New towns and koineization: linguistic and social correlates *.
Kerswill, Paul ; Williams, Ann
Abstract
The establishment of new towns in the twentieth century in many
parts of the world is a test bed of koineization, the type of language
change that takes place when speakers of different, but mutually
intelligible language varieties come together, and which may lead to new
dialect or koine formation. This article presents the case of Milton
Keynes, an English new town designated in 1967. Our study investigated
the speech of a sample of 48 working-class children divided into three
age groups: four, eight, and twelve years of age, along with one
caregiver for each. We hypothesize that the formation of a new dialect
is in the gift of older children. We also hypothesize that dialect
leveling, which is part of koineization, will be more rapid in a new
town than in an old-established town. Detailed quantitative results for
four vowels strongly support these hypotheses. At the same time, we
investigate the social network types contracted by new town residents.
We found many to be socially isolated locally, but that they maintained
contacts with their place of origin. We show that migrants violate what
the Milroys argue to be the normal inverse relationship between
socioeconomic class and social network density: migrants have uniplex
networks, while still having a low socioeconomic status. The
consequences for dialect change are considered.
1. Introduction: linguistic and sociolinguistic approaches to the
study of new town koines
This issue of Linguistics deals with language, migration and the
city. In this article, we focus on all three by referring to the case of
"koineization"--the process of dialect contact leading to what
Trudgill has called "new dialect formation" (Gordon et al.
2004; Trudgill 2004). New dialect formation is the emergence of
distinctive, new language varieties following the migration of people
speaking mutually intelligible dialects to linguistically
near-"virgin" territory (or at least territory where there is
relatively little contact with any prior languages spoken). We will
approach new dialect formation from two angles. The first involves
employing a "dialect levelling" model (Milroy 2002). Dialect
leveling is the "process whereby differences between regional
varieties are reduced, features which make varieties distinctive
disappear, and new features develop and are adopted by speakers over a
wide geographical area" (Williams and Kerswill 1999: 149). By a
more linguistic definition, dialect leveling can be viewed as the
reduction of variation in exponents of phonological and morphological
categories.
Of course, an understanding of this essentially linguistic process is closely tied to sociolinguistic factors. Our second angle is
therefore sociolinguistic or, better, sociological, focusing very much
on the social integration of migrants in a city. In sociolinguistic
studies, migrants are often treated as a "special case": they
are no longer part of their old communities, nor yet are they fully
insiders in their adopted ones. For this reason, they have tended to be
excluded from working definitions of the "speech community,"
such as that of Labov (1966) (see Patrick 2002). The few language
variation studies which have singled out migrants have demonstrated the
uniquely fluid and changing network characteristics of such individuals
(Bortoni-Ricardo 1985), their complex and highly variable linguistic and
social relationships with a "host" community (Kerswill 1994),
and their contribution to the overall sociolinguistic structure of a
city (Horvath 1985). This article deals, however, with a city--Milton
Keynes in the south of England--that is composed almost entirely of
migrants and their offspring. This means that the processes of
integration will be of a different nature, as the new inhabitants and
their children feel their way in a linguistic no-man's-land. We
hope to show that, after a generation or two, they eventually succeed in
forming a "new town koine."
2. The Milton Keynes and dialect leveling projects: some premises
Between them, the Milton Keynes and dialect leveling projects (1)
investigated new dialect formation as it was actually happening, while
at the same time placing this process within its wider geographical
context. The Milton Keynes project was designed around the following two
premises:
1) The formation of a new dialect in a new town is in the gift of
children, not adults, because:
--adults' speech is relatively fixed for both psycholinguistic (critical period effects) and sociolinguistic reasons (relative
fixedness of social identity);
--change in children's speech is initially not limited by
either of these considerations, but is increasingly influenced by them
as the children reach their early- to mid-teens.
2) The shape of the new dialect will be discernible in older
children's speech, not that of younger children. This is because:
--preschool children will be acquiring their parents' dialect;
--as children grow older, they will increasingly align themselves
linguistically with the speech of their peers. In a new town, there will
be no available focused model in the form of adult speech. For the first
cohort of young people growing up, there will also be no older teenage
model to form a (potential) target. This is the group who will lay the
basis for the new dialect.
The project used an apparent-time variationist methodology (Bailey
2002), with the difference that, in addition to adults, three child age
groups were chosen--4, 8, and 12 years--to allow us to investigate the
second premise. Appendix 1 shows the structure of the project.
The dialect leveling project extended the scope of its predecessor
by taking in two other towns/cities. One, Reading (to be discussed in
this article), was chosen to enable a comparison to be made between a
new town with high mobility (Milton Keynes) and a long-established town
situated in the same region but without this level of mobility. At the
same time, the presence of "regional dialect levelling" (2)
(in addition to new town koine formation) could be assessed. (3) The
premises for the dialect leveling project were the following:
1) In areas of high population movement, there may be rapid changes
in dialect and accent features, especially those involving leveling.
Social networks are loose-knit. The speech community is diffuse (Le Page
and Tabouret-Keller 1985).
2) Membership of a stable social network with strong local ties
leads to linguistic conformity. This inhibits change, including that
manifesting as leveling. The speech community is focused (Le Page and
Tabouret-Keller 1985).
3) A new town will prefigure any leveling tendencies within its own
region.
Thus, two variables were systematically varied: high vs. low
mobility (Milton Keynes vs. Reading), and high vs. low distance from
London (Hull vs. Reading). Here, we will only deal with the first of
these: mobility. The extent to which regional leveling is anticipated in
a new town can be gauged by a comparison of Milton Keynes and Reading.
The structure of the dialect leveling project is given in Appendix 2.
In Kerswill and Williams (2000a), we discussed the process of new
dialect formation from the point of view of eight "principles of
koineization," covering the loss of geographically and socially
marked forms, simplification, and the time scale of the process.
