Towards a typology of stop assibilation *.
Hall, T.A. ; Hamann, Silke
Abstract
In this article we propose that there are two universal properties
for phonological stop assibilations, namely (i) assibilations eannot be
triggered by /i/ unless they are also triggered by /j/, and (ii) voiced
stops cannot undergo assibilations unless voiceless ones do. The article
presents typological evidence from assibilations in over 30 languages
supporting both (i) and (ii). It is argued that assibilations are to be
captured in the Optimality Theoretic framework by ranking markedness
constraints grounded in perception that penalize sequences like [ti]
ahead of a faithfulness constraint that militates against the change
from /t/ to some sibilant sound. The occurring language types predicted
by (i) and (ii) will be shown to involve permutations of the rankings
between several different markedness constraints and the one
faithfulness constraint. The article demonstrates that there exist
several logically possible assibilation types that are ruled out because
they would involve illicit rankings.
1. Introduction
This article examines stop assibilations--defined here as processes
that convert a (coronal) stop to a sibilant affricate or fricative before high vocoids, e.g., /t/ is realized as [ts] or [s] before /i/. We
propose two properties for assibilation tules that we claim are
universal, namely (i) assibilations cannot be triggered by /i/ unless
they are also triggered by /j/, and (ii) voiced stops cannot undergo
assibilations unless voiceless ones do. The descriptive goal of this
article is to test these two claims by examining assibilation processes
in a large number of typologically diverse languages. Theoretically we
propose that assibilations are to be captured in the Optimality
Theoretic framework (henceforth OT; Prince and Smolensky 1993) by
ranking phonetically grounded markedness constraints penalizing
sequences like [ti] ahead of a faithfulness constraint that militates
against the change from /t/ to some sibilant sound. The occurring
language types predicted by the two universal properties for
assibilations referred to above will be shown to involve permutations of
the rankings between several different markedness constraints and the
one faithfulness constraint. A major claim of the present article is
that there exist several logically possible assibilation types that can
all be ruled out because they would involve illicit rankings.
The present treatment is important for several reasons. First, we
provide additional evidence that phonological assibilations can only be
adequately explained by appealing to phonetics (see also Clements 1999;
Kim 2001). Our study supplements the afore-mentioned studies, since
neither linguist considers the properties in (i) and (ii). Second, we
argue that the markedness constraints that trigger assibilations are
based in perception and that they are therefore not speaker-driven but
listener-driven. In this respect our treatment differs significantly
from traditional markedness constraints in OT, which are typically based
in articulation. Third, we show how our analysis of assibilations is
superior to the one proposed by Kirchner (1998), who attempts to capture
this process with a fortition constraint. Finally, our study shows how
the OT framework can capture occurring vs. nonoccurring rule types by
appealing to a universal constraint hierarchy among markedness
constraints whose inherent ranking derives from phonetics (see also
Boersma 1998; Hamann 2003, who propose similar hierarchies).
The article is structured as follows. In Section 2 we discuss stop
assibilations from the phonetic perspective and show that these
processes are characterized by several general properties (based on the
findings of Clements 1999 and Kim 2001). In Section 3 we discuss the two
universal properties for assibilations referred to above and posit a
typology of six language types that we show are attested in a number of
languages. By contrast, there are several logically possible
assibilation types that will be shown not to be attested. In Section 4
we posit an OT analysis of the typological generalizations presented in
Section 3 that accounts for the six occurring assibilation types while
simultaneously ruling out the five non-occurring ones. Section 5
concludes.
2. Stop assibilations
In this section we define what we mean by stop assibilation and
then present several universal properties for such processes (discussed
by Clements 1999 and Kim 2001). (1)
Stop assibilations (or assibilations for short) are defined here as
processes whereby stops become sibilant affricates or sibilant
fricatives before high vocoids. Three examples of such rules have been
presented in (1). (2)
(1) Three examples of assibilation rules:
a. t [right arrow] s / i Finnish (Kiparsky 1973) spirantization
b. t [right arrow] ts /--i Korean (Kim 2001) affrication
[t.sup.h] [right arrow]
[ts.sup.] / --i
c. t [right arrow] West Futuna-Aniwa posteriorization
t[integral] /-- i (Dougherty 1983)
We classify the three assibilation processes in (1) according to their output; thus, we call rules like the ones in (1a)-(1c)
'spirantizations', 'affrications' and
'posteriorizations' respectively. Although we are primarily
interested in affrications and spirantizations (because these processes
are not as well studied as posteriorizations) we include
posteriorizations in the typology we posit below for three reasons.
First, many languages have processes that have as the output either [ts]
or [t[??]]. Second, posteriorizations seem to obey the same kinds of
generalizations as affrications and spirantizations, namely the two
universal properties referred to in Section 1. And third, the three
processes arguably have the same function of avoiding surface sequences
like [ti].
Although processes like the ones in (1) can also affect a velar
stop (e.g., in Late Latin/k g/surfaced as [ts dz] before/j/; Pope 1952)
and in some rare languages a labial (e.g., in Lahu labial stops and
nasals are affricated before /u/; Matisoff 1982: 3), we restrict our
typology in Section 3 and the analysis in Section 4 to assibilations
that have a coronal stop as the input segment, in particular the input
is dental or alveolar, i.e., [+coronal, +anterior] in terms of features.
Assibilations like the ones in (1) can either be lexical or
postlexical rules. For example, in Korean (see [lb]) assibilation is
lexical because it is restricted to applying within a derived
environment and does not affect tautomorphemic /ti/, /[t.sup.h]i/
sequences. In Quebec French (Cedergren et al. 1991; Kim 2001) the
assibilation rule is postlexical because it applies across the board,
both within and across words. Since the properties we discuss below hold
for postlexical and lexical assibilations we do not see the need to
distinguish between the two rule domains. On similar lines we discuss
both synchronic rules of assibilation as well as diachronic ones because
both types of processes display the properties we discuss below
(although see Note 7 below).
The term 'assibilation' is used here in a very narrow
sense since we restrict our discussion below to processes like the ones
in (1), which share the following three properties (based on the
findings of Foley 1973, 1977; Clements 1999; Kim 2001):
(2) Three properties of stop assibilations:
a. the trigger is some subset of the high front vocoids (i.e., /i
j/)
b. the output is a sibilant (either an affricate or a fricative)
c. the trigger is to the right of the target
Kim (2001) and Clements (1999) offer a phonetic explanation for the
properties of stop assibilation in (2a)-(2c). The creation of sibilants
from stops has its phonetic origin in the brief period of turbulence (or
'friction phase') that occurs at the release of a stop into a
following high vocoid. Thus, Clements (1999) and Kim (2001) observe that
the friction phase that occurs in some languages following the release
of an alveolar stop into a high front vocoid is significantly greater
than the friction phase of the same stop that is released into a nonhigh
and/or nonfront vocoid (see also Ohala 1983). This phonetic explanation
is captured directly in the markedness constraints we propose in Section
4.
In (3) and (4) we have listed processes that are not in accordance
with our narrow definition of assibilation as they violate one or more
of the properties in (2):
(3) Examples of changes excluded by our definition of
assibilation:
a. t [right arrow] s Woleaian (Tawerilmang and Sohn
/--i, u, e, o 1984: 184)
b. t [right arrow] Tumpisa Shoshone (Dayley 1989:
[theta] / i-- 407)
c. t d [right arrow] t Pima Bajo (Fernandez 1996: 4)
[integral] [d.sup.3] / i--
t [right arrow] t[integral] Apalai (Koehn and Koehn 1986:
/ i-- 120)
t [right arrow] t[integral] Basque (Hualde 1991: 108-109)
/ i--
(4) a. t [right arrow] ts / #-- Old High German (Penzl 1972)
b. t [right arrow] ts / Danish (Basboll and Wagner
[sigma] [-- 1985: 67)
c. t d [right arrow] ts dz / Polish (Rubach 1994)
--s z ts dz s z ts dz
In (3a) we have presented an example of a language in which
assibilation is not triggered by a subset of the high front vocoids. The
process in (3b) is the only example to our knowledge of a process in
which a nonsibilant fricative is the output. And in (3c) we have listed
three languages in which the trigger is to the left of the stop (this
also holds for [3b]). In (4) we give examples of processes that are
often referred to as 'assibilations' in the literature but are
not conditioned by a vocalic element. The Old High German example in
(4a) is a sound change that transpired in the sixth and seventh
centuries. (3) The process in (4b) is apparently an allophonic one in
Modem Danish. The rule in (4c) is an optional process in Modern Polish.
Processes like these are excluded from our analysis because we focus
only on processes like the ones in (1) that are triggered by high
(front) vocoids.
3. Typology of assibilations
In this section we present a typology of rules like the ones in (1)
on the basis of our investigation of assibilations in over 30 languages
(see the appendix for a complete list of the languages discussed in this
article and the respective genetic classification). In Section 3.1 we
posit a set of ten logically possible assibilation types, only five of
which we maintain are actually attested. The five nonoccurring types
will be shown to be excluded due to two properties of assibilations we
propose below. In Section 3.2 we present examples of all of the
occurring assibilation types. (A sixth occurring type will be discussed
in Section 4.3).
3.1. Introduction
Recall from (2a) that the trigger for stop assibilation is some set
of the high front vocoids (i.e., the vowel /i/ and glide /j/). Given the
two triggers /i/ and /j/ there are four logical assibilations, which we
have listed in (5): (4)
(5) a. Assibilation is triggered by /i/ and (if present) /j/
b. Assibilation is triggered only by /j/
c. Assibilation is triggered only by /i/
d. Assibilation is triggered by neither /i/ nor /j/
Please observe that variable (5a) subsumes two possible language
types, namely those in which both /i/ and /j/ trigger assibilation, as
well as those in which only /i/ triggers assibilation because there is
no /j/ at all (or no /j/ in the assibilation context). By contrast, for
variable (5b) both /i/ and /j/ must occur in the assibilation context,
but only the latter segment triggers the rule. (The mirror image
situation holds for variable 5c). Another point concerning the logical
assibilation types in (5) is that the /j/ referred to here is intended
to include not only the segment /j/ but also secondary palatalization
(see Romanian in [11] below).
The second property we discuss concerns the sounds undergoing
assibilations, in particular we investigate the difference between
voiceless and voiced stops in the input. Thus, given the two input
segrnents /t/ and /d/, four possible assibilations are summarized in
(6):
(6) a. /t/ and (if present) /d/ assibilate
b. Only /t/ assibilates
c. Only /d/ assibilates
d. Neither /t/ nor /d/ assibilate
Variable (6a) describes two types of languages. First, languages in
which /t/ and /d/ assibilate, and second, those in which only /t/
assibilates because there is no /d/ (or no /d/ in the assibilation
context). By contrast, variable (6b) means that /t/ and /d/ must occur
in the assibilation context but only the former sound undergoes the
rule. (The mirror image generalization holds for [6c]).
Combining the eight variables in (5) and (6) yields sixteen
logically possible assibilation types. Four of these sixteen
combinations involve variable (6d), i.e., alveolar stops do not
assibilate at all (= [6d] + [5a], [6d] + [5b], [6d] + [5c], [6d] +
[5d]). We have classified all four of these combinations into one
language type, namely type E (see [7] below). Three of the remaining
twelve combinations show assibilation without a high front vocoid
trigger (i.e., [5d] + [6a], [5d] + [6b], (5d) + [6c]). Examples for
these kinds of changes (i.e., those in which the trigger is not some
high vocoid) were given under (4). Since these rule types are not topic
of the present article we do not include them in our typology in (7).
The remaining nine combinations correspond to the additional language
types in (7) and (8) (i.e., A-D, F-J). In this typology we have two
general categories (to be justified in Section 3.2), namely assibilation
types that are occurring (types A-E) and those that are not (types F-J).
(7) Occurring assibilation types:
Language Assibilating Trigger(s)
type segment(s)
A /t (d)/ /i (j)/ 6a + 5a
B /t (d)/ /j/ 6a + 5b
C /t/ /i (j)/ 6b + 5a
D /t/ /j/ 6b + 5b
E none /i (j)/, 6d + (5a, 5b, 5c, 5d)
/i/, /j/,
none
(8) Nonoccurring assibilation types:
Language Assibilating Trigger(s)
type segment(s)
F /t (d)/ /i/ 6a + 5c
G /t/ /i/ 6b + 5c
H /d/ /i (j)/ 6c + 5a
I /d/ /j/ 6c + 5b
J /d/ /i/ 6c + 5c
The sounds in parentheses in (7)-(8) are intended to capture the
optionality described above with respect to variables (5a) and (6a).
