The structure of the German vocabulary: edge marking of categories and functional considerations(1).
WIESE, RICHARD
Abstract
That phonology can signal the boundaries of constituents has been
well known since Trubetzkoy identified the delimitative function of
phonology. In this paper, it is argued that such edge marking may be
much more systematic than observed in earlier work. In particular, the
stress domains (foot, word, compound, phrase) and the morphological
categories (root, stem, word) of German systematize their edge marking
by a repeating alternation in moving from left to right (stress) or by a
switch in feature values for [consonantal] (root, etc.). For the latter
preference, quantitative evidence is provided. Such alternating patterns
of edge marking are argued to be highly functional. Ways of treating
these phenomena by means of a metaconstraint in optimality theory are
discussed in the final section.
Introduction: the relevance of the "delimitative
functions" or "boundary signals"
In the beginning of phonological theorizing, Trubetzkoy expressed
the far-reaching hypothesis that the phonological subsystems of
languages were by no means restricted to a system of phonemic contrasts
between segments. Besides the distinctive function of sounds carried by
phonemes, Trubetzkoy (1967) identified two further functions, which he
called delimitativ and gipfelbildend `cumulative'. In this paper, I
discuss the role of the so-called delimitative function in phonology,
with the aim of arguing that it is actually more central than Trubetzkoy
could envisage. Second, a treatment of these regularities in terms of
optimality-theoretic constraints will lead to the conclusion that
constraints enter specific relations with each other, hitherto
unnoticed.
The quote in (1) makes it clear that Trubetzkoy, although he
clearly saw the importance of phonological delimitation, still claimed
that the distinctive function has priority and is the only one for which
a language has a logical necessity. In other words, phonology is there
to distinguish German Tisch `table' from Fisch `fish'. In
addition, it may (but need not) signal where units start and end in a
stream of such units.(2)
(1) Trubetzkoy's notion of delimitative functions
"Diese zwei Schallfunktionen, die distinktive und die
delimitative, mussen streng unterschieden werden. Die distinktive
Funktion ist fur die Sprache als solche unentbehrlich: die einzelnen
Schallkomplexe, die den Bedeutungseinheiten entsprechen, mussen
unbedingt verschieden sein, damit sie nicht verwechselt werden; (...)
Dagegen ist die auBere Abgrenzung der bedeutungsgeladenen Schallkomplexe
gar nicht unbedingt notwendig. Diese Komplexe konnen in einem
ununterbrochenen RedefluB ohne jede Andeutung ihrer Grenzen aufeinander
folgen" (Trubetzkoy 1967: 241-242).(3)
In the following, I will try to combine the insight from
Trubetzkoyan phonology with analytical tools provided by optimality
theory: linguistic units may, to a large extent, be signalled by marking
their left and/or right edges. More specifically, it is postulated that
roots, stems, and words in German are much more pervasively signalled by
rather concrete phonological properties than is generally assumed, even
by Trubetzkoy. Furthermore, when moving from one linguistic level to the
next in a hierarchy of linguistic categories, we often find a switch in
the value of the feature under discussion, the features being segmental
and/or prosodic. This observation, if correct, bears consequences for a
theory of constraints, such as optimality theory: constraints are
mutually constrained.
I present two types of evidence for these claims. First, it will be
argued (as in Wiese 2000 [1996]: 311) that there is indeed a general
pattern such that linguistic units are consistently marked on the edges,
and that the direction of marking reverses from left to right and back
again, when we go through the relevant hierarchy. This is demonstrated
by looking, in section 1, at the stress patterns of German. Disregarding
cases interpreted as exceptional or marked in the various analyses, the
basic generalization is that the main stress switches in its orientation
from one linguistic level to the next. In the main part of this article,
in section 2, I argue that German roots, stems, and words are
surprisingly systematically marked by a contrast of [consonantal] vs.
its absence at edges of roots, stems, and words.
1. From right to left in stress patterns
German is a stress-timed language, which, even if this
classificatory term has no further function, makes it easy to identify
stress patterns at various linguistic levels. There are numerous studies
of stress in German (see the thorough survey by Jessen 1999), which
disagree in many details. However, it is clear that the units given in
(2) are all relevant for the description of stress. In other words, we
need to talk about COMPOUND stress, because there is good evidence that
it differs systematically from, say, PHRASAL stress and WORD stress.