Kerswill and Trudgill (2005) cover the time scale in greater detail,
drawing additionally on New Zealand and Norwegian data. Williams and
Kerswill (1999) and Kerswill (2003) deal with regional dialect leveling
in England. The present article focuses very specifically on the role of
children in the process, drawing on new analyses of two variables
(Section 3). It deals with cross-generational continuity and speed of
change in new and old towns (Section 4). It also tackles the issue of
the type of individuals who are prone to migrate, and the linguistic
consequences of their particular social network characteristics
(Sections 5 and 6).
3. Dialect contact and the contribution of children to new dialect
formation in Milton Keynes
For the Milton Keynes project, three hypotheses were developed out
of the premises already established. These are as follows:
--Hypothesis 1: In a new town, children's speech will, in
general, be much more homogeneous than adults' speech. If this
hypothesis is supported, it can be inferred that children are creating a
new dialect. Homogeneity will, however, be tempered by the emergence of
structured variation, of the kind characteristic of focused speech
communities;
--Hypothesis 2: Older children will be linguistically more
homogeneous than younger children;
--Hypothesis 3: Older children diverge more from adult speech than
do younger children.
We consider data for two variables: the fronting of the offset of
the diphthong/[??]u/as in COAT and the fronting of/u:/as in GOOSE. (4)
The former has been sporadically noted as a minor tendency in British
Received Pronunciation, though the fronting of the whole diphthong appears to be involved (Gimson 1970: 134; Wells 1982: 294). Fronting of
the vowel nucleus (the onset) is found in Southern varieties of American
English (Labov 1994: 59, 202, 208). /u:/-fronting has been noted in many
parts of the English-speaking world (Bauer 1985; Clarke et al. 1995;
Torgersen 1997; Torgersen and Kerswill 2004). Because both changes
involve the fronting of central vowel, it may be instructive to compare
their sociolinguistic behavior; co-variation between them has been noted
in, for example, Philadelphia and other southern U.S. varieties (Labov
1994: 202).
Figures 1 and 2 provide visualizations of these changes. They show
F1-F2-F1 plots for several tokens of both COAX and GOOSE (the different
symbols representing different lexical items) and three reference
vowels, FLEECE, SXARX, and TRAP, with formant frequencies established
using SIL's Speech Analyzer software. The data is taken from an
elderly and a young speaker from Reading, where a similar change is
taking place. For COAX, the different trajectory taken by the diphthong
in the two speakers' productions is clearly visible. Each COAX
token is represented by a straight line showing its movement from an
onset with a high F1 (an open vowel quality) to an offset with a low F1
(a close vowel quality). In the case of the elderly speaker, this is not
accompanied by any appreciable forward movement. The young
speaker's diphthong starts at the same position as for the older
man, but moves rapidly forwards as well as upwards. For GOOSE, which has
both monophthongal and diphthongal variants, we find that the younger
speaker's pronunciations are much more fronted than the older
speaker's, overlapping with FLEECE to some extent. These patterns
were repeated for four elderly men and four boys analyzed in Reading and
Ashford (Kent).
[FIGURES 1&2 OMITTED]
Table 1 below shows the way in which the phonetic continuum for
/[??]u/-fronting was divided into four variants, and scores assigned.
For each speaker, an index score from 0 to 3 was calculated.
Figure 3 examines the association between caregivers and children.
The children have been ranked according to their index score, and the
caregiver plotted against the child. There is a significant but weak
correlation ([r.sup.2] =. 177; p < .01). (5) This is discernible from
the figure, there being some obvious associations towards the right-hand
end of the graph. While there is some association between adults and
children (which we discuss below), in almost every case the child has a
higher score than the caregiver. Only 11 of 48 caregivers have a more
front offset. The children would seem to be moving towards a new norm.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
The second premise of the Milton Keynes project suggests that we
should look for new dialect formation among older, not younger,
children. We would therefore expect a significant correlation between
caregivers and four-year-old children, on the grounds that at this age
the children are still strongly influenced by parental dialect/accent
features. Figure 4 shows the correlation of the four-year-olds'
index scores with those of their caregivers. Taking the caregivers'
scores first, we note that they cover a very wide range. Four of the
sixteen have scores close to 0, indicating high-back rounded
pronunciations characteristic of the north of England and Scotland. The
remaining twelve are all from the south of England, and show varying
degrees of fronting. Like the adults, the children fall into two groups:
those using high-back northern variants, and those favoring southern
diphthongs.
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
However, all the children are Milton Keynes-born, so we have here a
case of some young children acquiring their parents'
non-southeastern phonology, while others have either not acquired it or
have already accommodated to southeastern speech before the time of the
interview. In fact, we have direct evidence of this type of accent
mobility in this age group: one of the two boys at bottom left of the
figure, the child of Scottish parents, was using a mainstream
southeastern accent by the time he was recorded for a second time
eighteen months later.
For these four-year-olds, the choice between a high back variant
and a central or fronted diphthong is a binary one. Some follow their
parents, others turn away from them. However, there is a further, more
subtle pattern in the data. Among the twelve children who have
southeastern parents (represented by the large cluster in the center and
top-right of the figure), and who therefore do not have a gross binary
choice to make, there is a strong positive correlation with the degree
of fronting of their caregivers, with an [r.sup.2] of .3551 (p <
.05). A paired t-test on just these subjects (6) supports this result,
since there is no significant difference between caregivers and
children. This suggests, of course, that these children match their
(female) caregivers' quality for this vowel very closely.
Figure 4 shows that there is a great deal of diversity among the
four-year-old preschoolers, resulting (we argue) from the fact that many
of the children reflect the dialectal background of their principal
caregiver. However, in any speech community, we expect older children to
show greater homogeneity (as they turn away from parental models) and to
reflect a norm that is different from that of the caregivers (in many
cases advancing existing changes). These are essentially the arguments
put forward by Eckert (2000: 4) and Labov (2001: 427). In a new town,
these observations increase in importance because of the absence of
stable adult norms to act as a brake on change. In the next section, we
will consider the relative speed of change in old and new towns in
relation to the vowels of PRICE and MOUTH. For now, we must consider the
older children's scores, for which we would expect greater
homogeneity and a nonsignificant correlation with caregivers'
scores. Figure 5 displays the association between caregivers and eight-
and twelve-year-old children. Once the three children from northern
families have been removed, there is an almost complete absence of
correlation ([r.sup.2] = .0532). Paired t-tests for both the
eight-year-olds and twelve-year-olds and their caregivers show highly
significant differences (p < .001 in each case), and correlations are
not significant. By the age of eight, the children are apparently no
longer influenced by their caregivers' pronunciation--at least with
respect to this vowel.