The typology in (7)-(8) takes all three assibilation types in (1)
into consideration, i.e., affrications, spirantizations and
posteriorizations. Thus, we show below in Section 3.2 that these three
assibilation types are attested for the occurring types in (7). It is
our claim that none of the three assibilation types is attested in the
five languages in (8).
We argue here that the nonoccurring language types in (8) are true
'systematic gaps' whose absence can be accounted for with the
following two universal properties of assibilations. (5)
(9) Two additional properties of stop assibilations:
a. Assibilation cannot be triggered by /i/ unless it is also
triggered by /j/.
b. Voiced stops cannot undergo assibilations unless voiceless ones
do.
In Section 3.2 we present examples of languages corresponding to
the various language types in (7), thereby lending support to the two
properties in (9). In Section 4 we present phonetically grounded
constraints that account for why the properties in (9) hold. (6)
Property (9a) can be tested by scrutinizing languages with
sequences like /tj/ and /ti/ in which assibilation affects /t/. Our
study is confounded by the fact that in many assibilating languages
there is a strict phonotactic restriction prohibiting /Cj/ sequences (or
more generally, any sequence of nonsyllabic segments). It is important
to stress here that (9a) cannot be refuted with a language that
assibilates /t/ before /i/ and that simply does not have any /tj/
sequences. Thus, this example is not Type G, but instead Type C. A
similar point can be made with respect to /t/ and /d/ as inputs. Hence,
if a language assibilates /t/ before /i j/ then it can only be
classified as Type C if there are /di dj/ sequences that do not
assibilate. If this language has no /di dj/ sequences to begin with then
this language is not Type C, but instead Type A. Type G and Type C are
illustrated in (10a) and (10b) respectively. The language described
above with a defective distribution, in which /ti/ assibilates but that
does not have /tj/, is classified as Type C (see [10c]).
(10) a. A nonoccurring assibilation rule (Type G):
/ti/ [right arrow] [tsi]
/tj/ [right arrow] [tj]
(/di/ occurs but does not assibilate)
b. An occurring assibilation rule (Type C):
/ti/ [right arrow] [tsi]
/til [right arrow] [tsj] (/di dj/ occur but do not assibilate)
c. An occurring assibilation rule (Type C):
/ti/ [right arrow] [tsi]
(/tj/ does not occur; /di/ occurs but does not assibilate)
Some of the sources for the languages we cite do not give enough
information pertaining to the occurrence of the relevant segments to
determine whether or not there are definitely defective distributions as
in (10c). In the following we compare languages for which defective
distributions are definitely known not to exist (as in [10b]) with those
in which they do (as in [10c]). Languages in which defective
distributions are unknown are listed separately.
3.2. The occurring language types
In this section we present examples from language types A-E. In our
typology we present more than 30 assibilation rules (as defined in
Section 2) in a typologically and geographically diverse set of
languages (see the appendix). Our survey subsumes the three kinds of
assibilations in (1). The assibilations listed below include purely
allophonic (postlexical) processes, as well as neutralizing and highly
morphologized (i.e., lexical) assibilations. Historical processes are
included as well. (7) Although our analysis in Section 4 is only
intended to account for the assibilation of anterior sounds before high
front vowels, we have also included below assibilations triggered by
other vocalic elements (e.g., high back vowels, mid vowels) because
these rules seem to obey the same generalizations in (9).
It will become evident below that there is an unequal distribution
among language types, in particular, Types A, C and E are represented by
many languages whereas only a very small number belong to Types B and D.
Among the A, C and E languages it appears that Types A and E outnumber those of Type C. We hypothesize that this unequal distribution is truly
systematic and that these patterns would be confirmed by investigating
assibilations in additional languages. Since we take the unequal
distribution among the various types as systematic and not accidental we
discuss a possible reason for it in Section 4.5 below.
3.2.1. Type A. Examples of Type A languages have been presented in
(11). In the second column we list the corresponding rule type.
(11) Type A languages Assibilation type
a. Quebec French (Cedergren et al. affrication
1991)
b. Kpando (Vhe) dialect of Gbe affrication
(Capo 1991: 99ff.)
c. Romanian (Chitoran 2001) affrication,
spirantization
d. Nishnaabemwin (Valentine posteriorization
2001: 86ff.)
e. Nyakyusa (Labroussi 1999: spirantization
341)
f. Runyoro-Rutooro (Rubongoya spirantization
1999: 27)
g. Japanese (Ito and Mester 1995) affrication,
posteriorization
h. Sorbian (WowCerk 1954: 24-25) posteriorization
i. Shona (Brauner 1995: 13) affrication,
spirantization
j. Ikalanga (Mathangwane 1999: affrication
80ff.)
k. Papago (Hale 1965) posteriorization
l. Plains Cree (Wolfart 1973: 79) affrication
m. Wai Wai (Hawkins 1998: 160) posteriorization
n. West Futuna-Aniwa posteriorization
(Dougherty 1983)
o. Blackfoot (Frantz 1991: 16, 26) affrication
p. Finnish (Sulkala and spirantization
Karjalainen 1992)
q. West Greenlandic (Fortescue affrication
1984: 333)
A straightforward example of a Type A language is illustrated with
the data in (12) from Quebec French (see [11a]; data from Kim 2001: 91):
(12) Stop assibilation in Quebec French:
Standard French Quebec French Gloss
[ti]pe [tsi]pe 'type'
[di]x [dzi]x 'ten'
[tj]ens [tsj]ens '(I) hold'
[dj]eu [dzj]eu 'god'
The data in (12) show that /t d/ assibilate to [ts dz] before /i/
and /j/.
A second example of a Type A language is illustrated with the
(historical) process of assibilation in the Kpando (Vhe) dialect of Gbe
(see [11b]). In the first column the relevant sequences in Proto-Gbe are
presented (in their underlying form) and in the corresponding line of
the second column the same sequences in the daughter language Kpando
(Vhe) (data from Capo 1991: 99-100, 104-105). (8,9)
(13) Stop assibilation in the Kpando (Vhe) dialect of Gbe:
Proto-Gbe Kpando(Vhe) Gloss
a. *-ti [atsi] 'tree'
*ti [tsi] 'be fed up'
*didi [dzidzi] 'be far'
b. *tja [tsja] 'to choose'
*dj[??] [dz[??]] 'happen'
In (13a) it can be observed that /t d/ assibilate to [ts dz] before
/i/. That the palatal glide /j/ triggers the same process is shown in
(13b). (In the second example in [13b] the palatal glide /j/ was deleted
after triggering assibilation of the preceding/d/). (10)
Stop assibilation in Romanian (see [11c]) is illustrated with the
examples in (14a) (from Chitoran 2001: 187). Recall from Section 3.1
that the secondary palatalization is included in the 'j'
contexts in (5). The effects of the rule can be observed in the third
column, in which it is shown that /t/ assibilates to the affricate [ts]
when it bears the plural marker of secondary palatalization and that /d/
spirantizes to [z] in the same context. (11)
(14) Stop assibilation in Romanian:
a. [munte] 'mountain' [[munts.sup.]j] 'mountains'
[soldat] 'soldier' [[soldats.sup.j]] 'soldiers'
[brad] 'fir tree' [[braz.sup.j]] 'fir trees'
b. [mult] 'much' [mults-ime] 'crowd'
[krud] 'cruel' [kruz-ime] 'cruelty'
The additional examples in (14b) (Ioana Chitoran, personal
communication, November 2004) illustrate that /t d/ assibilate before
certain [i]initial suffixes.
The Algonquian language Nishnaabemwin in (11d) has a lexical
process of posteriorization whereby /t d/ surface as [t[??] d[??]]
before morphemes that start with /i I j/ (Valentine 2001: 86ff.):
(15) Stop posteriorization in Nishnaabemwin:
a. /ma:d-ja:/ [ma:d[??]a:] 'leave/take off'
/bi:d-I-blz[??]/ [bi:d[??]biz[??]] 'come driving'
/api:t-I-gI/ [pi:t[??]gI] 'grow to such extent'
b. /bi:d-a:-dage/ [bi:dadge] 'come swimming'
/api:t-a:/ [pi:ta:] 'have height to such
extent'
In (15a) examples are given in which posteriorization affects /d/
before /I j/ and /t/ before /I/. (12) The examples in (15b) illustrate
that the /t d/ in the final two stems in (15a) surface as such before a
morpheme beginning with anything other than a high front vocoid.
Valentine is explicit that a /j/ following /t/ also causes
palatalization (2001: 88) but no example is provided. The Bantu language
Nyakyusa in (11e) has a lexical process whereby certain morphemes
beginning with a high front vocoid cause the spirantization of /t d/ to
[s] (Labroussi 1999: 341), e.g., the causative morphemes -i- [j] and
-isy- [ISj]. For example, the stems -end-a 'walk' and -pond-a
'forge, beat' change to [-e:s-j-a] 'cause to walk'
and [[??]mpo:s-i] respectively. No examples are provided in which /t/
spirantizes before /I j/, although Labroussi (1999) is explicit that
they should undergo the rule. Runyoro-Rutooro in (11f) spirantizes /t/
to [s] and /nd/ to [nz] if affixes are added that begin with /i e j/
(Rubongoya 1999: 27). In Japanese (11g; Ito and Mester 1995: 825ff.)
/ti/ surfaces as [t[??]i], e.g., /kat-i/ 'win' (infinitive) is
realized as [kat[??]i], and /di/ as [dzi], e.g., in the loanword dilemma
as [dzi[??]emma]. The high back vowel [u] (= [w] in a narrow
transcription) causes affrication of the preceding alveolar stop, thus
/kat-u/ 'win' (pres.) surfaces as [katsu]. Japanese has no
native sequences of [tj] or [dj], but loanwords show that /j/ after
alveolar stops also triggers posteriorization (Ito and Mester 1995:
837), e.g., tube [tcu:bu], and juice [dzu:su]. (13) Sorbian ([11h];
Wowcerk 1954: 24-25) palatalizes /t d/ before /i j/, e.g., hro[d]
'castle' vs. na hro[??]e 'on the castle' (from
/d-j/), hro[d[??]]ik 'small castle' (from /d-i/) and sko[t]
'cattle' vs. w sko[t[??]]e 'in the cattle' (from
/t-j/), kru[t]y 'firm' vs. kru[t[??]]isi 'more firm'
(from /t-i/).
Examples (11i)-(11q) are classified as such due to defective
distributions of either the glide, the voiced alveolar stop, or both.
For example, assibilation as diachronic process occurred in the
development of Shona (see [11i]), in which Proto-Bantu */ti/ changed to
[tsi] after a vowel and to [si] word initially (Brauner 1995:13;
Mathangwane 1999: 88), see [16a]. Proto-Bantu */di/ changed to [tsi],
see (16b). A palatal glide did not seem to occur in assibilation context
in Proto-Bantu, see Guthrie (1967-1971, vol. 2 appendix C and D).
(16) Stop assibilation and spirantization in Shona:
Proto-Bantu Gloss Shona Gloss
a. * -tima 'displant' [-sima] 'transplant'
* -tinde 'grass' [sinde] 'grass'
* piti 'hyena' [svitsi] 'spotted hyena'
b. * -diba 'pont' [dziua] 'pont'
A similar diachronic process occurred in the development of
Ikalanga (Mathangwane 1999: 80ff.), in which Proto-Bantu * /ti di/ is
realized as [[ts.sup.h]i dzi]. In nonassibilating contexts, Proto-Bantu
*/d/ changed to [1], leading to opaque present day assibilations caused
by the causative morpheme -i, e.g., [galai 'sit' vs. [gadza]
'cause to sit' (Mathangwane 1999: 85).