(2) Stress domains:
Phrase
Compound
Word
Foot
Next, we need to identify the basic stress pattern of the units
given in (2). The generalizations are not always uncontroversial, but in
most cases, a rather clear picture emerges. As shown in (3), German has
left-strong feet, which are sometimes seen as syllabic (Giegerich 1985;
Hall 1992; Wiese 2000 [1996]), and sometimes as moraic (Alber 1998; Fery
1998). The major point under debate thus is the precise nature of the
foot: the foot is left-strong. Moving up the hierarchy, we find words
consisting of several feet, like those in (3b). It turns out that it is
the final (rightmost) foot in these cases that bears word stress. There
are problematic cases for this analysis, for example in trisyllabic
words such as Sansibar, Talisman, etc., but the generalization still
seems to be valid. It partly explains that word stress is predominantly
on the final or the penultimate syllable, and that word stress is always
found on one of the three final syllables. Strong units are italicized
in (3).
Turning to compounds, we find that binary compounds ([A B])
predominantly have initial stress, again with exceptions for which no
completely satisfactory analysis exists (see, e.g., Giegerich 1985;
Benware 1987). Finally, phrases predominantly bear final stress, at
least if not under special focus conditions. The classical stress rules
for English, the compound rule and the nuclear stress rule of SPE (Chomsky and Halle 1968: chapters 2, 3) apply equally well (or badly) to
German.(4)
(3) Stress directionality in German:
a. Feet:
[Hau ser], [Ko nig] (syllables within syllabic feet)
[La ti] [tu di] na [ris] mus (morae within feet)(5)
b. Words:
[Symme trien], [Uni versi taten] (feet within [phonological] words)
c. Compounds:
[Fahr zeug], [Streich holz] (words within compounds)
d. Phrases:
[Ottos Geburtstag], [alte Freunde],
[heute abend], [auf dem Tisch] (words within phrases)
The crucial and, to my knowledge, new observation here is that
stress alternates between the right and the left edge from one stress
unit to the next higher or lower one, a generalization summarized in
(4). This alternation is completely systematic, only perturbed by the
various exceptions and provisos mentioned above with respect to the
individual stress patterns.
(4) Accentual units and orientations of main stress:
Phrase [right arrow] right
Compound [right arrow] left
Word [right arrow] right
Foot [right arrow] left
Stress was discussed by Trubetzkoy as an instance of the
"delimitative function" of phonology. Presupposing
stress-relevant structures that are always or at least preferably
binary, stress can only highlight either the right or the left edge of
the domain. If stress is edge-related in this way, it serves to mark
boundaries of units. Consider now a stress system in which stress for
all units is uniform with respect to its left/right orientation. Such a
system would not be very efficient in terms of its delimitational power:
edges would be indicated through stress, but there would be no way of
expressing the TYPE of unit whose edge is indicated by stress. The
functional logic of a system such as the German one is now obvious: in
contrast to the hypothetical system just discussed with uniform stress
marking, the type of unit is in fact quite consistently indicated
through the position of its main stress. If a hearer encounters a
linguistic form of the shape [a B] (with capitalization indicating main
stress), she can conclude that the form is either a phrase or a word,
but not a compound. (The conclusion may be wrong, but it will constitute
a good guess.)
One might speculate whether the pattern can be extended to lower as
well as higher categories. The next lower category below the word is the
syllable. Remarkably, in an analysis of the syllable using a strong-weak
labelling for its constituents, Kiparsky (1979) assigns to the syllable
a weak--strong pattern, such as
[[[onset.sub.w]--[rhyme.sub.s]].sub.[Sigma]]; see also Giegerich (1985).
This is once again a switch in direction from the next higher level.
Furthermore, as a reviewer of this paper points out, it is possible to
say that the next higher level from the phrase is the clause (in the
sense of the main verb and its arguments). But it has been well known
since Kiparsky (1966) that the stress pattern of such structures is
left-strong, as in ein Buch lesen `read a book', auf den Tisch
klopfen `knock on the table'. The issue of stress patterns here is
difficult (see Cinque 1993 for a syntax-oriented approach), but
arguably, there is yet another switch in stress directionality involved
here.
The conclusion from these bird's-eye observations on stress
patterns is that the stress rules of German refer to each other and do
not seem to be independent of each other, although they are always
stated in this way. I am not aware of any analysis claiming that, for
example, the phrasal stress rule puts main stress on the final
constituent because the compound rule behaves in the opposite way.