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
Not only is there greater homogeneity, but there is also focusing
on a different norm even from that of the southeastern caregivers. Table
2 shows the mean scores for the southeastern caregivers and their
children, with paired t-test results, standard deviations, and a
calculation of [r.sup.2]. For the eight- and twelve-year-old groups, the
children's (ou) index is significantly higher than that of their
caregiver.
Figure 6 shows the data differently displayed. All the children
have, on average, a higher fronting score than the caregivers, a
difference which is greater for the two older groups. The fact that the
older children have higher scores strongly suggests that, in their
lifetimes, they have fronted their pronunciation of this vowel. We also
need evidence that the fronted vowel is a norm. Table 2 shows that there
is a reduction in the standard deviation as the age group rises, the
biggest reduction being between the four- and eight-year-olds. This is
visible in the tighter distribution of scores on the y-axis in Figure 5
than in Figure 4. This suggests that there is an increase in homogeneity
with age. Hence, we infer that the children are converging on a new,
fronted norm with respect to this vowel. For the moment, all three
hypotheses given above have been supported. (7)
[FIGURE 6 OMITTED]
For (ou), there are no significant gender differences within age
groups. However, this is not the case for (u:) (the fronting of the
vowel of GOOSE), to which we turn now. We ,will also find that our
hypotheses are not so unequivocally supported. (u:) was quantified by
calculating an index based on the scoring system shown in Table 3.
By contrast with (ou), there are no significant correlations
between caregivers and children either for the dataset as a whole
([r.sup.2] = .0424) or for any subgroup. Surprisingly, paired t-tests on
subgroups did not reveal any significant differences between caregivers
and children--though taking the dataset as a whole came close to
yielding a significant difference (p = .06). However, this
near-significance relates to a HIGHER score on the part of the
caregivers, the opposite of what we might have expected. Table 4 shows
the mean scores for caregivers, girls, and boys.
All this suggests (1) that there is no particular parental
influence with this vowel, and (2) that there is no ongoing change which
would give rise to a generational difference. We suggest that
/u:/-fronting may have been completed earlier than /[??]u/-fronting: the
women already have fronted vowels, and the absence of correlations with
caregivers or differences from caregivers means that there is no new
norm (which would be observable in late-adolescent or young adult
groups) for children to accommodate to as they grow older. Certainly,
for received pronunciation (RP), Bauer (1985) finds marked fronting
among speakers born in the 1940s. Our instrumental data shows the same
degree of fronting for elderly speakers in Reading (b. 1911-1915) as for
Bauer's RP speakers born 20-25 years later. This suggests that
/u:/-fronting was more advanced in nonstandard accents, such as those of
our speakers, and was already under way in the 1930s.
We suggested earlier that /u:/- and /[??]u/-fronting might be
linked. However, the correlation is insignificant, with a very low
[r.sup.2] of .0001. As before, we surmise that the reason for the lack
of correlation lies in the absence of age-related differences in the
data for (u:), unlike that for (ou). But there is one subgroup of
speakers whose behavior is very different: this is the twelve-year-old
girls. Figure 7 shows the correlation of the two variables for
twelve-year-old girls and boys.
[FIGURE 7 OMITTED]
Overall for the age group, the correlation is significant, though
it turns out that this effect is contributed almost entirely by the boys
([r.sup.2] = .7987, p = .002). However, the focus must still be on the
twelve-year-old girls, since this is the only group with a significantly
higher score than any other--t-tests showed that this group scores
significantly more highly than both twelve-year-old boys (p = .004) and
eight-year-old girls (p = .02). Table 5 shows the index scores.
In Kerswill and Williams (2000a: 93-94), we noted that the children
who scored the highest on (ou) were four twelve-year-old girls, all of
whom scored 2.0 or higher. We see from Figure 7 that three of the same
girls score highly on (u:) as well, though one does not (with a score of
.47). And we note that one of the high-scoring boys for (ou) also scores
highly on (u:), with an index of .87.
A qualitative analysis revealed (continued) /[??]u/-fronting to be
led by female children who are very well integrated into a (mainly
school-centered) peer group (Kerswill and Williams 2000a: 94). These
children were sociable and apparently well-liked. Interestingly, the boy
who scored highly on the index had mainly cited girls as his friends,
and was in turn cited by them; as noted, he too has a high /u:/-fronting
score. It appears that the interpretation adduced for /[??]u/-fronting
largely holds for /u:/-fronting, which has been noted elsewhere as a
female-led change (e.g. Torgersen 1997: 60).
In the context of a new town, we must consider whether these two
changes (one current, the other largely completed) are generated from
within the community (as an endogenous change) or whether they are
borrowed from outside (exogenous change: see Trudgill 1999 for a
discussion of this distinction in Norwich). It is quite clear that all
the changes we have observed in Milton Keynes are exogenous in
character--by contrast with the established city of Norwich. This is not
surprising: Milton Keynes inhabitants maintain contacts with their
original communities (on this, see Section 5), and the town has
extremely good transport links with other places, particularly London.
Indeed, we have hypothesized that, in Milton Keynes, these regional
changes are accelerated. We turn now to this question.