Papago (also called O'Odham; see [11k]) posteriorizes /t d/ to
[t[??] d[??]] before the high vowels /i u/ (Hale 1965: 299ff.). The
glide [j] only occurs in Spanish loanwords and as an epenthetic glide or
glided vowel /i/ (Hale 1965: 296), but not after /t d/. (14) In Plains
Cree in (111) /t/ assibilates before /i i:/ and before the palatal glide
(Wolfart 1973: 79). The output of this assibilation is a sound that
ranges from 'a blade-alveolar to a dorsolaminal affricate'
(Wolfart 1973: 79). Since Plains Cree has no /d/ that could potentially
assibilate we classify it as a Type A language. The situation is the
same for the Amazonian language Wai Wai (see [11m]), which has a lexical
process that posteriorizes /t/ to [t[??]] before /i e j/, e.g.,
/ti-irko/(15) [right arrow] [t[??]irko] 'fix/make it' (Hawkins
1998: 160), but that has no /d/ that could potentially assibilate. A
purely allophonic rule converting /t/ to [t[??]] or [d[??]] before /i j/
holds in West Futuna-Aniwa (see [11n]), e.g., the definite article /ti/
is realized as [t[??]i] and /tia/ 'to hit' as [d[??]a]
(Dougherty 1983: 7), but /d/ does not exist in this language. Based on
alternations between [t] and [ts] Frantz (1991: 25) posits the rule
't [right arrow] ts / --i' for Blackfoot (see 11o). (16)
Blackfoot has no /d/ but a palatal glide, which does not occur in
postconsonantal position (with the exeption of the glottal stop). In
Finnish (11p) there is a lexical rule spirantizing /t/ in stems that end
in -te before the nominative morpheme -i and the plural morpheme -i,
e.g., sute vs. susi 'wolf (ess.--nom.)' (Sulkala and
Karjalainen 1992). Finnish has neither j-initial morphemes that could
potentially trigger the rule, nor a /d/ that could potentially undergo
it. (17) In West Greenlandic (see 11q) singleton and geminate /t/ are
assibilated to [ts] and [tts], respectively, before /i/ but this
language has no /d/, and /j/ only occurs intervocalically (see Fortescue
1984: 335; Dorais 1986: 45).
The following 11 languages are possible examples of Type A
languages. The reason they cannot be definitively classified as Type A
languages is that the respective authors are unclear on whether or not
there are defective distributions like the ones described in the
preceding paragraph.
(17) Possible Type A languages Assibilation type
a. Fongbe dialect of Gbe affrication
(Lefebvre and Brousseau
2002: 21)
b. Taiof (Ross 2002b: 426ff.) affrication
c. Mongo (Spaandonck 1964: 192) posteriorization
d. Cheyene (Davis 1962: 36) affrication, posteriorization
e. Maori (Bauer 1993: 530ff.) affrication
f. Samoan (Mosel and affrication
Hovdhaugen 1992: 20)
g. Axininca Campa (Spring 1992: affrication
339)
h. Korean (Kim 2001) affrication
i. Sonora Yaqui (Dedrick and posteriorization
Casad 1999: 30)
j. Ancient Greek (Sommerstein spirantization
1973: 15)
k. Koyra Chiini and Humburi posteriorization
Senni (Heath 1999a: 34)
In the Fongbe dialect of Gbe /t d/ optionally affricate before/i/in
rapid speech (Lefebvre and Brousseau 2002: 21), e.g., /ti/ [tsi]
'squeeze', /di/ [dzi] 'be very good'. Lefebvre and
Brousseau (2002) mention no examples with /t/ or /d/ followed by /j/,
but Capo (1991: 104) transcribes the equivalent of Proto-Gbe /tj/
sequences as [t[??]j] for the Fon dialects. (18) In light of the
diverging sources, we cannot decide on whether Fongbe shows assibilation
before palatal glide or not. In Taiof /t/ assibilates to [ts] before /i/
and /d/ to [dz] before /i u/, with an optional posteriorization to
[t[??]] and [d[??]], respectively (Ross 2002b: 426ff.). (19) The
occurrence of a glide after /t d/ cannot be excluded, since consonant clusters stem from reduplication of monosyllabics, and /j/ occurs in
syllable-initial position. Ross's (2002b) data, however, do not
contain an example. In Mongo the nominalizing suffix -i causes the
posteriorization of /t/ and /[sup.n]d/ (Spaandonck 1964: 192), e.g.,
/-lot-/ 'flee' vs. [-lotsi] 'fugitive', and
/-k[[epsilon].sup.n]d-/ 'go' vs. [-k[[epsilon].sup.n]dzi]
'traveller'. Since the data available to us do not include any
-j initial suffixes, we cannot exclude j as a possible trigger of
posteriorization. Cheyenne changes /t/ to [ts] or [t[??]] before an /i/
(Davis 1962: 36), /j/ does not seem to occur in this language. Maori has
neither /d/ nor /j/, and /t/ assibilates to [ts] before /i/ (with an
optional realization as [t[??]]), e.g., iti 'small' is
pronounced as [itsi] or [it[??]i] (Bauer 1993: 530ff.). Bauer (1993:
533) explicitly mentions the gliding of /i/ to [j] after a consonant,
though no examples of gliding after /t/ are included in the data; thus,
we cannot infer whether a glide causes assibilation as well. In Samoan
(17f), /t/ is affricated to [ts] before /i/ (Mosel and Hovdhaugen 1992:
20). Like Hawaian, this language has no underlying glide, but the
authors do not explicitly state that the [j] that arises due to glide
formation (1992: 25) feeds affrication. In Axininca Campa the
morphologically conditioned process of assibilation (referred to by
Spring 1992: 339 as 'affrication') is only triggered by the
nonfuture tense marker /i/, e.g., /no-kant-i/ [right arrow] [nokantsi]
'I said'. Spring does not report that assibilation is
triggered by any /j/-initial morphemes. In Korean, like Axininca Campa,
assibilation is a derived environment rule that is triggered by suffixes
beginning with /i/ (see Kim 2001), e.g., before the nominative suffix
/i/ in /mar-i/ [right arrow] [madzi] 'first child' and before
the adverbial suffix /i/ in /[kat.sup.h]-i/ [right arrow]
[[kats.sup.h]-i] 'together'. (20) Kim lists no /j/-initial
suffixes that could potentially trigger the rule. In the Uto-Aztecan
language Sonora Yaqui (see 17i) /t/ is palatalized to [t[??]] if
followed by an /i/-initial morpheme, e.g., [wi:kit] 'bird' vs.
[wi:kit[??]im] 'birds' (Dedrick and Casad 1999: 30). (This
language has no /d/ that could potentially undergo the rule). In the
list of suffixes presented in their chapters on morphology Dedrick and
Casad (1999) list only one beginning with /j/, i.e., the cessative
aspect marker/-jaate/. However, the authors do not present any examples
in which a stem ending in /t/ combines with this morpheme. In Ancient
Greek (see 17j) /t d/ spirantize to [s] in the context V--iV
(Sommerstein 1973: 15), e.g., [plu:tos] 'wealth' vs.
[plu:sios] 'wealthy', but no examples are provided with a /j/
in the assibilation context. (21) According to Heath (1999a: 34) Koyra
Chiini and Humburi Senni (see 17k) underwent a diachronic process
whereby /t/ changed to [t[??]] before /i/, e.g., [t[??]i] 'be'
in Koyra Chiini and Humburi Senni has the cognate [ti] in the
neighboring language Koyraboro. /j/ does not occur after a stop due to a
very restricted set of consonant clusters (geminates and clusters of
nasal or liquid as first consonant). However, no example with /d/ is
given, and in Heath's (1999b) grammar of Koyra Chiini, this
diachronic process in not mentioned at all.
3.2.2. Type B. Type A languages can be contrasted with Type B
languages, in which only the palatal glide but not /i/ triggers the
rule. Three examples of Type B languages have been provided in (18):
(18) Type B languages Assibilation type
a. West Slavic (Carlton 1990) affrication,
spirantization
b. Sanskrit (Misra 1967: 142) posteriorization
c. Latvian (Forssman 2001: 97) posteriorization
The West Slavic example in (18a) requires some comment. According
to Carlton (1990: 114) Proto-Slavic *t and *d assibilated in the various
daughter languages before *j, e.g., in West Slavic *t surfaced as [ts]
while *d surfaced in the West Slavic languages Polish and Slovak as [dz]
and other West Slavic languages as [z] (i.e., Czech, Sorbian). We follow
the explanation presented in Carlton (1990:114), namely *t and *d before
*j surfaced as affricates in West Slavic and the change from voiced
affricate to [z] in Czech and Sorbian was a later development. Although
no examples are provided for *t and *d before *i, the discussion in
Carlton implies that assibilation does not occur in this context. Modern
Slovak and Polish are usually described as having a morphologically
conditioned rule (called Iotation), which converts /t d/ to [ts dz]
before /j/ only (Rubach 1993: 117ff.). If the historical process
described above is correct then this suggests that Modern Slovak and
Modern Polish inherited the rule.
According to Misra (1967: 142) in Sanskrit (i.e., Old Indo-Aryan)
/tj dj/ developed into geminate post-alveolar affricates, e.g., Sanskrit
/satja/ 'truth' and /vidjut/ 'lightning' were later
realized as /satt[??]a/ and /bidd[??]i/ respectively. Although no
examples are provided for *t and *d before *i the discussion in Misra
implies that assibilation does not occur in this context. Significantly,
this change was only triggered by the palatal glide and not by /i/.
In Latvian (18c; Forssman 2001: 97) [t[??] d[??]] derive
historically from /tj dj/, e.g., [lat[??]a] 'bear (gen. sg.) (from
*latsja', which itself derives from /tj/).
3.2.3. Type C. Type C languages (in which /t/ assibilates before
/i/ and (if present) /j/) are listed in (19).
(19) Type C languages Assibilation type
a. Hittite (Kimball 1999: 287ff.) posteriorization
b. Dutch (Booij 1995: 79ff.) affrication, spirantization
c. Woleaian (Tawerilmang and spirantization
Sohn 1984: 184)
d. Kosraean (Lee and Wang 1984: spirantization
406)
e. Solomon Islands languages affrication, spirantization
(Tryon and Hackman 1983: 77)
f. Tawala (Ezard 1997: 29ff.) spirantization
g. 'Ala'ala (Ross 2002a: 347ff.) spirantization
In all of the Type C languages in our survey but two (i.e., Hittite
[19a] and Dutch [19b]) /t/ assibilates before /i/, but /j/ either does
not occur at all or it does exist but it never surfaces after /t/ (as in
10c). As we noted above in Section 3.1 the preponderance of (10c)
examples over (10b) is simply indicative of the fact that many
assibilating languages like the ones discussed below ban Cj sequences.
According to Kimball (1999: 287ff.) Indo-European *t assibilated to
a (posterior) affricate before /i j/ in Hittite (see 19a), e.g., the
suffix *-tjo-in [hante-t[??]ja] 'last', and [ha:nt[??]]
'in front' (from an earlier form with a final */i/). Kimball
(1999) does not provide examples of an unassibilated *di. However, see
Luraghi (1997: 4), who has the word [edi] 'on this side'.
Dutch (see [19b]) is a language with a lexical rule that turns /t/
into [s] or [ts] after certain (Latinate) suffixes that start with [i]
or [j] (Booij 1995: 79ff.). These morphemes are -i, -io [io:] ~ [jo:],
-iaan [ia:n] ~ [ja:n], and -ion [i[??]n] ~ [j[??]n]. Examples are
provided in (20), in which /t/ surfaces as [s] after a consonant (see
[20a]) and as [ts] or [s] intervocalically (see [20b]).
(20) a. akt-ie 'action' [aksi]
president-ieel 'presidential' [presidensjel]
akt-ief 'active' [akti:f]
president 'id.' [president]
b. relat-ie 'relation' [relatsi] ~ [relasi]
rat-io 'ratio' [ratsijo:] ~ [rasjo:]
relat-ief 'relative' [relati:f]
rat-ificeer 'to ratify' [ratifise:r]
Significantly, /d/ does not change before suffixes that otherwise
trigger assibilation of /t/, e.g., kome[d]-ie 'comedy' (Booij
1995: 79, Note 30).
The remaining languages in (19), all Austronesian languages in the
Oceanic branch, are classified as Type C because of defective
distributions. For example in Woleaian (see [19c]) there was a
diachronic process of assibilation that converted Proto-Oceanic *t into
[s] before /i u e o/, as in (21) (Tawerilmang and Sohn 1984: 184): (22)
(21) Proto-Oceanic Woleaian
* tama > tama 'father'
* [??]atop > aso 'thatch'
* mate > mase 'to die'
* tika > sixa 'bad, angry'
* ?atun > asu 'louse'
Proto-Oceanic had a /j/, but allowed only CV syllables (Lynch et
al. 2002: 65). A similar process transpired in the history of Kosraean
(see [19d]; Lee and Wang 1984: 406), in which Proto-Oceanic *t surfaced
as [s] before front vowels (but *d was not affected). Tryon and Hackman
(1983: 77) note that assibilation also affected Proto-Oceanic *t in the
Solomon Islands languages (19e) Vaghua, Varisi, Ririo and Sengga (also
known as Central-East Choiseul), all spoken on the island of Choiseul.