The term "stress rule" in the preceding paragraphs is
intended to be used in a pretheoretical sense. Stress regularities can
be expressed by a number of different means. Within optimality theory,
they are expressed by means of standard constraints, in particular those
that align the prominent daughters (heads) with the left or right edge
of their respective mother node (see Prince and Smolensky 1993; McCarthy
and Prince 1993a, 1993b, and numerous subsequent literature). A
description of German stress by means of constraints is not the topic of
the current paper, but see Alber (1997, 1998) and F6ry (1998) for
modelling German word stress in OT. I return to a constraint-based
description of edge marking in section 3 below.
One advantage of using a constraint-based framework for the point
under discussion, namely the switch in left-right orientation over a
hierarchy of categories, may be that this aspect is not mandatory. That
is, the observation made for German is not without counterexamples in
other languages. For Finnish, for example, it seems that both feet and
words are left-strong (see Hanson and Kiparsky 1996; Alber 1997). In
other words, the left-to-right alternation noted for German and argued
to be a highly functional organization of stress patterns is not
universal. In terms of optimality theory, a system of violable constraints may be responsible for the situation found. At the same
time, optimality theory makes a prediction here: the Finnish-type,
one-sided edge marking over adjacent categories can only arise if
enforced by some higher-ranked constraint. I can only speculate at this
point that a preference for a marking of initial edges is higher than
that of marking final edges. The preference for the marking of left
edges (as opposed to right edges) makes sense in terms of speech
processing, which is generally left-to-right.
2. From vowel to consonant in lexical categories
Pursuing the idea that such edge marking of units with a pervasive
reversal of values within a hierarchy is inherent in the linguistic
system (of German), I will, second, argue that other categories are
similarly marked at their edges, and that units that are vertically
adjacent in a hierarchy are marked with the opposite value of some
feature. In particular, I present some results of a study in which
roots, stems, and words are analyzed and their edge marking is under
scrutiny. The study presupposes an arrangement of these categories as in
(5), with recursion allowed for the stem (see, e.g., Selkirk 1982;
McCarthy and Prince 1993a, 1993b).
(5) Morphological categories:
Word
Stem
Root
A second theoretical background assumption of the study is the
proposal made in direct optimality theory (Golston 1996a) that the
representation of any linguistic unit is identical to the set of
constraints it violates; see (6). According to direct OT, there is no
need to see the (partial) representation of linguistic units as separate
from the set of constraints that are violated for the particular
linguistic unit. In this conception, a particular constraint may be
obeyed by a large number of particular entities, but it can still be
violated by a minority of them, and violations of this constraint are
part of the phonological representation of each of these entities
(simple or complex).
(6) Direct OT (Golston 1996a):
Morphemes are represented by the constraints they violate.
It turns out that the constraints used in standard OT are so varied
and manifold that violations of them can be used to specify morphemes:
classes of linguistic forms and even individual items can be
characterized as specific sets of constraint violations. This makes
standard representations redundant since constraint violations, which
are routinely used to evaluate forms, can also be drawn upon to
represent the very same forms. This is the central claim of direct OT,
one I will make use of in the following.
Constraints thus penalize lexical items for their violations of
particular edge-marking constraints (just as for any other violation of
constraints). For present purposes, it is hypothesized that roots are
marked by initial and final consonants. In other words, a root beginning
or ending in a vowel violates an alignment constraint that requires the
opposite. Therefore, such a root (take See `sea') is represented
(partially) by this particular constraint violation; see (7). The
display in (7) is nothing but a specific elaboration of the bipartite
Saussurean sign: a combination of a meaning (abbreviated as SEE) with a
form (the constraint violation).
(7) Partial representation for See:
ALIGN-R (root, [cons.]) SEE
2.1. Roots
With this conception of linguistic representation providing the
basic analytical tools, Golston and Wiese (1998) look at the edge
marking for roots in German. On the basis of a computer-readable,
independently derived root dictionary of German (compiled on the basis
of the database of roots by Ortmann [1993] containing 6,512 native
roots), they find the results displayed in (8).