4. Cross-generational discontinuity and speed of change in a new
speech community
Two vowels in particular are converging on an RP-like pronunciation
in the working-class speech of the southeast of England. These are/au/,
as in MOUTH, which is converging on an RP-like [au], moving away from
localized pronunciations both urban and rural, including [[epsilon]:]
and [aeu] (London) and [[epsilon]I] and [[epsilon]u] (the southeast
north and west of London). Tables 6 and 7 show developments in this
vowel over about four generations in Milton Keynes and Reading: in both
towns, the youngsters almost exclusively favor [au]. We conclude that
this vowel shows both regional leveling (towards a supra-local form) and
social leveling (the difference between working-class and middle-class
speakers is reduced, in favor of middle-class forms). However, there are
differences between Milton Keynes and Reading. In Milton Keynes, there
appear to be three stages in the development of this vowel (Table 6):
first, a period of stability in which [[epsilon][??]] and [[epsilon]I]
predominated, followed at the height of the Milton Keynes settlement in
the 1970s by a period of greater heterogeneity in which [aeu], the form
favored by the majority of the in-migrants, was dominant. A
"re-focusing" finally began with the second-generation
migrants (the 1990s children), who are settling on [au]. The
discontinuity between the scores of each generation in Table 6 reflects
the lack of social continuity in this town, where most children have
grandparents originating elsewhere. We do not have data for the younger
adult generation in Reading; however, Table 7 indicates that young
working-class speakers, like their Milton Keynes counterparts, are
rejecting the regionally marked forms in favor of [au]. Significantly,
some young speakers (perhaps twenty percent) occasionally use the old
forms of their grandparents in a way that is indicative of the strong
social continuity in the part of the town in which they live (as shown
in Table 10, below).
The difference between Milton Keynes and Reading is more striking
for /aI/, as in PRICE. In Milton Keynes, [[??]I] or [[??]I] can only be
heard among the very oldest, pre-new town speakers, while in Reading
they are still to a considerable extent maintained by children. In both
towns, the new target appears to be [[??]I], which can be construed as
intermediate between RP [[??]I] and broad London [[??]:]. This variant
is geographically widespread in southeastern urban varieties. However,
in Reading, the generational change is much slower, reflecting (we
argue) the greater social continuity there than in Milton Keynes.
5. Network types among migrants to a new town
A question we need to answer is: do in-migrants to new towns show
particular levels and kinds of social integration that set them off from
people in old towns? If so, what are the linguistic consequences of
this? To begin to answer this, we must look at the relationship between
social integration, social mobility, and social class.
One of the most important contributions to the understanding of the
social embedding of language change in recent years has been the
proposal by Milroy and Milroy (1992) to combine the two fundamental
concepts of social class and social network into a single model. Here,
we hope to show that the relationship between class and network that
they propose needs modification to take account of highly mobile, but by
no means socially marginal, groups of internal migrants whose
sociolinguistic patterns are not normally considered in speech community
studies. (9) A discussion of the class-network relationship is one part
of a more general consideration of the social characteristics of
migrants.
The Milroys provide a critique of the Labovian variationist model,
the consensus model, based on a view of society as an integrated whole
in which the different parts work in harmony with one another.
Linguistically this should give rise to shared norms of evaluation and
cohesive speech communities. The consensus view is limited, however, by
its inability to account for the dynamic nature and continued vitality
of nonstandard vernaculars and therefore is unable to provide an
explanation for linguistic change. Such phenomena can be better
understood, according to the Milroys, by adopting as a framework a
conflict model which takes account of the inequalities, divisions, and
opposing interests found within society. This model shows how varieties
other than standard, legitimized varieties persist strongly and act as
badges of identity for less privileged groups.
Social network theory provides an explanation for the maintenance
of these nonstandard dialects by showing what the forces acting on
individual speakers might be. Close-knit networks act as powerful
norm-enforcement mechanisms. Strong networks both bind a local community
together and reduce the possibility of changes in behavior, including
linguistic behavior.
The interaction between social network and social class can be seen
when one considers that close-knit networks in the West are to be found
mainly at the bottom (and very top) of the socioeconomic scale. Thus,
the least powerful maintain strong social networks because of the NEED
to maintain such ties for survival. Many other speakers, however, do not
have the need for strong networks and are socially and geographically
mobile. They therefore come into contact with a wider range of people.
Within all groups in society, it is, according to the Milroys,
individuals who establish exceptionally large numbers of weak ties
outside their immediate communities who are able to facilitate language
change. Concomitantly, the transmission of innovations between groups is
effected by such individuals. We would infer from this that the spread
of changes occurs rapidly in socially and geographically mobile groups,
such as migrants. Conversely, they will be slow in groups with a strong
local base and close-knit networks.
The question arises: in a new town, where we can assume that most
people of whatever social class have loose-knit nonlocal networks, can
we expect dialect change to be accelerated? In order to address this
question, we begin by considering some of the social characteristics of
the families we interviewed in both projects.
--What kind of people move to a new town? A factor which seemed
to militate against the formation of close relationships was the
practice of "estate hopping." Most families in the Milton Keynes
project had moved at least once within Milton Keynes while three
families had moved six, seven, and nine times, respectively. Several
families had moved frequently before moving to Milton Keynes.
When attempts were made to locate the families a year after the
original recordings, only 39 of the original 48 children could be
traced, an attrition rate of 18.7%. However, a striking feature of
the families we interviewed was the fact that many people moved
as part of an extended family of three generations or adult siblings.
43% of our twelve-year-olds had their grandparents living in the
town, while this was true of as many as 75% of the four-year-olds.
This is in contrast to what we may suppose is the largely INDIVIDUAL
migration undertaken by middle-class families, who migrate
to seek better employment. The project families had mainly moved
with the promise of better housing (Kerswill and Williams 2000a).
--What is it like living in a new town? We can quote from a social
study carried out in Milton Keynes in the 1970s (Bishop 1986: 88):
"43% of Heelands residents reported no friendships or even casual
acquaintances on the estate. Neath Hill, (10) despite a low
friendliness score, recorded a high proportion of residents with
casual and friendly relationships within the estate." This slightly
conflicting report suggests a high level of isolation among a large
segment of the population, while others are not at all isolated, or
prefer to keep to casual acquaintanceships.