In the first of these languages the assibilation was an affrication that
went into effect before high vowels and in the final three it was an
assibilation triggered by /i/. (23) In ali of these so-called Choiseul
languages Proto-Oceanic *d surfaced as [r], a trait it shares with the
neighbouring language families New Georgia and Santa Isabel. But none of
the latter languages underwent assibilation, which leads us to the
conclusion that the change of *d to [r] preceded assibilation, and
therefore the Choiseul languages had no /d/ to assibilate. Tawala (see
19f; Ezard 1997) underwent a diachronic process whereby /t/ was
fricativized to [s] before the high front vowel /i/. According to Ezard
(1997: 30), "the dialect variation of some forms reflect this
rule", e.g., [emota] ~ [emosi] 'one', [hota] ~ [hosi]
'only'. By contrast, /d/ remained unchanged, cf. badila
[badila] 'the name of a native almond'. Tawala has also a
palatal glide, but does not allow other than (C)V syllables, thus a
potential sequence tjV to trigger spirantization does not occur.
Synchronic processes of assibilation are also common in Oceanic
languages. For example, in 'Ala'ala (see [19g]; Ross 2002a:
347ff.) spirantization creates the allophone [s] from /t/ before /i/,
e.g., /?iti/ 'upward' surfaces as [?isi] but /a?ate/
'women' as [a?ate]. /d/ remains unchanged in this language,
e.g., /nodi/ surfaces as [nodi] 'coughs' (Ross 2002a: 348).
'Ala'ala has no underlying palatal glide, and though glide
formation from /i/ occurs, it takes place in initial and intervocalic
position only (2002a: 348).
The following four languages are possible examples of Type C
languages, since the respective authors are unclear on the possible
defective distributions.
(22) Possible Type C languages Assibilation type
a. Italian dialects (Tuttle 1997: 26ff.; posteriorization
Cordin 1997: 261)
b. Arosi (Lynch and Horoi 2002: 562) spirantization
c. Turkana (Dimmendaal 1983: 8-9) spirantization
d. Ambae (Hyslop 2001: 16-17) spirantization
In the Northern Venetian dialect of Italian (see [23a]), /t/
palatalized before /i/ and /j/. Thus the Old Venetian form [tiol]
'he removes' is now realized as [t[??]ol], and [tjeni]
'hold' as [t[??]en] (Tuttle 1997: 26). Tuttle (1997: 30)
reports that the inflectional plural marker -i, which became a glide
before vowels in rapid speech and was later lost, palatalized the
preceding /t/ in Ticino. Examples are quanti 'how much' (pl.)
that is realized as [kwent[??]] and alti 'high' (pl.) as
[alt[??]] or [elt[??]]. In the Trentino dialect a similar process must
have taken place, as the example gatti 'cats' [gat[??]] from
Cordin (1997: 261) suggests. Neither Tuttle nor Cordin explicitly state
that /d/ was not palatalized, nor are examples found that prove this
point. Furthermore, we do not know whether glide formation occurred
before or after the process of palatalization. For this reason we have
to classify the Italian dialects as possible Type C language. A further
possible Type C language is Arosi (22b), in which the contrast between
/t/ and /s/ is neutralized to [s] before /i/ (Lynch and Horoi 2002:
562). A glide [j] occurs only intervocalically in this language. Though
Lynch and Horoi (562) do not include any examples with [di] sequences,
they do not mention any restriction against this sequence or an
assibilation rule changing /d/ in this context, either. In Turkana (see
22c) Dimmendaal (1983: 8-9) reports that /t/ spirantizes to [s] before
suffixes beginning with a front vowel, e.g., /a-kI-mat/ [akImat]
'to drink' vs. /a-mat-I/ [right arrow] [amasI] 'I am
drinking'. Although this language has a /j/, no examples are
provided with a suffix beginning with /j/ that occurs after a stem
ending in /t/. Since /d/ is not explicitly excluded from the
assibilation and no counterexamples with nonglided [di] sequences are
given, we cannot safely classify Turkana as a Type C language. According
to Hyslop (2001: 16-17, 26) in all dialects of Ambae (see [22d];
Vanuatu) except for Lolokaro a fricative [s] developed from
Proto-Oceanic /t/ before /i/, as in (23):
(23) Stop spirantization in Ambae:
Proto-Oceanic Ambae Gloss
*tibo- > sibo- 'self'
*pati > [beta]esi 'four'
Proto-Oceanic *d became a prenasalized voiced alveolar stop [nd] in
the dialects of Ambae in all contexts, e.g., didiu 'ant' is
realized as [[sup.n]dI[sup.n]di[??]] (Hyslop 2001: 31) but it is not
clear from the discussion in this work whether or not /d/ was present at
the point in time when /t/ assibilated.
3.2.4. Type D. In Type D languages /t/ assibilates before [j].
Examples are provided in (24). The lack of Type D languages that exhibit
spirantization and posteriorization is probably accidental, due to the
small number of languages belonging to this category.
(24) Type D languages Assibilation type
a. Latin (Pope 1952) affrication
b. German (Hall 2004) affrication
Stop assibilation in Latin is illustrated in (25). According to
Pope (1952: 129ff.) and Jacobs (1989: l17ff.) /t/ affricated to [ts]
before /j/ in the course of Late Latin. Pope (1952: 129) writes that the
change is attested as early as the fourth century. This development is
illustrated with the examples in (25) from Pope (1952: 130):
(25) Stop assibilation in Late Latin:
* fortja > * fortsja 'force'
* faktjone > fatsun 'manner'
In contrast to the assibilation in (25), Pope (1952: 129) notes
that the same process did not affect/dj/. (24) The nonassibilation of
/ti/ is illustrated with the examples [santir] 'to feel' (from
*sentire) and [ortie] 'nettles' (from *urtika).
Stop assibilation in German is illustrated with the data in (26)
(from Hall 2004). That this is a regular process of the language and not
simply an inheritance from Latin is discussed in that source.
(26) Stop assibilation in German:
Negation [nega'tsjo:n] 'negation'
negativ ['ne:gati:f] 'negative'
Konsortium [k[??]n'z[??][??]tsj[??]m] 'syndicate'
Konsorten [k[??]n'z[??][??]t[??]n] 'gang'
In these examples we can observe an assibilation of /t/ to [ts]
before /j/. The example negativ in the second column is important
because it shows that the rule is not triggered by the vowel /i/.
Examples like Studium [[??]tu:dj[??]m] 'studies (sg.)' show
that assibilation only affects /t/ and not /d/.
An additional example of a Type D language may be certain dialects
of Albanian. Buchholz and Fiedler (1987: 38) note that /tj/ surfaces as
[ts] in Central Albanian, but the authors write that this process only
occurs in certain words.
3.2.5. Type E. Type E languages are those in which no segments
assibilate. Our main descriptive goal in this article has been to find
examples of languages with assibilations, so we do not claim to have an
extensive list of Type E languages. However, we do maintain that
languages belonging to Type E are extremely common. One example of a
Type E language is Chamorro (see [27a]). According to Topping's
(1973) description of the phonology there are no processes in this
language resembling assibilations as defined in Section 2. Although Lahu
(see [27b]) is one of the few languages in which labials assibilate
(recall Section 2), this language has no process of assibilation in
which the input is a coronal stop (Matisoff 1982). We speculate here
that certain language families (and possible linguistic areas) tend not
to assibilate/t d/. (25) The indiginous languages of Australia (see
Dixon 1980 for a survey) are an instance of such a language family. Two
examples of Australian languages are provided in ([27c], [27d]). In
neither of these descriptions is reference made to assibilation
processes.
(27) Type E languages:
a. Chamorro (Topping 1973)
b. Lahu (Matisoff 1982)
c. Nhanda (Blevins 2001)
d. Gaagudju (Harvey 2002)
A possible reason for the lack of spirantizations and affrications
in Australian languages might be that the typical Australian language
does not have fricatives or affricates (i.e., [s] and [ts]). Note that
the ban on sounds like [s] and [ts] in typical Australian languages is
also enforced at the level of grammar where allophonic rules go into
effect (i.e., postlexically).
4. A formal analysis
In this section we present a formal OT analysis of the typology in
Section 3.2. Specifically, we show that assibilation is captured by
ranking one or more markedness constraint ahead of a faithfulness
constraint that militates against changing the feature [strident]. It
will be argued below that the markedness constraints required to capture
assibilations are grounded in phonetics and that a (phonetically
motivated) universal ranking can be posited that rules out all of the
nonoccurring language types in (8). The occurring language types in (7)
(discussed in Section 3.2) will be shown to involve the ranking of the
one faithfulness constraint with respect to the universal ranking for
markedness constraints. In the following analysis we only restrict
ourselves to the assibilation of alveolar stops before high fronts
vocoids. Other kinds of assibilation processes (e.g., those in which the
process is triggered by other vowels, those in which velars form the
input) require markedness (and/or faithfulness) constraints not
discussed below.
4.1. Phonetically grounded markedness constraints
Clements (1999: 287ff.) observes that a sequence of voiceless
alveolar stop followed by a high vowel often results in a transitional
noise between the two segments, but no noise is present if the stop is
released into a nonhigh vowel. The cause of this transitional noise is
the narrow stricture of the high vocoid after the t release that
generates turbulent airflow. This observation was experimentally tested
by Kim (2001) with Korean sequences of heteromorphemic /t+i/ (which
undergoes the process of affrication), and monomorphemic /ti/, /tu/,
/te/ and /ta/. Her results show that the friction noise is longest for
the heteromorphemic sequence with a truly affricated t. The remaining
contexts can be ordered according to their length of friction from
longest in /ti/ to /tu/ to /te/ to shortest or almost nonexistent in
/ta/. (26) Kim supplemented these results with a discussion of X-ray
data from Quebec French to attest the point that a high tongue body for
following vowels in alveolar stop-vowel sequences favours transitional
noise. Clements states that for a language to develop a rule of
affrication or posteriorization, the frication noise has to be
"reassigned" to the initial stop segment (1999: 288). Clements
further assumes that the transitional noise in such sequences is not
universally present, but "it is just necessary that it appear[s]
with sufficient frequency in some contexts in a given language for it to
come to the attention of speakers" (1999: 289).
We adopt Clements's and Kim's findings that the
transition noise in these sequences is generated due to the stricture of
the following high vocoid. We also follow Clements in the assumption
that this noise is reinterpreted by the speaker/listener as belonging to
the preceding stop in cases of assibilation. We depart from Clements
with respect to the nonuniversality of the noise, since we posit that in
contrast to sequences of t plus nonhigh vowels, sequences of t plus high
vocoid always create strident noise, although its amount might be
language specific and dependent on context and other criteria such as
aspiration of the stop. An example of such a context is
morpheme-internal vs. heteromorphemic position; hence, in Korean the
friction phase of the /t/ in heteromorphemic /at+i/ sequences is
considerably longer than in tautomorphemic /ati/ sequences (Kim 2001:
100). Another context is before a high vs. a nonhigh vowel. For example,
in Korean the friction phase of /t/ in (tautomorphemic) /ati/ is longer
than in tautomorphemic /ate/ and /ata/ (Kim 2001: 100).
Thus, we argue that the friction noise in /ti/ and /tj/ sequences
follows automatically from their articulation. But the perception and
reassociation of this friction noise as belonging to the preceding stop
is language dependent and can be expressed in the perceptual markedness
constraints (28a) and (28b). (27)
(28) Two perceptual markedness constraints:
a. *ti: interpret the friction noise in the realization of /ti/ as
[tsi]. (28)
b. *tj: interpret the friction noise in the realization of /tj/ as
[tsj].
Markedness constraints that refer to perceptual or auditory information are not new in phonology; see, for instance, Flemming's
(1995) MinDist constraints and Steriade's (2001) correspondence
constraints that are based on a perceptibility map (the so-called
'P-map'). Kirchner (1998: l17ff.) also posits perceptually
based markedness constraints in his account of assibilations; see the
discussion of his proposal and how it differs from the present one in
section Section 4.3 below. The two constraints in (28) are similar to
the constraint *[ti] proposed by Kenstowicz (2003: 19), which he employs
for the palatalization process in Lauan Fijian and paraphrases as the
knowledge of Lauan speakers that /ti/ is realized as [t[??]i] (see below
for more discussion on this example). But whereas Kenstowicz's
constraint is purely speaker driven, the two constraints in (28) are
foremost listener oriented: the listener reinterprets the friction noise
as belonging to the stop and consequently produces underlying /ti/ as
[tsi].
The constraints in (28) only militate against forms without
friction noise, e.g., [ti] and [tj] respectively, which would lose out
to [tsi] and [tsj]. Note that candidates that involve a vowel change
(e.g., [tse]) satisfy both * ti and * tj. This point illustrates the
necessity of (vowel) faithfulness constraints (see Section 4.2 below for
discussion and tableaux).
