater, Max A. (1975). Mas datos sobre el espanol de la
Republica Dominicana. Santo Domingo: Ediciones Intec.
Kurisu, Kazutaka (2001). The phonology of morpheme realization.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.
[ROA 490, http://roa.rutgers.edu/].
Martinez-Gil, Fernando (1997). Word-final epenthesis in Galician.
In Issues in the Phonology and Morphology of the Major Iberian
Languages, Fernando Martinez-Gil and Alfonso Morales-Front (eds.),
235-267. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
McCarthy, John (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In
University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18: Papers
in Optimality Theory, Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh, and Suzanne Urbanczyk
(eds.), 249-384. Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistics Student Association.
--(2000). A Thematic Guide to Optimality Theory. Cambridge and New
York: Cambridge University Press.
--(2002). A Thematic Guide to Optimality Theory. Cambridge and New
York: Cambridge University Press.
--; and Prince, Alan (1994). The emergence of the unmarked. In
Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society 24, Merce Gonzalez
(ed.), 333-379. Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistics Student Association.
Morin, Regina (1999). Spanish substantives: How many classes? In
Advances in Hispanic Linguistics, Javier Gutierrez-Rexach and Fernando
Martinez-Gil (eds.), 214-230. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Moyna, Irene; and Wiltshire, Caroline (2000). Spanish plurals: Why
[s] isn't always optimal. In Hispanic Linguistics at the Turn of
the Millenium. Papers from the 3rd Hispanic Linguistics Symposium,
Hector Campos, Elena Herburger, Alfonso Morales-Front, and Thomas J.
Walsh (eds.), 31-48. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Nunez-Cedeno, Rafael A. (1980). La fonologia moderna y el espanol
de Santo Domingo. Santo Domingo: Taller.
--(2003). Double plurals in Dominican: a morpho-pragmatic account.
In Theory, Practice, and Acquisition: Papers from the 6th Hispanic
Linguistics Symposium and the 5th Conference on the Acquisition of
Spanish, Paula Kempchinsky and Carlos-Eduardo Pineros (eds.), 68-82.
Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Pineros, Carlos-Eduardo (2003). An alignment-based account of coda
effects in a Caribbean Spanish dialect. [ROA 531,
http://roa.rutgers.edu].
Prince, Alan; and Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory:
Constraint Integration in Generative Grammar. Report no. RuCCS-TR-2. New
Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science.
Roca, Iggy (1996). Phonology-morphology interface in Spanish plural
formation: an optimality analysis. In Interfaces in Phonology, Ursula
Kleinhenz (ed.), 210-230. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.
Saltarelli, Mario (1970). Spanish plural formation: apocope or
epenthesis. Language 46, 89-96.
Samek-Lodovici, Vieri (1993). A unified analysis of crosslinguistic
morphological gemination. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rutgers
University.
Selkirk, Elizabeth (2002). Contrastive FOCUS vs. presentational
focus: prosodic evidence form right-node raising in English. Paper
presented at the Speech Prosody Conference, Aix-en-Provence.
[http://www.lpl.univ-aix.fr/sp2002/pdf/selkirk.pdf].
Terrell, Tracy D. (1986). La desparicion de /s/ posnuclear a nivel
lexico en el habla dominicana. In Estudios sobre la fonologia del
espanol del Caribe, Rafael A. Nunez-Cedeno, Paez Urdaneta, and Jorge M.
Guitart (eds.), 117-134. Caracas: Ediciones La Casa de Bello.
Truckenbrodt, Hubert (1995). Phonological phrases: their relation
to syntax, focus, and prominence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
MIT.
Notes
* I would like to thank Rafael Nunez-Cedeno for making available to
me a prepublication copy of his 2003 article. My gratitude also goes to
three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions.
Any remaining errors are my own. Correspondence address: Dept. of
Languages and Literatures, Arizona State University, Main Campus, P.O.
Box 870202, Tempe, AZ 85287-0202, USA. E-mail: scolina@asu.edu.
(1.) I purposely avoid mention of the underlying representation(s)
of the plural morpheme at this point, referring to the plural allomorphs
in orthographic and phonetic form only. The issue of the underlying
representation(s) will be discussed later in the analysis.