We now look at some of our own data. The following statements are
taken from the interviews with the working class women in the Milton
Keynes project (Williams and Kerswill 1997).
It took me about two years to even speak to someone. After the
first year I was cracking up. I just wanted to go back. I hated it.
Nobody had been born in Milton Keynes. Everybody had come from somewhere
else. You had them from everywhere--London, Scotland, Ireland. And if
you didn't come down with them ... they stuck to their own groups
They [the neighbors] only spoke to me once and that was to complain
You've got to keep yourself to yourself. You've always
got the feeling "Are they going to be talking behind my back?"
I say hello and that's about it
I never hardly see her [the neighbor]. The only time I see her is
when the video van pulls up. (Williams and Kerswill 1997:48-49)
But:
I love it here. It's the best thing I've ever done.
I'm not one for popping in for cups of tea here and there. After
all the years I've been on this estate, I've only got two
friends
The only people I talk to are the people opposite and the old
couple next door, but that's the way I prefer it. (Williams and
Kerswill 1997:48-49)
We have a picture, then, of independent, sometimes isolated
individuals, many with their main family and even routine social
contacts elsewhere in the country. Their networks are often uniplex,
though (as we have seen) some have migrated with their extended
families. Many of them in fact seem to seek out an isolated lifestyle.
As a rough measure of people's "rootedness" in their town
of residence, we can find out whether or not they and their parents were
born there. Quantifying this data will give us an estimate of the degree
to which the communities as a whole are composed of close-knit networks.
Table 10 shows, as predicted, that there is a great difference between
the working- and middle-class samples in Reading. The figures back up
information gained through our interviews: it was clear that the
majority of the working-class children had extended families on the same
estate, while this was not true of the middle-class group.
Table 11 for Milton Keynes shows that there is little to choose
between the two class groups: both are nonlocal in origin, to an even
greater extent than the Reading middle-class families. However, a number
of the working-class families have maintained kin-supported networks by
migrating in larger family groups--a factor which may promote the
maintenance of the family's dialect and inhibit leveling. We return
to this point below.
6. Class, networks, and dialect leveling in a new town and its
region
The picture we have of Milton Keynes is potentially at odds with
the Milroyian concept of the inverse relationship between social class
and dense social networks. The question is: do the relatively uniplex
networks of the working-class migrants lead to rapid language change? Do
they match the middle-class subjects in using few nonstandard
grammatical features, combined with accent features that are not easy to
place geographically? Milton Keynes working-class speech is indeed much
less of a LOCAL accent than is its counterpart in Reading. Elsewhere, we
have shown the "compromise" nature of much of the local
phonology (Kerswill and Williams 2000a: 86), resulting from the leveling
out of regionally and socially marked forms. A Reading accent, by
contrast, is still perceived as "country" (rural), even by its
own users (Kerswill and Williams 2002a), with vowels (especially the
local stereotype [[??]I] for PRICE) contributing strongly to this
impression. Certainly, too, in terms of dialect leveling, change has
taken place faster in Milton Keynes than in Reading, in a way predicted
by a model which sees loose-knit networks as promoting leveling and
change.
But there is quite another aspect to this. If, instead, we look at
the most commonly used nonstandard grammatical features in the southeast
of England, we get a different picture. Figure 8 shows the use of
nonstandard features by working-class teenagers in both Milton Keynes
and Reading in the interviews we conducted.
[FIGURE 8 OMITTED]
It can be seen that neither town has the advantage as far as the
use of nonstandard grammar goes. In both places, the use of these
features is relatively high. Moreover, their use is in striking contrast
with middle-class speech in both towns, where the use of these features
is so low as to be negligible. Class, not network (represented by the
proxy variable 'town'), appears to be the primary predictor in
terms of grammatical variation. A similar picture emerges if we consider
the use of three low-prestige consonantal features which are currently
spreading in Britain (Kerswill 2003): the use of [f] for /[theta]/, the
use of [v] for noninitial /[??]/ ("th-fronting"), and the use
of [?] for noninitial /t/ ("t-glottaling"). For these, class
remains the main predictor, taking precedence over 'town' and
'gender' (Kerswill and Williams 2002b).
The variables are strongly differentiated in terms of how they
pattern across social class and town/network--and we must seek
explanations for this. We can report that one of the vowels, ([??]u), is
being fronted in a way unrelated to either class or town (Cheshire et
al. 1999), while (u:) shows only marginal further fronting. Two others,
(au) and (aI), are changing towards regionally unmarked variants,
leading to the advergence of working-class forms towards those of
middle-class speakers; for these, Milton Keynes is quite strongly in the
lead. The three consonantal variables saw nonprestige forms entrenched among working-class speakers, with middle-class speakers making less use
of them. Finally, the same class-based pattern is found for the
nonstandard grammatical features.
In an earlier article (Kerswill and Williams 2000b), we argued for
the influence of different class-based cultures, resulting,
particularly, from different degrees and types of literacy, as a partial
explanation of the strong class-related differences in the use of
nonstandard grammatical features. We suggested that, for such features,
this overrode any leveling effect of loose-knit networks. We now pursue
this argument further by pointing out that pronunciation features are
thought to be more open to social marking than are grammatical features
because of their much greater frequency (Cheshire 1999: 61). This
openness in turn leads to the subtle, gradient vocalic patterns which
serve to differentiate the two towns in a way that correlates with their
characteristic network types. Yet not all pronunciation features show
this subtlety: the three consonantal variables are differentiated along
class lines, reflecting (we would argue) the fact that, like the
grammatical variables, their variants are binary, with one being
strongly associated with standard or "correct" speech. There
is no such association for any of the vocalic variables.
Even this is only part of the story, since it is clear from the
earlier discussion that there are quite marked qualitative differences
between the social networks of the working-class and middle-class
families in Milton Keynes, despite the fact that they all predominantly
originate elsewhere. This lies in the tendency for the working-class
groups to reproduce kin-based social networks in the new location,
through the migration of extended families--in contrast to the
middle-class migrants, who came mostly as single people or small family
units. The effect of this on the working-class speakers who are part of
such networks is likely to be conservative. Despite this, the members of
the working-class Milton Keynes sample AS A WHOLE have moved further
towards a leveled southeastern variety than their Reading counterparts
have.