For the two constraints in (28) we propose the universal ranking in
(29):
(29) A universal ranking:
*tj >> *ti
The universal ranking in (29) is based on the following
argumentation. [i] and [j] can be assumed to differ either
articulatorily (with the glide having a more narrow constriction) and/or
aerodynamically (with the glide requiring stronger air pressure). The
articulatory difference results in the same amount of air as for the
high front vowel passing through a more narrow constriction in a /tj/
sequence, the difference in air pressure results in more air passing
through the same constriction. Both arguments lead to a longer time for
the air to be released into the glide, resulting in longer frication
noise, which is attested by the findings by Hall et al. (2006), in which
the friction phase in German and Polish nonce words with /tj/ sequences
was significantly longer than that in nonce words with /ti/ sequences.
This observation can be expressed as universal ranking between the two
markedness constraints in (28) as in (29). Similar universal constraint
rankings grounded in phonetics are proposed by Boersma (1998) and Hamann
(2003). (29) In differentiating between the influence of the palatal
glide and a high front vowel on the duration of friction noise and
expressing this in separate constraints (28b) and their universal
ranking in (29), we differ from Clements' and Kim's
approaches, who treated /i/ and /j/ on a par.
Furthermore, both Clements and Kim restricted their predictions and
investigations to voiceless stops. In Section 3, however, it was shown
that voiced stops also undergo assibilation in a number of languages.
For the voiced stops, the two constraints *dj and *di can be stated (see
[30]). Like the constraints in (28), the ones in (30) express the
perceptual markedness of the relevant sequences:
(30) Two perceptual markedness constraints:
a. *di: interpret the friction noise in the realization of /di/ as
[dzi].
b. *dj: interpret the friction noise in the realization of /dj/ as
[dzj].
Since the articulatory and the aerodynamic differences between the
vowel /i/ and the glide /j/ stay the same independent of the nature of
the preceding stop, we posit the universal constraint ranking in (31).
(31) A universal ranking:
*dj >> *di
This ranking is also attested in Hall et al.'s (2006)
experimental study with German and Polish alveolar stop-high vocoid
sequences in nonce words, in which the friction phase in /dj/ sequences
was significantly longer than that in /di/ sequences. The same study
showed that /dj/ and /di/ sequences were always significantly shorter
than /tj/ and /ti/ sequences. That sequences with voiced stops generally
show less friction is attributable to two factors. First, the vibrating vocal cords of the voiced stop allow less air to build up behind the
constriction in the vocal tract than when the vocal cords are open for
the voiceless stops. As a consequence, there is less air pressure at the
release of the voiced stop and thus less frication noise generated.
Furthermore, voicing of stops requires a difference between subglottal
and supraglottal pressure (in order to let the vocal folds vibrate),
which is usually maintained by pharyngeal expansion and larynx lowering
(Kent and Moll 1969; Perkell 1969; BellBerti 1975). Pharyngeal expansion
also results in less air pressure at the constriction and less friction
at the stop release (Ohala and Riordan 1979). (30)
Taking these aerodynamic observations and the findings by Hall et
al. (2006) into account, the constraint *tj has to be ranked above its
counterpart for the voiced stop, *dj, and *ti similarly needs to outrank *di. It is not clear whether or not the friction phase is longer in /ti/
sequences than in /dj/ sequences; we therefore suggest that the two
constraints *ti and *dj are not universally ranked with respect to each
other. (31) Taken together, the two constraint hierarchies from (29) and
(31) produce the following universal ranking:
(32) A universal ranking:
*tj >> {*ti, *dj} >> *di
In sum, the markedness constraints posited above are based on the
listener's inclination to parse the perceived friction noise as
belonging to the stop. This idea differs significantly from traditional
markedness constraints in OT that are speaker driven, only, see the
discussion of Kirchner's (1998) approach in Section 4.3.
4.2. An OT analysis
The typology we present below relies on the interaction between the
universal ranking of the four markedness constraints in (33) with the
following faithfulness constraint:
(33) A faithfulness constraint: Ident-[strid]
The faithfulness constraint in (33) belongs to the IDENT family; it
penalizes the change from nonstrident (e.g., /t/) to strident (i.e.,
[ts], [s], or [t[??]]). We assume, following several authors, e.g.,
Jakobson et al. (1952), LaCharito (1993), Rubach (1994), Clements
(1999), and Kehrein (2002), that stops differ from the corresponding
affricates in terms of the feature [strident]. According to this view a
stop like/t/is [-strident] and an affricate like/ts/is [+strident]. The
analysis of any assibilation process requires that some markedness
constraint(s) be ranked ahead of the faithfulness constraint in (33).
This point is illustrated in the tableau in (34), in which the change
from /atia/ to [atsia] is shown:
(34) /atia/ [right arrow] [atsia]:
/atia/ *ti Ident-stri
a. [atia] *!
b. [??] [atsia] *
In the analysis that follows we do not distinguish between the
three outputs of the assibilation processes in (1), i.e., spirantization
with [s], affrication with [ts] and posteriorization with [t[??l]].
Instead we only discuss the manner change of stop to some strident sound
(indicated as [ts] in the following tableaux) without specifying the
exact phonetic realization. The different outputs (i.e., [ts] vs. [s]
vs. [t[??]]) require additional constraints that are not important for
capturing the typology in Section 3.2.
Given the universal markedness constraint hierarchy in (32) the
process of assibilation is captured by ranking at least one of these
constraints ahead of the Ident constraint in (33). This ranking is
illustrated in (35)-(38) for a Type A language (e.g., Quebec French). In
these tableaux the only crucial ranking is that all four of the
markedness constraints outrank the one faithfulness constraint. Evidence
for the ranking among the markedness constraints (e.g., *tj >>]
*ti) was discussed in (32) above. It is shown below how these rankings
rule out the nonoccurring language types. (32)
(35) /atja/ in Type A languages:
/atja/ *tj *ti *dj *di Ident-stri
a. [atji] *!
b. [??] [atsja] *
(36) /atia/ in Type A languages:
/atia/ *tj *ti *dj *di Ident-stri
a. [atia] *!
b. [??] [atsia] *
(37) /adja/ in Type A languages:
/adja/ *tj *ti *dj *di Ident-stri
a. [adia] *!
b. [??] [adzia] *
(38) /adia/in Type A languages:
/adja/ *tj *ti *dj *di Ident-stri
a. [adia]
b. [??] [adzia]
In these tableaux it can be observed that all four markedness
constraints outrank the one faithfulness constraint.
The analysis described above for Type A languages in (35)-(38)
cannot predict why *ti violations are repaired by assibilation, since
there are other theoretically possible avoidance strategies. One obvious
example would be the change from /i/ in /ti/ to some other vowel. Thus,
the question is why [atsia] is the optimal output form for the
input/atia/in (36) and not [atua] or [atea]? We account for the fact
that vowel changes are the dispreferred repair strategy for sequences
like/ti/by positing that there is a high ranked Ident constraint present
in (35)-(38) that militates against a change in vowel quality. Since the
kind of vowel changes described above are not the kind of repair
strategy one encounters in the languages of the world, we argue that
this Ident constraint is universally ranked ahead of all of the four
Markedness and Ident constraints in (35)-(38). In his discussion of
posteriorization in Fijian, Kenstowicz (2003: 20) posits precisely such
a constraint to account for the fact that the optimal output for a
sequence like /ti/ is [t[??]i] in Fijian and not [te]. To account for
the fact that no language allows vowel quality changes to repair
sequences like/ti/, Kenstowics posits a universal ranking in which the
Indent constraint militating against a vowel change outranks the Indent
constraint that militates against the change from/t/to [t.[??]].(33)
The occurring language types posited above in (7) are summarized in
(39) with a corresponding example. In (40) we have repeated from (8) the
nonoccurring language types.
(39) Occurring assibilation types:
Assibilating segment(s) Trigger(s) Example
A /t (d)/ /i (j)/ Quebec French
B /t (d)/ /j/ Romanian
C /t/ /i (j)/ Hittite
D /t/ /j/ Latin
E none none Nhanda
(40) Nonoccurring assibilation types:
Assibilating segment(s) Trigger(s)
F /t (d)/ /i/
G /ti /i/
H /d/ /i (j)/
I /d/ /j/
J /d/ /i/
The universal hierarchy in (32) together with the faithfulness
constraint Ident-[stri] yield six rankings, five of which correspond to
the occurring language types in (39). Here and below a nonranking
between the constraints in indicated with the curly brackets.34
(41) Language type Ranking
a. Type A *tj >> {*ti, *dj} >> *di >> Ident-stri
b. Type B *ti >> *dj >> IDENT-STRI >> *ti >> *di
c. Type C *tj >> *ti >> IDENT-STRI >> *dj >> *di
d. Type D *tj >> IDENT-STRI >> {*ti, *dj} >> *di
e. Type E IDENT-STRI >> *ti >> {*ti, *dj} >> *di
The sixth logically possible language type is the ranking *tj
>> {*ti, *dj} >> Ident-stri >> *di. This language type
is discussed in Section 4.3 below, where we show that it is in fact
attested.
The five language types in (40) do not occur because they would
require rankings that are not in harmony with the universal rankings in
(32). This point is made clear in (42):
(42) Nonoccurring language types:
Language type Illicit ranking
a. Type F {*ti, *di} >> Ident-stri >> {*tj, *dj}
b. Type G *ti >> Ident-stri >> {*ti, *dj, *di}
c. Type H {*dj, *di} >> IDENT-STRI >> {*ti, *tj}
d. Type I *dj >> Ident-stri >> {*ti, *ti, *di}
e. Type J *di>> Ident-stri >> {*ti, *tj, *dj}
An examination of the rankings in (42) reveals that they all
violate at least one of the universal rankings in (32). Thus, Type F
requires (by transitivity) that {*ti, *di} outrank {*tj, *dj} and Type G
that *ti outrank *tj. Types H-J are nonoccurring because they would
require *dj and/or *di to outrank *tj and/or *ti.
4.3. An alternative account
In this section we discuss Kirchner's (1998) OT analysis of
assibilation. In his account, a Lazy constraint is employed, which
militates against too much effort on the part of the speaker. As
Kirchner himself (1998: 116ff.) shows, this constraint alone is not
sufficient for an account of assibilation processes, since every
assibilated output involves more articulatory effort than a
nonassibilated one. To solve this problem, a so-called fortition
constraint is introduced, "which serve[s] to enhance the salience and robustness of perceptual distinctions" (Kirchner 1998: 26).
According to Kirchner (1998:117), a sequence such as/ti/is automatically
produced with some friction, which is in accordance with the position
taken in the present article. Therefore, Kirchner represents the output
candidates of an underlying form/ti/ as [[t.sup.s]i] (with a weakly
fricated release) and [tsi] (with a true affricate), see the tableau in
(43). This representation includes perceptual phonetic detail (namely
the transitional friction noise) in an OT production tableau, which is
traditionally employed to compare articulatory inputs to articulatory
outputs (recall Note 32).
(43) /ti/ [right arrow] [tsi] according to Kirchner (1998):
/ti/] [ *[+fric release, -strident] Lazy
a. [[t.sup.s]i]] *!
b. [??] [tsi] *
In this tableau, the highly ranked fortition constraint *[+fricated
release, -strident], militating against fricated releases that are not
strident, selects the candidate [tsi] as the winner, since this
candidate has a strident release. The speaker thus actively decides for
the strident output [tsi] to enhance a perceptual distinction, as the
definition of fortition constraints implies.
However, it is not clear from Kirchner's treatment which
perceptual distinction is meant to be enhanced by this output. In [tsi]
the friction is without question more salient than in [[t.sup.s]i], but
why should the output be maximally salient with respect to friction if
the underlying form has no friction at all? The approach applied in the
present article assumes instead that the production of an affricate is
preceded by a perceptual misclassification, thus the speaker just
produces what he or she interpreted as a listener. Furthermore, with
Kirchner's constraints, the restriction on occurring and
nonoccurring language types with respect to assibilation cannot be
predicted, since the segment undergoing assibilation is not included in
the constraints. Again, the present approach is superior since it
proposes separate constraints for each possible input and an inherent
ranking of these constraints, which automatically excludes impossible
language types.
4.4. Additional language types
As noted in Section 4.2 above the constraints posited predict a
sixth language type, which we refer to below as Type E':
(44) Language type Type E'
Ranking *tj >> {*ti, *dj} >> Ident-stri >> *di
Effect t, d assibilate before j; t assibilates before i
In the final row of (44) it can be seen that the ranking for Type
E' describes a 'mixed' system in the sense that it
captures two separate processes, namely one that assibilates/t
d/before/j/and the other that assibilates /t/ before /i/. In this
respect Type E' is very different from Types A-D, which all
describe a single process each. Note that Type Et is essentially a Type
B language that also has a process assibilating /t/ before/i/. We are
aware of only one Type E' language, namely English (see below);
however, we speculate that additional examples might be found among the
Type B languages.