(2.) The data in (1)-(4) are from Nunez-Cedeno (2003).
(3.) The facts concerning consonants in Dominican are more
complicated. Obstruents may some times vocalize (instead of deleting).
While sonorants are usually retained, liquid sonorants often vocalize,
and nasals assimilate to a following consonant or velarize. The
interested reader is referred to Pineros (2003) for a more detailed
analysis of the coda facts. For the purposes of this article, I simply
refer to the general asymmetry between obstruents and sonorants:
"whereas the former may delete regularly, the latter are normally
preserved" (Pineros 2003: 27).
(4.) An alternative analysis in which the plural morpheme is
attached twice /s - s/ also misses this generalization.
(5.) That the issue remains unresolved, however, is evidenced by
the deletion proposal of Roca (1996); an earlier deletion proposal can
be found in Foley (1967). One of Roca's arguments against
epenthesis has to do with the reliance of the epenthesis analysis on
lexical epenthesis ("which borders on logical inconsistency, since
epenthesis is by its very nature non-lexical"). While I agree that
word-final -e is not predictable in Spanish and that as a result lexical
epenthesis is not justified, I also believe that this in itself does not
preclude an epenthesis analysis. In any case, these issues, including a
more detailed response to Roca (1996), deserve separate treatment,
especially in light of an OO approach to the plural as the one proposed
here.
(6.) The Dominican data have the potential for providing insights
on how the RM conceptualization of morpheme realization would work for
concatenative morphology (Kurisu 2001 focuses on nonconcatenative and
subtractive morphology).
(7.) Faithfulness constraint that refers to nonsegments in the
base.
(8.) In (26) and the tableaus that follow, '&'
indicates the output of the singular. The output of the plural is
indicated in brackets without '&'.
(9.) The observations made with regard to RM and conjoined or
separate [Faith-IO.sub.Morpheme] constraints apply to RM/FOC as well.
(10.) [??] indicates a candidate incorrectly selected as optimal.
(11.) Pineros (2003) examines the Northern Rustic dialect of
Dominican Spanish in which sonorants are instead vocalized and
incorporated into the nucleus in order to avoid a *Coda sonorant
violation. Under an alignment formulation of coda constraints and a
consonant alignment hierarchy (ALIGN-L (stop, [sigma]) >> ALIGN-L
(fricative, [sigma]) >> ALIGN-L (nasal, [sigma]) >> ALIGN-L
(liquid, [sigma])), deletion of obstruents only is the result of ranking
MAX(seg) below ALIGN-L (stop,[sigma]), ALIGN-L (fricative,[sigma])
(*Coda Obs) and above ALIGN-L (nasal,[sigma]), ALIGN-L (liquid,[sigma]).
Vocalization of sonorants is obtained through the domination of ALIGN-L
(nasal,[sigma]), ALIGN-L (liquid,[sigma]) over the relevant IDENT
constraints. Dialects without vocalization rank the IDENT constraints
over ALIGN-L (nasal, [sigma]), ALIGN-L (liquid, [sigma]). In the current
analysis, *CodaObs comprises Pinero's ALIGN-L (stop,[sigma]) and
ALIGN-L (fricative,[sigma]) and *Coda is the equivalent of ALIGN-L
(nasal, [sigma]) ALIGN-L (liquid, [sigma]).
(12.) Notice that (34e) incurs three DEP-OO violations because of
two epenthetic [e].
(13.) An anonymous reviewer mentions the singular/plural pair
regimen, regimenes as evidence against an identity account. However,
this plural could not be found in any of the standard sources for
Dominican. Given the Greek etymology of the word, regimenes may be a
lexicalized form, possibly even entirely absent from the lexicon of the
speakers of this dialect of Dominican (generally uneducated).
Furthermore, informal data collected by the author of this article
suggest that regimenes occurs in child language in the standard
dialects. In any case, plural forms of standard dialects (regimenes) per
se do not constitute evidence against the current analysis of
nonstandard Dominican dialects. Additional data need to be obtained for
Dominican.
(14.) Notice also that most cases of phonological epenthesis in
Spanish are in word-initial position. For recent discussion on other
types of epenthesis in Spanish, see Morin (1999) and Colina (2003).
SONIA COLINA
Arizona State University