7. Conclusions
Our conclusions must necessarily be complex. Dealing first with the
issues raised in the last section, we find linguistic variables behaving
very differently. Quite clearly, in the new town some features are prone
to rapid change by leveling. Two are vowels (MOUTH and PRICE) which
previously had regionally strongly marked forms, and which are now being
replaced by RP-like variants over a wide geographical area. Here, Milton
Keynes is in the vanguard of change. We have not conducted quantitative
studies of GOAT and GOOSE outside Milton Keynes; however, indications
are that changes in these vowels are NOT more advanced there than
elsewhere. If this is correct, we can point out that, unlike MOUTH and
PRICE, these innovations do not lead either to leveling or to
convergence with RP-like accents. Arguably, nonleveling changes (that
is, those which do not lead to greater homogeneity overall) are not
accelerated in a new town. By contrast, in a new community, leveling
changes will be promoted, since they are the result of processes
emerging from dialect contact; at the individual level, these changes
will be propelled by the frequent accommodation that takes place between
speakers of different varieties. In a new community, strategies of
neutrality might also play a part (cf. Scotton 1976; Maehlum 1992), and
it is in this context that a standard-like (RP) realization might be
favored by speakers--though it must be added that these variants are
probably not perceived as "RP," since they are not identical
with variants found in more conservative forms of that variety. RP,
narrowly construed, is unlikely to be a model for speakers today, since
it is an accent which has all but lost its position in English public
life (Kerswill 2001).
For the same reasons that changes in GOAT and GOOSE are not
necessarily favored in a new town, the spread of the nonprestige
consonantal features of th-fronting and t-glottaling takes place at
about the same rate in Milton Keynes as elsewhere. Unlike the near-RP
pronunciations [aI] and [au], these are not compromise forms, and so are
not part of leveling. Instead, they are innovations which are diffusing
in a geographically gradual manner (Kerswill 2003).
In this article, we have combined three methodologies. First, we
have undertaken detailed quantitative analyses of four phonetic
variables, analyzed in the light of the fact that their users are
growing up in a new community. Second, we have adopted a comparative
approach involving a carefully matched established town. Third, we have
applied an interpretive approach in assessing the type of social
networks people in new towns tend to contract. The three methodologies
complement each other, in that the quantitative patterns can only be
understood against the backdrop of the particular social structures of a
new community.
Appendix 1. Structure of the Milton Keynes project
--Milton Keynes: a "new town" designated in 1967; 1967
population 43,000, 1991 population 176,000. 70 kms from London,
Coventry, and Cambridge. Migration mainly from southeast England,
including London. London: 35%. Rest of Southeast: 41%.
--Research site: adjoining districts in the original part of the
new town; largely rented accommodation in fiats and terraced houses.
This site was selected in the expectation of locating mainly nonstandard
speakers.
--Subjects: ages 4, 8, and 12, girls and boys, eight in each cell.
Total 48 children, all born in Milton Keynes or having arrived there
within the first two years of life. Located through local nursery and
primary schools.
--The principal caregiver of each child was recorded--in all but
two cases the mother; remaining cases were an aunt and the father. All
caregivers are broadly "working class" by the criteria of
occupation and educational level.
--Three "styles," each eliciting a set of target words:
single word elicitation; connected speech task; reading list (not
four-year-olds). Recordings conducted in 1991.
--Families studied are from London, area immediately north of
London, Essex, Milton Keynes area, and Scotland. The sample reflects the
balance of in-migrants to the town (MKDC 1990).
--Linguistic variables: 6 vowels and 4 consonants.
--Transcription: 10-30 tokens of each variable from each speaker
were transcribed (less for some four-year-olds). The authors agreed on
auditory values. AW recorded, transcribed, and coded the data, with
5-10% checked by PK.
Subjects were located through local secondary schools, selected on
the basis of local knowledge of the social characteristics of their
catchment areas.
--Recordings:
(i) Ethnographic interviews in pairs, including reading lists;
group discussions on school, teenage concerns, fashion, music, and
language; dialect recognition task (adolescents)
(ii) One-to-one interviews (elderly respondents)
--Linguistic variables: 3 vowels, 4 consonants, 12 grammatical
features
--Transcription: as for Milton Keynes project (vowels and
consonants); orthographically transcribed searchable corpus (grammatical
variables)
Appendix 2. Structure of the dialect leveling project
Three towns:
Table 12. Levels of mobility and locations of the towns investigated
High mobility (new town)? Location
Reading no Southeast
Milton Keynes yes Southeast
Hull no North
In each town:
Table 13. Structure of sample in each town
N Age
Middle class Girls 8 14-15
Boys 8
Working class Girls 8
Boys 8
Elderly women 2 70-80
Elderly men 2
Notes
* Correspondence address: Paul Kerswill, Department of Linguistics and English Language, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YT, United
Kingdom. E-mail: p.kerswill@lancaster.ac.uk.
(1.) A new dialect in a new city: children's and adults'
speech in Milton Keynes, directed by Paul Kerswill at the University of
Reading, 1990-1994, funded by the Economic and Social Research Council (ref. R000232376). Research fellow: Ann Williams. The role of
adolescents in dialect leveling, jointly directed by Jenny Cheshire,
Paul Kerswill, and Ann Williams, 1995-1999, funded by the ESRC (ref.
R000236180). Research fellows: Ann Williams and Ann Gillett.
(2.) Regional dialect leveling is also known as "dialect
supralocalization" (cf. Milroy et al. 1994). This is the formation
of leveled, "supralocal" varieties, with few local differences
within a region, resulting from social changes, particularly increases
in mobility. Kerswill (2003) distinguishes between regional dialect
leveling as primarily a geographical phenomenon and "dialect
levelling by accommodation," which refers to the social
psychological mechanisms leading to the leveled varieties.