The English examples in (45a) illustrate that /t d/surface as
[t[??]] [d[??]] before/j/-initial suffixes and the ones in (45b) that
the same kind of process takes place across words (especially before the
words you and your) in casual speech. (35) Importantly, neither of the
two processes in (45a)-(45b) goes into effect before a morpheme
beginning with a high front vowel, e.g., wha[t] if, *wha[t[??]] if.
(45) Assibilation in English:
a. perpe[t??]ual (cf. perpe[t]uity)
resi[d[??]]ual (cf. resi[d]ue)
b. wha[t[??]] you
ha[[d??]] you
c. democra[t] democra[s]y
presiden[t] presiden[s]y
vacan[t] vacan[s]y
luna[t]-ic luna[s]y
here[t]-ic here[s]y
poli[t]-ics poli[s]y
d. proso[d]-ic proso[d]y
melo[d]-ic melo[d]y
Besides the posteriorization process in (45a)-(45b), English has a
separate process that assibilates /t/ to [s] before/i/. Several
alternating pairs have been listed in (45c) that motivate this process.
Importantly, the process in (45c) does not affect [d], as illustrated in
(45d).
In addition to the language type in (44), there are five further
examples of mixed languages, but in contrast to Type E', these five
additional mixed types are all nonoccurring. The additional nonoccuring
language types are listed below in (46).
(46) Five additional nonoccurring language types:
Assibilating Trigger(s) Illicit ranking required
segment(s)
a. /t/ /i j/ {*tj, *ti, *di} >> Ident-stri >> *dj
/d/ /i/
b. /t/ /i/ {*ti, *di, *dj} >> Ident-stri >> *tj
/d/ /i j/
c. /t/ /i/ {*ti,i, *dj} >> Ident-stri >> {*tj, *di}
/d/ /j/
d. /t/ /j/ {*tj, *di, *dj} >> IDENT-STRI >> *ti
/d/ /i j/
e. /t/ /j/ {*tj, *di} >> Ident-stri >> {*ti, *dj}
/d/ /i/
As was the case in (43) each of the additional language types
in (46) is nonoccurring because it would violate the universal
constraint rankings in (30). Thus, the ranking {*ti, *ti, *di} >>
*dj in (46a) violates the universal ranking *dj >> *di and in
(46b) and (46c) *ti >> *tj is the opposite of the proposed ranking
*tj >> *ti. The ranking in (46d) requires *di to be ahead of *ti,
but it was argued above that *ti >> *di is universal. Finally,
(46e) requires *di to outrank *dj.
4.5. Frequency
As noted above in Section 4.1 the distribution among the six
occurring language types is not equal, since many languages fall into
the A, C and E category, and only a few in B, D and E' each. What
is more, Type C appears to be less common than Type A and Type E. We
hypothesize that these proportions are not due to chance and therefore
propose an explanation below.
We argue here that the unequal distribution among language types
-in particular the crosslinguistic preference of {A, C, E} over {B, D,
E'} --can be accounted for by considering whether or not the
natural class of vowels and glides (i.e., [i j]) is captured by the
markedness constraints. When the constraints *ti and *tj (as well as *di
and *dj) are ranked together above or below the one faithfulness
constraint then we see this as evidence that [i j] function together as
a unit. By contrast, if *ti and *tj (as well as *di and *dj) are ranked
on opposite sides of faithfulness then this means that [i j] do not
function together as a natural class. This point can be illustrated with
each of the six occurring language types in (39) and (41) to determine
whether or not the natural class [i j] is respected. This is shown in
(47), where we list each of the six occurring language types in the
first column. In the second column '+' or '-'
indicates whether or not the respective language respects or does not
respect the natural class [i j] (which we symbolize here as
'i/j').
(47) Language type i/j
Type A +
Type B -
Type C +
Type D -
Type E +
Type E' -
The table in (47) indicates that Types A, C and E are the three
language types in which the natural class [i j] is respected and that
Types B, D and E' are the three where [i j] are not treated as a
class. The lower frequency of Type B, D, and E' languages can
therefore be interpreted as a consequence of the tendency in the
languages of the world to treat [i j] as a unit. That this natural class
is important is substantiated by the fact that [i] and [j] are virtually
the same sound from the point of view of articulatory phonetics (recall
the discussion in Section 4.1 above). In addition, many linguists have
shown that [i] and [j] are positional variants in various languages,
suggesting that these two sounds are--at least in the unmarked case--one
at the underlying level.
A second generalization concerning frequency is that within the
A/C/ E category languages of Type A and Type E seem to be more common
than those belonging to Type C. This generalization can be expressed by
considering the natural class of/t d/(represented as 't/d'
below), which would be satisfied if the constraints *ti and *di (as well
as *tj and *dj) are ranked together with respect to the faithfulness
constraint. An examination of the rankings for Type A and Type E reveals
that both of these languages satisfy the t/d natural class but this is
not the case with Type C.
To summarize, the six occurring language types can be arranged in a
harmonic scale, which corresponds to frequency. (See Prince and
Smolensky 1993, who argue that markedness relations for segment types
can be arranged in a scalar fashion, e.g., Cor >> LAB, which says
that coronal is less marked than labial. Note that markedness in this
sense is also often correlated with crosslinguistic frequency).
(48) {Type A Type E} >> {Type C} >> {Type B Type D Type
E'}
What this scale says is that Type A and Type E are the most
harmonic assibilation types, which we interpret to mean that they are
the most common ones in the languages of the world. We hypothesize that
given a large enough sample of assibilations Type A and Type E will
predominate over the other types. Based on our typology Type C is
slightly less common than Type A and Type E but much more common than
Type B, Type D and Type E'. Again, only future research can
(dis)confirm the crosslinguistic predictions made by the hierarchy in
(48).
5. Conclusion
In this article we proposed two new universal properties for
assibilation rules and presented typological evidence from assibilations
in over 30 languages supporting them. We argued that assibilations are
to be captured in the OT framework by ranking markedness constraints
grounded in perception that penalize sequences like [ti] ahead of a
faithfulness constraint that militates against the change from/t/to some
sibilant sound. The six occurring language types were shown to involve
permutations of the rankings between several different markedness
constraints and the one faithfulness constraint. The article
demonstrated that there exist several logically possible assibilation
types that are ruled out because they would involve illicit rankings.
There are several questions referred to in the preceding paragraphs
that are worth further investigating in the future. For example, one
might want to establish a typology for assibilation processes that
affect velar sounds (e.g., '/k/ [right arrow] [t[??]] / _ i' )
similar to the one posited above in Section 3 for assibilations with a
coronal as an input. Our assumption is that the properties for
assibilation established in (9) would hold for these additional
processes as well. A future study dealing with assibilations might want
to consider more fine-grained vocalic contexts. For example, in the
present study we restricted our analysis to high, front, unrounded
vocoids, but, as we noted above, assibilations can be triggered by other
vowels as well, e.g., /y/, /u/, /e/. We suggest provisionally that the
vocalic triggers can be arranged in a hierarchy according to the
likeliness that they trigger assibilation, e.g.,/j/>
/i/>/y/>/u/>/e/etc. Clearly, this hierarchy can only be
(dis)confirmed with evidence from natural languages.
Appendix
Table 1. Index of languages
Geographical
Language Family area Source
'Ala'ala Austronesian Papua New Guinea Ross (2002a)
(Oceanic)
Albanian Indo-European Albania Buchholz and
(Albanian) Fiedler (1987)
Ambae Austronesian Vanuatu (Ambae Hyslop (2001)
(Oceanic) islands)
Apalai Carib Brazil (Paru Koehn and Koehn
Leste River) (1986)
Arosi Austronesian Solomon Islands Lynch and Horoi
(Oceanic) (2002)
Axininca Campa Arawakan Peru (Pachitea Spring (1992)
River)
Blackfoot Algic Canada (Alberta) Frantz (1991)
(Plains)
Chamorro Austronesian Guam Topping (1973)
Cheyenne Algic USA (Montana) Davis (1962)
(Plains)
Danish Indo-European Denmark Basboll and
(Germ.) Wagner (1985)
Dutch Indo-European The Netherlands Booij (1995)
(Germ.)
English Indo-European United Kingdom Luick (1921)
(Germ.) etc.
Finnish Uralic Finland Kiparsky (1973)
(Finno-Ugric)
Gaagudju Australian Australia Harvey (2002)
Gbe (Ewe) Niger-Congo
--Kpando (Vhe) (Kwa) Ghana Capo (1991)
dialect
--Fongbe dialect Benin Lefebvre and
Brousseau
(2002)
German Indo-European Germany Hall (2004),
(Germ.) Penzl (1972)
Greek (Ancient) Indo-European extinct Sommerstein
(Hellenic) (1973)
Greenlandic (West) Eskimo-Aleut Greenland Fortescue
(1984),
Dorais (1986)
Hittite Indo-European extinct Kimball (1999)
Humburi Senni Nilo-Saharan Mali Heath (1999a,b)
(Songhay)
Italian Indo-European Italy Tuttle (1997),
(Romance) Cordin (1997)
Ikalanga Niger-Congo Botswana Mathangwane
(Bantu) (1999)
Japanese isolate Japan Ito and Mester
(1995)
Kashmiri Indo-European India Wali and Koul
(Indo-Iran.) (1997)
Kinyamwezi Niger-Congo Tanzania Maganga and
(Bantu) Schadeberg
(1992)
Korean isolate North and South Kim (2001)
Korea
Kosraean Austronesian Caroline islands Lee and Wang
(Oceanic) (1984)
Koyra Chiini Nilo-Saharan Mali Heath (1999a)
(Songhay)
Lahu Tibeto-Burman Thailand Matisoff (1982)
Latin Indo-European extinct Pope (1952),
(Italic) Sommer (1948)
Latvian Indo-European Latvia Forssman (2001)
(Baltic)
Maori Austronesian New Zealand Bauer (1993)
(Oceanic)
Mongo (Lomongo) Niger-Congo Congo Spaandonck
(Bantu) (1964)
Nadroga (Fijian) Austronesian Fiji Lynch et al.
(Oceanic) (2002)
Nhanda Australian Australia Blevins (2001)
Nishnaabemwin Algic Canada (Ontario) Valentine
(Ojibwe) (Algonquian) (2001)
Nyakyusa Niger-Congo Tanzania Labroussi
(Bantu) (1999)
Papago (O'odham) Uto-Aztecan USA (Arizona) Halle and
(Tempiman) Clements (1983)
Pima Bajo Uto-Aztecan Mexico Fernandez
(Tempiman) (1996)
Plains Cree Algic Canada Wolfart (1973)
(Algonquian)
Polish Indo-European Poland Rubach (1994),
(Slavic) Carlton (1990)
Quebec French Indo-European Quebec Cedergren et
(Romance) al. (1991)
Ririo Austronesian Solomon Islands Tryon and
(Oceanic) Hackman (1983)
Romanian Indo-European Romania Chitoran (2001)
(Romance)
Runyoro-Rutooro Niger-Congo Uganda Rubongoya
(Bantu) (1999)
Samoan Austronesian Western Samoa Mosel and
(Oceanic) Hovdhaugen
(1992)
Sanskrit Indo-European extinct Misra (1967)
(Indo-Aryan)
Sengga Austronesian Solomon Islands Tryon and
(Oceanic) Hackman (1983)
Serbo-Croatian Indo-European former Kordic (1997)
(Slavic) Yugoslavia
Shona Niger-Congo Zimbabwe Brauner (1995)
(Bantu)
Slovak Indo-European Slovakia Carlton (1990)
(Slavic)
Sonora Yaqui Uto-Aztecan Mexico (Sonora) Dedrick and
(Sonoran) Casad (1999)
Sorbian Indo-European Germany Wowcerk (1954)
(Slavic)
Southern Kongo Niger-Congo Angola Halle and
(Bantu) Clements (1983)
Taiof (Saposa) Austronesian Papua New Guinea Ross (2002b)
(Oceanic)
Tawala Austronesian Papua New Guinea Ezard (1997)
(Oceanic)
Tumpisa Shoshone Uto-Aztecan USA (California) Dayley (1989)
(Numic)
Turkana Nilo-Saharan Kenya Dimmendaal
(1983)
Vaghua Austronesian Solomon Islands Tryon and
(Oceanic) Hackman (1983)
Varisi Austronesian Solomon Islands Tryon and
(Oceanic) Hackman (1983)
Wai Wai Carib Brazil Hawkins (1998)
(Northern)
West Futuna-Aniwa Austronesian Vanuatu (Futuna Dougherty
(Oceanic) Islands) (1983)
Woleaian Austronesian Caroline islands Tawerilmang and
(Oceanic) Sohn (1984)
References
Basboll, Hans and Wagner, Johannes (1985). Kontrastive Phonologie
des Deutschen und Danischen: Segmentale Wortphonologie und-phonetik.