(8) Edge marking of roots (from Golston; Wiese 1998: 178-179):
Consonant Vowel
no. % no. %
Left edge 6147 94 365 6
Right edge 6268 96 244 4
The result of this quantitative survey is that there is a very
strong tendency to mark both edges of a German root by the feature
[consonantal]. That is, the two constraints ALIGN-L/R (Root, [cons.])
are obeyed by almost all roots. The minority of roots that violate one
of the two constraints are characterized by exactly this constraint
violation and are in this respect more marked than the unmarked cases
(see [9]). Note that only the first result (left edge marked by
consonants) is possibly reducible to the onset requirement of syllables,
but the second result (consonant at right edge) is in fact unexpected by
syllabic requirements. A typical (and good) root such as Tisch
`table' is a bad syllable in that it is a closed syllable.
Expressed in terms of standard OT constraints, vowel-initial roots
are represented (inter alia) by a violation of (9b), and vowel-final
roots by a violation of (9a). As seen in (8), only a minority of native
German roots incur these constraint violations.
(9) OT constraints for roots (Golston and Wiese 1998):
a. ALIGN-R (root, [cons.])
b. ALIGN-L (root, [cons.])
The tendency to mark root boundaries by a consonant is actually not
restricted to German but is widespread in typologically diverse
languages. It is even found for languages that have a strong preference
for open syllables, as seen in the Indo-European or the Bantu languages.
(For Indo-European roots, see the dictionary by Watkins [1985].) Bantu
roots (radicals) are also predominantly consonant-final (see Meeussen
1967; de Blois 1975: 72f.), though Bantu syllables strongly prefer to be
open.
To take up a completely different language, Olawsky (1999: 95)
presents relevant statistics from the language Dagbani, a Niger-Congo
language spoken in Ghana. Olawsky collected 512 nominal roots of
Dagbani, of which 71% end in a consonant, although the preference for
open syllables is very strong in Dagbani.
2.2. Stems
If roots are marked by [consonantal], what about stems? Stems
dominate roots but are less easily identified on the basis of a corpus.
While roots are identifiable as some sort of minimal or core morpheme,
and while words are the minimal freely occurring units, no such
minimality condition appears to work for stems, which are intermediate
constituents (see [5]). For current purposes, I therefore rely on
existing linguistic descriptions of the stem in general and the stem in
German in particular.(6)
If roots are marked by final consonants, stems should be marked by
nonconsonants, according to the functional considerations of this study.
As a first piece of evidence that this might be the case, I suggest that
theme vowels of many languages are primarily stem markers. Their main
function is to sort the roots of the language into a set of classes in
order to make them available for further derivational and inflectional
morphology. Thus, Latin and some of the Romance languages display
precisely the edge marking of stems by vowels predicted by the present
account. The main point in the present context is that I am not aware of
their unpredicted counterparts, namely "theme consonants." But
if most languages have more consonants than vowels, why should these not
exist? The hypothesis that roots tend to end in consonants, while stems
tend to end in vowels, predicts the nonexistence of theme consonants:
there is no functional reason to give stems a consonantal ending.
Matters change, of course, once we are dealing with a more specific
inflectional morpheme carrying other information. In this case,
grammarians seem not to use the term "theme vowel/consonant."
As for German, it is possible to argue that final schwa is a stem
marker. This proposal was first suggested by Wurzel (1970), who
interprets final schwa in most of its occurrences as a STEM marker. In
(10), the final subrule is one instance in a complex rule for noun-stem
formation. In other words, noun stems are often marked by the vowel
schwa in final position.(7) In a way, final schwa is a theme vowel,
assigning a substantial number of German nominal roots to a particular
inflectional class. The linguistic analysis of words such as Glaub-e
`belief', Lieb-e `love', Ros-e `rose', Lug-e `lie'
would thus be that they contain a root (consonant-final!) plus a
stem-marker schwa, to the effect that these words constitute a noun
class. Other stems (not in this class) are identical to roots and thus
violate a constraint requiring a stem to end in a vowel. The first two
subrules in (10) are further stem-formation rules, regulating more
specific noun classes (with fewer members). Clearly, the schwa-insertion
rule in the final line is the default rule with respect to the two
preceding ones. While the first subrule (adding /r/) derives specific
forms, namely the plural of a particular (small) class of nouns, the
second one requires specific morphological features that are not needed
for the schwa-insertion rule.
(10) Noun-stem formation rule (Wurzel 1970: 47)
[ILLUSTRATION OMITTED]
The number of such schwa-final nouns (noun stems according to the
present analysis) is large in present-day German. A count over the CELEX database of German words (Baayen et al. 1993) reveals the following: of
4,174 monomorphematic German nouns found in this database, 1,034 end in
schwa, among them many relatively recent assimilated loan words such as
Orange `orange' or Fassade `facade'. On the other hand, only
152 of these nouns end in [en].(8) That is, while this type of schwa
still occurs in a minority of cases, the minority is sizable. It must
also be taken into account that this ending is productive only with
feminine nouns.