(3.) The third town was Hull on the northeast coast of England,
chosen to test the effect of geographical distance from London on
dialect leveling between regions.
(4.) These words are used mnemonically following Wells' (1982)
system.
(5.) Using the Excel regression function.
(6.) Using the Excel paired t-test function.
(7.) Wells (1982: 146) notes the great social and regional
variability of this vowel; this would make it particularly available as
a social marker. The subtle gradient and quantitative patterns reported
here are doubtless symptomatic of this.
(8.) Data from 14/15-year-olds is from the dialect leveling
project.
(9.) Kerswill and Williams (2000b) elaborate further on the link
between class and network.
(10.) Heelands and Neath Hill are pseudonyms.
References
Bailey, Guy (2002). Real and apparent time. In The Handbook of
Language Variation and Change, J. K. Chambers, Peter Trudgill, and
Natalie Schilling-Estes (eds.), 312-332. Oxford: Blackwell.
Bauer, Laurie (1985). Tracing phonetic change in the received
pronunciation of British English. Journal of Phonetics 13, 61-81.
Bishop, Jeff (1986). Milton Keynes--the best of both worlds: public
and professional views of a new city. Occasional Paper 24, University of
Bristol School for Advanced Urban Studies.
Bortoni-Ricardo, Stella M. (1985). The Urbanization of Rural
Dialect Speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cheshire, Jenny (1999). Taming the vernacular: some repercussions for the study of syntactic variation and spoken grammar. Cuadernos de
Filologia Inglese 8, 59-80.
--; Kerswill, Paul; and Williams, Ann (1999). The role of
adolescents in dialect levelling. Final report submitted to the Economic
and Social Research Council of Great Britain.
Clarke, Sandra; Elms, Ford; and Youssef, Amani (1995). The third
dialect of English: some Canadian evidence. Language Variation and
Change 7, 209-228.
Eckert, Penelope (2000). Language Variation as Social Practice.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Gimson, A. C. (1970). An Introduction to the Pronunciation of
English, 2nd ed. London: Arnold.
Gordon, Elizabeth; Campbell, Lyle; Hay, Jennifer; Maclagan,
Margaret; Sudbury, Andrea; and Trudgill, Peter (2004). New Zealand
English: Its Origins and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Horvath, Barbara (1985). Variation in Australian English. The
Sociolects of Sydney. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kerswill, Paul (1994). Dialects Converging. Rural Speech in Urban
Norway. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
--(2001). Mobility, meritocracy and dialect levelling: the fading
(and phasing) out of received pronunciation. In British Studies in the
New Millennium: The Challenge of the Grassroots, Pilvi Rajamae and
Krista Vogelberg (eds.), 45-58. Tartu: University of Tartu. [Also at
www.universalteacher.org.uk/lang/rp.htm.]
--(2003). Dialect levelling and geographical diffusion in British
English. In Social Dialectology. In Honour of Peter Trudgill, David
Britain and Jenny Cheshire (eds.), 223-243. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
--; and Trudgill, Peter (2005). The birth of new dialects. In
Dialect Change: Convergence and Divergence in European Languages, Peter
Auer, Frans Hinskens, and Paul Kerswill (eds.), 196-220. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
--; and Williams, Ann (2000a). Creating a new town koine: children
and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society 29 (1),
65-115.
--; and Williams, A. (2000b). Mobility and social class in dialect
levelling: evidence from new and old towns in England. In Dialect and
Migration in a Changing Europe, Klaus Mattheier (ed.), 1-13. Frankfurt
a.M.: Peter Lang.
--; and Williams, Ann (2002a). Dialect recognition and speech
community focusing in new and old towns in England: the effects of
dialect levelling, demography and social networks. In A Handbook of
Perceptual Dialectology, Vol. 2, Daniel Long and Dennis Preston (eds.),
178-207. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (Also in Dialectal Variation in
English. Proceedings of the Harold Orton Centenary Conference 1998.
Leeds Studies in English New Series XXX, Clive Upton and Katie Wales (eds.), 205-241. Leeds: School of English, University of Leeds.)
--; and Williams, Ann (2002b). "Salience" as an
explanatory factor in language change: evidence from dialect levelling
in urban England. In Contact-Induced Language Change. An Examination of
Internal External and Non-linguistic Factors, Marl C. Jones and Edith
Esch (eds.), 81-110. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. (Also in
[2002] Reading Working Papers in Linguistics 4, Richard Ingham and Paul
Kerswill (eds.), 63-94. Reading: Department of Linguistic Science,
University of Reading, http:ffwww.personal.rdg.ac.uk/~llsroach/wp4.pdf.)
Labov, William (1966). The Social Stratification of English in New
York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.
--(1994). Principles of Linguistic Change, Vol. 1. Internal
Factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
--(2001). Principles of Linguistic Change, Vol. 2: Social Factors.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Le Page, Robert; and Tabouret-Keller, Andrre (1985). Acts of
Identity: Creole-based Approaches to Language and Ethnicity. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Maehlum, B. (1992). Dialect socialization in Longyearbyen, Svalbard
(Spitsbergen): a fruitful chaos. In Language Contact and Language
Change, Ernst Hakon Jahr (ed.), 117-130. Berlin and New York: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Milroy, James; Milroy, Lesley; and Hartley, Sue (1994). Local and
supra-local change in British English: the case of glottalisation.
English World- Wide 15, 1-15.
Milroy, Lesley (2002). Introduction: mobility, contact and language
change--working with contemporary speech communities. Journal of
Sociolinguistics 6 (Theme issue: investigating change and variation
through dialect contact), 1-15.
--; and Milroy, James (1992). Social network and social class:
toward an integrated sociolinguistic model. Language in Society 21,
1-26.
MKDC (Milton Keynes Development Corporation) (1990). Milton Keynes
Population Bulletin 1990.
Orton, Harold; Dieth, Eugen; and Wakelyn, Martin (1968). The Survey
of English Dialects, Vol. 4: The Southern Counties. Leeds: E. J. Arnold.