Tubingen: Niemeyer.
Bastin, Yvonne (1983). Lafinale-IDE et l'imbrication en
bantou. Tervuren: Musee Royal de l'Afrique Centrale.
Bauer, Winfried (1993). Maori. London: Routledge. Bell-Berti,
Fredericka (1975). Control of pharyngeal cavity size for English voiced
and voiceless stops. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 57,
456-461.
Bhat, D. N. S. (1978). A general study of palatalisation. In
Universais of Human Language, J. H. Greenberg, C. A. Ferguson, and E. A.
Moravcsik (eds.), 47-92. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Blevins, Juliette (2001). Nhanda. An Aboriginal Language of Western
Australia. Honolulu: University of Hawai'i Press.
Boersma, Paul (1998). Functional Phonology: Formalizing the
Interaction between Articulatory and Perceptual Drives. The Hague:
Holland Academic Graphics.
Booij, Geert (1995). The Phonology of Dutch. Oxford: Clarendon
Press.
Borowsky, Toni J. (1990). Topics in the Lexical Phonology of
English. New York: Garland.
Brauner, Siegmund (1995). A Grammatical Sketeh of Shona: Including
Historical Notes. Cologne: Koppe.
Buchholz, Oda and Fiedler, Wilfried (1987). Albanische Grammatik.
Leipzig: VEB Verlag Enzyklopadie.
Capo, Hounkpati B. C. (1991). A Comparative Phonology of Gbe.
Dordrecht: Foris.
Carlton, Terence R. (1990). Introduction to the Phonological
History of the Slavic Languages. Columbus: Slavica.
Cavar, M. and Hamann, Silke (2003). Polish velar and coronal
palatalization its perceptual background. In Investigations into Formal
Slavie Linguisties, Peter Kosta, Joanna Blaszczak, Jens Frasek, Ljudmila
Geist, and Marzena Zygis (eds.), 31-48. Berlin: Peter Lang.
Cedergren, Henrietta J.; Archambault, Daniele; and Boulianne,
Gilles (1991). Stop assibilation in Quebec French: an analysis by
articulatory synthesis. Proceedings of the 12th International Congress
of Phonetic Sciences, vol. 3, 186-189. Aix-en-Provence.
Chitoran, Ioana (2001). The Phonology of Romanian: A
Constraint-Based Approach. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Clements, G. N. (1999). Affricates as noncontoured stops. In Item
Order in Language and Speech, O. Fujimura, B. Joseph, and B. Palek
(eds.), 271-299. Prague: Charles University Press.
Cordin, Patrizia (1997). Trentino. In The Dialects of Ba(v, M.
Maiden and M. M. Parry (eds.), 260-262. London: Routledge.
Davis, Irvine (1962). Phonological function in Cheyenne.
International Journal of American Linguistics 28(1), 36-42.
Dayley, Jon P. (1989). Tumpisa (Panamint) Shoshone Grammar.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Dedrick, John M. and Casad, Eugene H. (1999). Sonora Yaqui Language Structures. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (1983). The Turkana Language. Dordrecht:
Foris.
Dixon, R. M. W. (1980). Languages of Australia. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Dougherty, Janet W. D. (1983). West Futuna-Aniwa: An Introduction
to a Polynesian Outlier Language. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Dorais, Louis-Jacques (1986). Inuktitut surface phonology: a
trans-dialectal survey. International Journal of American Linguistics
52(1), 20-53.
Ezard, Bryan (1997). A Grammar of Tawala. Canberra: Pacific
Linguistics.
Fernandez, Zarina Estrada (1996). Pima Bajo. Munich: Lincom.
Flemming, Edward S. (1995). Auditory representations in phonology.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, UCLA.
Foley, James (1973). Assibilation as universal phonological rule.
Folia Linguistiea 6(3-4), 251-262.--(1977). Foundations of Theoretical
Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Forssman, Berthold (2001). Lettisehe Grammatik. Munich: Roll.
Fortescue, Michael (1984). West Greenlandic. London: Croom Helm.
Frantz, Donald G. (1991). Blackfoot Grammar. Toronto: University of
Toronto Press.
Guthrie, Malcolm (1967-1971). Comparative Bantu, 4 vols.
Fanrborough Heights: Gregg International Publishers.
Hale, Kenneth (1965). Some preliminary observations on Papago
morphophonemics. International Journal of American Linguistics 31,
295-305.
Hall, T. A. (2004). Assibilation in Modern German. Lingua 114,
1035-1062.
--; Hamann, Silke, and Zygis, Marzena (2006). The phonetic
motivation for phonological stop assibilation. Journal of the
International Phonetic Association 36(1), 59-81.
Halle, Morris and Clements, G. N. (1983). Problem Book in
Phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hamann, Silke (2003). The Phonetics and Phonology of Retroflexes.
Utrecht: LOT.
Harvey, Mark (2002). A Grammar of Gaagudju. Berlin and New York:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Hawkins, Robert (1998). Wai Wai. In Handbook of the Amazonian
Language, vol. 4, D.
Derbyshire and G. Pullum (eds.), 25 224. Berlin and New York:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Heath, Jeffrey (1999a). A Grammar of Koyraboro. Cologne: Koppe.
--(1999b). A Grammar of Koyra Chiini. Berlin and New York: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Hualde, Jose Ignacio (1991). Basque Phonology. London: Routledge.
Hyman, Larry (2003). Sound change, misanalysis, and analogy in the
Bantu causative. Journal of African Languages and Linguisties 24, 55-90.
Hyslop, Catriona (2001). The Lolovoli Dialect of the North-East
Ambae Language, Vanuatu. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
Ito, Junko and Mester, Armin (1995). Japanese phonology. In The
Handbook of Phonological Theory, J. Goldsmith (ed.), 817-838. Cambridge,
MA: Blackwell.
Jacobs, Haike (1989). Nonlinear studies in the historical phonology
of French. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nijmegen.
Jakobson, Roman; Fant, Gunnar; and Halle, Morris (1952).
Preliminaries to Speech Analysis: The Distinctive Features and their
Correlates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kehrein, Wolfgang (2002). Phonological Representation and Phonetic
Phrasing: Affricates and Laryngeals. Tubingen: Niemeyer.
Kenstowicz, Michael (2003). Salience and similarity in loanword
adaption: a case study from Fijian. Unpublished manuscript, MIT.
Kent, Raymond D. and Moll, Kenneth L. (1969). Vocal-tract
characteristics of the stop cognates. Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America 46, 1549-1555.
Kim, Hyonsoon (2001). A phonetically based account of phonological
stop assibilation. Phonology 18, 81-108.
Kimball, Sara E. (1999). Hittite Historical Phonology. Innsbruck:
Innsbrucker Beitrage zur Sprachwissenschaft.
Kiparsky, Paul (1973). Abstractness, opacity and global rules. In
Three Dimensions of Linguistie Theory, O. Fujimura (ed.), 57-86. Tokyo:
Taikusha.
Kirchner, Robert (1998). An effort-based approach to consonant
lenition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, UCLA.
Koehn, Edward and Koehn, Sally (1986). Apalai. In Handbook of the
Amazonian Languages, vol. 1, D. Derbyshire and G. Pullum (eds.), 33-127.
Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Kordic, Snjezana (1997). Serbo-Croatian. Munich: Lincom.
Labroussi, Catherine (1999). Vowel systems and spirantization in
Southwest Tanzania. In Bantu Historiem Linguistics, J.-M. Hombert and L.
Hyman (eds.), 335-377. Stanford: CSLI.
LaCharite, Darlene (1993). The internal structure of affricates.
Ph.D. dissertation, University of Ottawa.
Lee, Kee-Dong and Wang, Judith W. (1984). Kosraean reflexes of
Proto-Oceanic phonemes. In Studies in Micronesian Linguistics, B. W.
Bender (ed.), 403-442. Pacific Linguistics Series C-No. 80. Canberra:
The Australian National University.
Lefebvre, Claire and Brousseau, Anne-Marie (2002). A Grammar of
Fongbe. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Luick, Karl (1921). Historische Grammatik der englischen Sprache.
Leipzig: Tauchnitz.
Luraghi, Silvia (1997). Hittite. Munich: Lincom.
Lynch, John and Horoi, Rex (2002). Arosi. In The Oceanic Languages,
John Lynch, Malcom Ross, and Terry Crowley (eds.), 462-572. Richmond:
Curzon.
Lynch, John; Ross, Malcolm; and Crowley, Terry (eds.) (2002) The
Oceanic Languages. Richmond: Curzon.
Maganga, Clement and Schadeberg, Thilo C. (1992). Kinyamwezi:
Grammar, Texts, Vocabulary. Cologne: Koppe.
Mathangwane, Joyce T. (1999). Ikalanga Phonetics and Phonology: A
Synchronic and Diachronic Study. Stanford: CSLI.
Matisoff, James A. (1982). The Grammar of Lahu. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Meussen, A. E. (1967). Bantu grammatical reconstructions. Africana
Linguistica 61(3), 81-121.
Misra, B. G. (1967). Historical phonology of Modern Standard Hindi.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University.
Mosel, Ulrike and Hovdhaugen, Even (1992). Samoan Reference
Grammar. Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.
Netsell, R. (1969). Subglottal and intraoral air pressures during
the intervocalic contrast of /t/and /d/. Phonetica 20, 68-73.
Ohala, John J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract
constraints. In The Production of Speech, P. MacNeilage (ed.), 189-216.
New York: Springer. --; and Riordan, Carol J. (1979). Passive vocal
tract enlargement during voiced stops. In Speech Communication Papers,
J. J. Wolf and D. H. Klatt (eds.), 89-92. New York: Acoustical Society
of America.
Penzl, Herbert (1972). Methoden der gerrnanistischen Linguistik.
Tubingen: Niemeyer.
Perkell, Joseph S. (1969). Physiology of Speech Production: Results
and Implications of a Quantitative Cineradiographic Study. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Pope, M. K. (1952). From Latin to Modern French with Special
Consideration of AngloNorman: Phonology and Morphology. Manchester:
Manchester University Press.
Prince, Alan and Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality theory.
Unpublished manuscript, Rutgers University and University of Colorado.
Ross, Malcolm (1988). Proto Oceanic and the Austronesian Languages
of Western Melanesia. Pacific Linguistics Series C-No 98. Canberra: The
Australian National University. --(2002a). 'Ala'ala. In The
Oceanic Languages, John Lynch, Malcom Ross, and Terry Crowley (eds.),
426-439. Richmond: Curzon. --(2002b). Taiof. In The Oceanic Languages,
John Lynch, Malcom Ross, and Terry Crowley (eds.), 347-361. Richmond:
Curzon.
Rubach, Jerzy (1993). The Lexical Phonology of Slovak. Oxford:
Clarendon. --(1994). Affricates as strident stops in Polish. Linguistic
Inquiry 25, 119-143.
Rubongoya, L. T. (1999). A Modern Runyoro-Rutooro Grammar. Cologne:
Koppe.
Sommer, Ferdinand (1948). Handbuch der Lateinischen Laut- und
Formenlehre. Heidelberg: Winter.
Sommerstein, Alan H. (1973). The Sound Pattern of Ancient Greek.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Spaandonck, Marcel van (1964). Palatalization: a phonological
process in Bushoong. Journal of African Languages 3(2), 191-201.
Spring, Cari (1992). The velar glide in Axininca Campa. Phonology
9(2), 329-352.
Stappers, Leo J. (1971). Esquisse de la langue lengola. Africana
Linguistica 5, 255-307.
Steriade, Donca (2001). Directional asymmetries in place
assimilation: a perceptual account. In Phonology: The Role of Speech
Perception in Phonology, E. Hume and K. Johnson (eds.), 219-250. San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Sulkala, Helena and Karjalainen, Merja (1992). Finnish. London:
Routledge.
Tawerilmang, Anthony F. and Sohn, Ho-min (1984). Proto-Oceanic
reflexes in Woleaian. In Studies in Micronesian Linguistics, B. W.
Bender (ed.), 171-199. Pacific Linguistics Series C-No. 80. Canberra:
Australian National University.