I draw the conclusion from this independent description that stems
of German tend to end in a vowel, namely schwa in the more general case,
and that it is one of the major functions of the German schwa in final
position to provide stems with a final vowel.(9)
For further evidence that this interpretation of final schwa might
be correct, we may note how nominal inflectional markers for nouns are
treated by Wurzel in his account. As shown in (11), there are
inflectional markers (Flexive), which are always ordered after the
Stammbildungselement (stem-formation element). I interpret this ordering
as being related to the categories of stems and words: stem-formation
elements are attached to stems (hence the name), inflectional markers
are attached outside of those, at the word level. Examples in the final
column of (11) contain both stem-formation elements (r, n) and
inflectional markers (n, e, plus s from the preceding column).(10)
(11) Difference betweeen stems and words (Wurzel 1970: 49):
ohne Affix Stammbildungs- Flexiv Stammbildungselement
element und Flexiv
kind ?? ?? - kind ?? + s kind + r + n
das Kind des Kindes den Kindern
bar ?? ?? bar + n ?? - bar + n + e
der Bar des Baren die Baren
- stras + e ?? - stras + n + n
die StraBe den StraBen
- end + e ?? - end + n + e
das Ende die Enden
tank ?? ?? - tank ?? + s -
der Tank des/die Tanks
Note now that the inflectional markers are always consonants --
with the exception of e, which requires some discussion. Wurzel
postulates a final schwa (again written as e) for some of the nominal
paradigmatic classes, but this segment is always deleted afterward. That
is, the surface form of assumed bar + n + e is invariably [b?? Ren)
(Baren). In other words, to postulate this final schwa as an
inflectional marker is in spirit with the theoretical approach of the
time (SPE framework) but is in strong contrast to the surface structure
and therefore highly dubious on theoretical grounds. A unit is first
introduced and then deleted without a trace (similarly for + n + n in
the final column). If we therefore neglect this item, we can conclude
that the set of word-related inflectional markers postulated in Wurzel
(1970) for the German noun system invariably consists of consonants,
specifically /s/ and /n/.
2.3. Words
We have now moved upward from the stem to the word and have seen
some very tentative evidence that the words might be marked by
consonants in inflectional paradigms of nouns. But for words, further
independent evidence for the claim is available. I suggest that proper
nouns (names) are distinct from common nouns in that they are always
words, but not stems or roots. The evidence for this is morphological:
names cannot be inflected in the way common nouns can be. In accordance
with the analysis of inflectional suffixes above, which separates
stem-related suffixes from word-related ones, it is indeed the case that
proper names can only be suffixed by the word-related suffixes, namely
genitive singular, or plural /s/, see Peter-s (gen. sg. or pl.).
Furthermore, we can ask again how the edges of names are marked by
looking at a corpus of such names. As one first result, in (12) the
number of vowel-final and consonant-final second names in an electronic
telephone directory for the city of Rostock containing more than 50,000
names is tabulated.(11)
(12) Edge marking of words: last names
Consonants Vowels
no. % no. %
Right edge 42421 85.2 7352 14.8
The result is that a large majority of last names (85%) end in a
consonant. Before discussing the relevance of this finding, a second set
of figures can be added: (13) presents a quantitative summary for a list
of German first names, this time from a smaller database found on the
internet.
(13) Edge marking of words: first names
Consonants Vowels -- total Vowels -- a
no. % no. % no. %
Right edge 194 63.8 110 36.2 69 22.7
Here, the result is blurred somewhat by the fact that many first
names for females end in -a which can actually be seen as a (borrowed)
suffix deriving female names, such as Martin-Martin-a, Josef--Josef-a,
and numerous others. Subtracting the female names ending in -a from the
total, the result is that there are only 13.5% of vowel-final names.
Again, the observation is confirmed: names are predominantly
consonant-final.
However, these results from given names and family names must be
held against some consideration of what would be expected by chance.