Patrick, Peter (2002). The speech community. In Handbook of
Language Variation and Change, J. K. Chambers, Peter Trudgill, and
Natalie Schilling-Estes, (eds.), 573-597. Oxford: Blackwell.
Scotton, Carol M. (1976). Strategies of neutrality: language choice
in uncertain situations. Language 52, 919-941.
Torgersen, Eivind N. (1997). Some phonological innovations in
southeastern British English.
Unpublished cand.philol, thesis, Department of English, University
of Bergen.
--; and Kerswill, Paul (2004). Internal and external motivation in
phonetic change: dialect levelling outcomes for an English vowel shift.
Journal of Sociolinguistics 8 (1), 24-53.
Trudgill, Peter (1999). Norwich: endogenous and exogenous
linguistic change. In Urban Voices. Accent Studies in the British Isles,
Paul Foulkes and Gerard Docherty (eds.), 124-140. London: Arnold.
--(2004). New-Dialect Formation." The Inevitability of
Colonial Englishes. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Wells, J. C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Williams, Ann; and Kerswill, Paul (1997). Investigating dialect
change in an English new town. In Issues and Methods in Dialectology,
Alan Thomas (ed.), 46-54. Bangor: Department of Linguistics, University
of Wales, Bangor.
--; and Kerswill, Paul (1999). Dialect levelling: change and
continuity in Milton Keynes, Reading and Hull. In Urban Voices. Accent
Studies in the British Isles, Paul Foulkes and Gerard Docherty (eds.),
141-162. London: Arnold.
PAUL KERSWILL AND ANN WILLIAMS
Lancaster University
University of Wales, Bangor
Received 3 December 2002 Revised version received 5 March 2004
Table 1. Key to (ou): the fronting of the offset of /[??]u/,
as in GOAT
Variant Score Comment
[O:], [OU] 0 northern and Scottish realizations
[[??]u], [[??][??]] 1 older Buckinghamshire and London
[[??]y] 2 fronting
[[??]I] 3 fronting and unrounding
Table 2. Index scores for (ou): children and caregivers from the
southeast of England
Child age (on) index: (on) index: Standard
group southeastern children with deviation
caregivers southeastern (children)
caregivers
4-year-olds 1.47 1.52 0.4887
8-year-olds 1.44 1.80 0.3545
12-year-olds 1.31 1.70 0.3124
Child age Significance [r.sup.2]
group (paired
t-test)
4-year-olds p > .05 .355 (p < .01)
8-year-olds p < .01 .045 (p > .05)
12-year-olds p < .001 .23 (p > .05)
Range of index: 0-3
Table 3. Key to (u:): the fronting of /u:/ as in GOOSE
Variant Score
u 0
Y 1
[y.bar] 2
Table 4. Index scores for (u:): major subgroups
Index
Caregivers .60
Girls .55
Boys .37
Table 5. Index scores for (u:): child subgroups
Girls Boys
4-year-olds .31 .42
8-year-olds .48 .27
12-year-olds .86 .43
Range of index: 0-2
Table 6. Percentage use of variants of (au) (MOUTH), Milton Keynes
working class, interview style
[[epsilon][??]] [[epsilon]I]
Survey of English Dialects (SED) [check]
informants, 1950-1960s
(Orton et al. 1968)
Elderly (2f, 2m) 63.2 25.6
Women age 25-40 (n = 48) 0 0
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) (8) 0 0
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 0
[[epsilon]:] [a:[??]]
Survey of English Dialects (SED)
informants, 1950-1960s
(Orton et al. 1968)
Elderly (2f, 2m) 9.8 0
Women age 25-40 (n = 48) 11.7 17.2
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) (8) 0 5.9
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 12.3
[aeu] [au]
Survey of English Dialects (SED)
informants, 1950-1960s
(Orton et al. 1968)
Elderly (2f, 2m) 1.2 0
Women age 25-40 (n = 48) 38.6 31.5
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) (8) 4.7 88.8
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 3.8 83.1
Table 7. Percentage use of variants of (au) (MOUTH), Reading
working class, interview style
[[epsilon][??]] [[epsilon]I]
SED informants [check]
Elderly (2f, 2m) 53.5 38.1
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 2.3
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 3.8 3.2
[[epsilon]:] [a:[??]]
SED informants
Elderly (2f, 2m) 3.3 0
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 8.0
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 5.7
[ae[u]] [au]
SED informants
Elderly (2f, 2m) 4.1 0.7
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 90.4
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 87.1
Table 8. Percentage use of variants (at) (PRICE),
Milton Keynes working class, interview style
[[??]I] [[??]I] [aI]
Elderly age 70-80 (2f, 2m) 0 0 24.4
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 25.4 44.6 29.2
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 1.0 38.0 60.0
[[??]I] [[??]I] [[??]I]
Elderly age 70-80 (2f, 2m) 56.6 15.3 3.4
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 0.5 0 0
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 0 0 0
Table 9. Percentage use of variants (at) (PRICE), Reading working
class, interview style
[[??]I] [[??]I] [aI]
Elderly age 70-80 (2f, 2m) 0 12.4 47.8
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 2.8 21.2 45.1
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 0.6 19.1 63.7
[[??]I] [[??]I] [[??]I]
Elderly age 70-80 (2f, 2m) 21.8 1.7 15.7
Girls age 14/15 (n = 8) 21.1 4.3 5.1
Boys age 14/15 (n = 8) 13.7 2.7 0
Table 10. Birthplace of Reading teenagers and their parents
Working class
Teenagers Mothers Fathers
% born in Reading 93.7 81.2 81.2
Middle class
Teenagers Mothers Fathers
% born in Reading 62.5 26.7 11.8
Table 11. Birthplace of Milton Keynes teenagers and their parents
Working class
Teenagers Mothers Fathers
% born in MK 50.0 12.5 13.3
Middle class
Teenagers Mothers Fathers
% born in MK 26.7 0 6.7