Topping, Donald M. (1973). Chamorro Reference Grammar. Honolulu:
University of Hawai'i Press.
Tryon, Darrell T. (1976). New Hebrides Languages: An Internal
Classification. Pacific Linguistics Series C-No. 50. Canberra:
Australian National University. --and Hackman, Brian D. (1983). Solomon
Island Languages: An Internal Classification. Pacific Linguistics Series
C-No. 72. Canberra: Australian National University.
Tuttle, Edward (1997). Palatalization. In The Dialects of Italy, M.
Maiden and M. M. Parry (eds.), 26-31. London: Routledge.
Valentine, J. Randolph (2001). Nishnaabemwin Reference Grammar.
Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Wali, Kashi and Koul, Omkar N. (1997). Kashmiri: A
Cognitive-Descriptive Grammar. London: Routledge.
Wolfart, Chfistoph H. (1973). Plains Cree: A Grammatical Study.
Philadelphia: The American Philosophical Society.
Wowcerk, Pawol (1954). Kurzgefaflte obersorbische Grammatik:
Phonetik und Morphologie. Berlin: Volk und Wissen Verlag.
Notes
* We would like to thank an anonymous referee and Marzena Zygis for
comments on earlier versions of this article and Hristo Velkov, who
found a number of the examples presented in Section 3.
Correspondence address: T. A. Hall, Department of Germanic Studies,
Indiana University, Ballantine Hall 644, 1020 East Kirkwood Ave.,
Bloomington, IN, 47405-7103, USA. E-mail: tahall2@indiana.edu.
(1.) In addition to the two studies mentioned above the previous
literature on stop assibilations includes Foley (1973, 1977) and Bhat
(1978). It should be noted that the often cited typological study of
Bhat (1978) does not discuss the issues we treat below.
(2.) In the present article we represent affricates as a sequence
of stop plus (homorganic) fricative, e.g.,/ts dz t[??] d[??]/.
(3.) In addition to the word-initial context in (4a)/t/was
assibilated to [ts] in other environments as well, e.g., after/r/in
words like her[ts]a (cf. the English cognate heart). In addition,/p/and
/k/surfaced as the corresponding affricates.
(4.) In the ensuing analysis we employ slants '/ ... /'
to denote the input to assibilation and not to the underlying
representation. Thus, the segment we transcribe below as '/j/'
could either be an underlying /j/ that triggers assibilation, or it
could be a /j/ that forms the input to assibilation bur that itself
derives from some other sound, e.g.,/i/. The same point holds to the
triggers/t/and/d/in (6) below.
(5.) Foley (1973, 1977) proposes what he seems to considera
universal generalization for assibilations that is equivalent to (9a),
but he only discusses examples from English and French in support of it.
To our knowledge no one to date has proposed (9b).
(6.) We noted above in Section 2 that our study is restricted to
assibilations in which the input consists of an (oral) stop, but we
hypothesize that the same generalizations holds for
'assibilations' in the broad sense of the word. For example,
our impressionistic view of processes that change a velar stop into a
postalveolar affricate (i.e., /k/ [right arrow] [t[??]] before a front
vowel) suggests that property (9a) also holds. (9a) may also subsume processes of posteriorization in which the input is some sound other
than a nonstrident stop, e.g., fricatives like/s z/, as well as nasals
and laterals. It may even hold for processes not commonly characterized
as assibilations, e.g., the change from/[theta]/to [s] before /i, i:, j/
in Plains Cree (Wolfart 1973: 79). One might also want to speculate that
(9a) should be generalized to all glides and high vowels and not simply
/i/ and /j/. We also hypothesize that (9b) is valid for processes like
the ones in (4) above, in which the trigger is not a vocalic element.
Further research is therefore required to determine the extent to which
(9a) and (9b) hold for other phonological processes.
(7.) There is a general problem with admitting historical
assibilations, namely it is not always clear what the intermediate
stages were, or what the chronology was. For example, if /tj/ changes to
[tsj] at a later stage and /dj/ does not, one would not necessarily have
to conclude that this is a Type B language because the /di/ might have
undergone the same change to [dzj] followed by a subsequent change to
[zj]. In the present section we include historical examples only if the
source includes enough information to classify the language
unambiguously into the language types presented above. If the source
does not include enough information then we list the historical examples
separately.
(8.) Throughout this article we use transcriptions that are in
accordance with the IPA; hence, in certain examples the symbols in our
sources are replaced with the equivalent IPA sounds. For example, in
(13) Capo's [y] = [j] and his [[t.sup.s] [d.sup.z]] - [ts dz]. In
the data in (13) and in following all tones have been omitted in
examples from tone languages.
(9.) Capo (1991) posits a synchronic assibilation rule for
Proto-Gbe, according to which the underlying forms (as in the left
column in 13) are realized as [t[??]] and [d[??]]. If this process did
indeed exist in Proto-Gbe, then Kpando inherited and modified the rule.
(10.) According to Hyman (2003: 56ff.) most Bantuists (e.g.,
Guthrie 1967-1971; Meussen 1967) assume Proto-Bantu to have had seven
vowels. These vowels included the distinction between i and i, and u and
u, which correspond phonetically to [i i u o], respectively. A large
number of Bantu languages have merged *i/*i, and *u/*u to yield the 5
vowel system /i e u o a/. All 5 vowel Bantu languages except Lengola
(Stappers 1971) 'fricate' stops before *i and *u, as do some 7
vowel languages. It is also interesting to consider Bastin's (1983)
remarks on the following hierarchy of frication contexts for Bantu
languages:
a. before tautomorphemic i
b. before causative suffix gx *-ic.
c.before nominalizing suffix *-i
d. before perfective suffix *-id
This hierarchy means that d implies c, c implies b and b implies a.
Bastin's hierarchy manifests itself in at least three ways.
First, frication may be totally lacking in a context lower in the
hierarchy while present in the higher contexts. Second, frication may be
optional or affect only certain roots in the lower context. Finally, all
consonants may be fricated in a higer environment vs. fewer consonants
in the lower.
(11.) Chitoran (2001: 185ff.) treats the process of stop
assibilation in (14) as a part of a larger process she calls
'palatalization', which shifts the place of articulation for
other segment types, e.g.,/s/surfaces as [[??]] when secondarily
palatalized.
(12.) The triggers are deleted by a separate process. The initial
[a] in the final example in (15a) and (15b) similarly deletes by an
independent rule.
(13.) As was rightfully pointed out to us by an anonymous reviewer,
the input for the Japato be British English [[t.sup.h] ju:b], nese
assimilation of the English loan word tube is assumed h not American
English [[t.sup.h]u:b]. Our source for this language (Ito and Mester
1995) does not comment on this word.
(14.) Spanish loans do not undergo posteriorization in Papago,
e.g., [tianda] 'store' and [t[??]okola:di]
'chocolate'.
(15.) The prefix-final /i/ is dropped.
(16.) We would like to thank Fernando Zuniga for bringing the
Blackfoot data to our attention.
(17.) According to Sulkala and Karjalainen (1992: 365ff.), [d] was
introduced into standard Finnish only in the 19th century as
pronunciation of the letter d, earlier pronounced as [[??]]. This
segment only occurs in intervocalic word-medial position, as the weak
form of /t/ in consonant gradation.
(18.) The transcriptions by Capo probably reflect the diachronic
rule of posteriorization that applied in Proto-Gbe, recall Note 10,
whereas Lefebre and Brousseau describe an optional synchronic process.
(19.) The assibilation of /t/ seems to be a diachronic process in
Taiof, whereas assibilation of /d/ is still productive synchronically.
(20.) In the example [madzi] the effects of an independent
allophonic process of intervocalic voicing can be observed.
(21.) According to Sommerstein (1973: 28ff.), classical Attic can
be assumed to have had an assibilation rule that turned /t d/ into [ts
dz] before the (then deleted) glide /j/, with later changes of /dz/ to
[zd], of preconsonantal /ts/ to [s] and of prevocalic /ts/ to [tt]. This
might explain the lack of /j/ after coronal stops in Greek.
(22.) Oceanic is an example of a language family in which a
significant number of languages have assibilations (typically
affrications and spirantizations). In our typology we have only included
a handful. For references in which assibilations in Oceanic languages
are posited see Ross (1988) (for the Western Oceanic Linkage branch) and
Tryon (1976) (for Central-Eastern Oceanic).
(23.) For Varisi and Sengga assibilation occurred word initially
only, whereas in Vaghua and Ririo it also occurred word internally, with
the respective outputs [ts] and [t[??]] (Tryon and Hackman 1983: 77).
(24.) Word-initial /dj/surfaced first as a voiced palatal stop
[[??]] and then later on as [d[??]]; intervocalically /dj/ went to
[[??]] and then to [j]; see also Sommer (1948), who agrees that there
was a stage in the history of Latin with [[??]].
(25.) By contrast, an inspection of the literature in Section 3
reveals that assibilation rules are overrepresented in certain language
families. For example, a significant number of Oceanic languages are
attested with affrications and spirantizations (recall Note 22) and
Bantu languages with posteriorizations (recall Note 10).
(26.) The difference in friction length between /ti/ and /tu/ was
statistically not significant in Kim's (2001) experiment.
(27.) No markedness constraint for /tu/ sequences is included,
since the present article is not concerned with assibilation before high
back vowels.
(28.) The nature of the friction noise is not relevant for the
following discussion. Clements (1999) claims this friction to be
spectrally similar to the fricative noise of a postalveolar fricative [[??]]. Judging from perception, it seems more similar to the
alveolopalatal fricative [[??]], which finds confirmation in the
articulatory closeness of tongue position for [i] to [[??]]. The
intepretation of the transitional noise as belonging to the stop could
result in either of the affricates [t[??]], [t[??]] or [ts]. We assume
that other factors than mere acoustic quality of the transition noise,
such as already existing fricatives or affricates in the language in
question, determine the outcome of the perceptual integration process.
Ina perceptual experiment Cavar and Hamann (2003) compare the perceptual
similarity of [ti] with [t[??]] and [t[??]] and showed that listeners
judged the [ti] to be more similar to [t[??]].
(29.) Contrary to the present analysis, both Boersma (1998) and
Hamann (2003) pose underlying perceptual representations and a
distinction between production and perception grammar. Such an OT
production grammar contains perceptual faithfulness constraints and
articulatory markedness constraints. An example for the latter are
*Distance constraints (Boersma 1998: 150; Hamann 2003: 172), which refer
to the articulatory distance between different positions of an
articulator, and which can be universally ranked as *DISTANCE (x, z)
>> *Distance (x, y) if the distance between x and z is greater
than that between y and z ((z - x) > (y - x)).
(30.) The higher subglottal pressure and lower intraoral air
pressure for voiced stops compared to voiceless stops has been attested
for example by Netsell (1969) for American English.
(31.) The acoustic investigation of German and Polish described in
Hall et al. (2006) supports the ranking *ti >> *dj for these two
languages. We leave open whether or not this ranking is universal.
(32.) As is common use in OT, both input forro and output
candidates are given as articulatory representations. For the
assibilation cases and the perceptual constraints at hand, it is more
precise to distinguish articulatory forms from corresponding perceptual
forms, as done by Boersma in his Functional Phonology model (1998 et
sequei.). Applying this approach to the tableau in (35), the
articulatory candidate [atja] would have the corresponding perceptual
form |[at.sup.s]ja|, and [atsja] the percepttml form |atsja|.
(33.) Note that in a rule-based system one would also want to
account for the fact that '/t/ [t[??]] /-i' is an example of a
natural phonological process, as oppposed to '/i/ [right arrow] [e]
/ t _ '.
A question we leave open for further study is whether or not vowel
changes are truly not attested as repair strategies for /ti/ sequences.
See Kenstowicz (2003) for some discussion. Other repair strategies, such
as the change from/ti/to [ki], [pi] etc. are ruled out with additional
(universally) high ranked Ident constraints.
(34.) Technically speaking a 'nonranking' between two
constraints implies two separate rankings, e.g., for Type A in (4la) two
rankings:
(i) *tj >> *ti >> *dj >> *di >>
Ident-stri *tj >> *dj >> *ti >> *di >>
Ident-stri
Importantly, both rankings in (i) yield the same effect.
35. We follow tradition in English phonology in assuming that the
suffixes in examples like the ones in (45a) are /j/-initial, even though
this segment does not surface in many dialects. It should also be noted
that the process in (45a) only affects foot-internal/t d/, since these
segments are not palatalized before a /j/-initial suffix that begins a
foot, e.g., the underlined t in perpetuity (see Borowsky 1990).
Received 16 December 2003 Indiana University Revised version received Utrecht University 5 November 2004