Suppose, in a first approximation, that consonants and vowels are
distributed randomly over words, constrained only by hard phonotactic
constraints, such as the effect of the German rule of final devoicing,
that is, without any preferences for particular sounds or classes of
sounds. If this distribution of consonants vs. vowels should be
indistinguishable from those found in (12) and (13) above, we would have
no reason to argue that words preferably end in consonants. Finding such
a chance distribution is not trivial, but one possible approach is
presented here. One possible count (presented in [14]) then reveals that
14 possible consonantal phonemes can appear word-finally, while 13
possible vowel phonemes can appear in the same context. Two affricates,
/pf/ and /ts/, were counted separately here, as well as three
diphthongs. The two major classes of phonemes excluded are the voiced
obstruents and the short, lax vowels, neither of which occur
word-finally according to most descriptions. Alternative classifications
of phonemes are always possible, of course, but would not dramatically
change the picture. In particular, if we add both classes of
nonoccurring sounds, namely voiced obstruents and short vowels, the
situation is basically the same again.
(14) Chance distribution (phonemes in word-final position):
Consonants Vowels
no. % no. %
14 51.9 13 48.1
A possible objection to this way of calculating a chance
distribution is that the types and qualities of consonants and vowels
(and thus their respective numbers) are in fact irrelevant. This would
lead to the simpler null hypothesis that words could end in either an
open syllable (i.e. final vowel) or a closed syllable (i.e. final
consonant). With this binary split (a word can only end in a closed or
in an open syllable), final consonants and vowels would have a
completely even probability of occurring. For German, this distribution
is in fact very close to the one found in (14), and again, it does not
match the factual situation.
The empirical results reported above for first and last names are
expected not by the chance distribution based on the numbers of
consonants and vowels, nor by the chance distribution of open and closed
syllables, nor by the requirements of syllabic well-formedness, which
would give vowel-final words a clear majority. The general picture found
in the data is again surprisingly uniform and can be sketched as in
(15): for the right edge, there is a switch from consonant to vowel and
back to consonant when we move from roots to stems to words in German.
(15) Edge marking of categories:
Word [right arrow] Consonant
Stem [right arrow] Vowel
Root [right arrow] Consonant
As in the case of roots and stems, we can easily state
optimality-theoretic constraints to express the findings of words; see
(16) for one possible attempt. Of these, the left-alignment constraint
in (16b) was not justified above. However, as there is no mechanism at
all, in either the phonology or the morphology of German, that shows a
tendency to form initial vowels, we may actually assume for the time
being that it holds.
(16) OT constraints for words:
a. ALIGN-R (word, [cons])
b. ALIGN-L (word, [cons])
The alignment schema for constraints provides an obvious mechanism
for the description of all the facts enumerated so far. More important,
however, is the functional view of phonological structures made possible
by these results: phonological features mark the boundaries of
linguistic units and thereby fulfill the demarcative function first
proposed by Trubetzkoy (see [1]). It must be added, though, that the
proposal does not imply that words are identical to roots. This is
obviously not the case: if roots are generally monosyllabic, words tend
to be polysyllabic. Also, the word-final consonants are often a subset
of those found for roots (see Golston 1996b on Sanskrit and Greek).
The description of rightmost edge marking for roots and stems given
above allows for a straightforward prediction: suppose it is true that
roots are generally marked with a consonant at their right edge, as the
evidence strongly suggests. Constraint (9a) then holds. If, in addition,
the switch from consonant to vowel in moving up the morphological
hierarchy to the stem is the preferred move, then we have all the
grounds needed for a prediction: we should be able to find evidence for
a constraint (17a), but there could be no constraint (17b). In fact,
constraint (17a) is something like a derived constraint, one that does
not need to be stated independently, while (17b) cannot be thus derived.
(17) Predictable and unpredictable constraints
a. ALIGN-R (stem, [-cons])
b. ALIGN-R (stem, [cons])
There is the additional aspect that the constraint in (17a) is not
even statable if a feature model with a unary (not binary) feature
[consonantal] is adopted. Suppose that only [consonantal] exists, but
[-consonantal] does not. Introducing a katathetic vowel for stems could
not count as a violation of feature insertion since no such feature is
available. Is there evidence that the predictions just made ([17a] is
either a derived constraint or unstatable, while [17b] is disallowed)
are true? As we have seen in the preceding section, theme consonants do
not seem to exist, while theme vowels are widespread. (In the
perspective of the present paper, noun-final schwa in German may well be
called a theme vowel.) Furthermore, a notion developed in the minimalist
theory of Chomsky (1995) may be used to ensure that not all the
structural categories available in morphology are actually present.
Consider a simple, and consonant-final, root of German and its
structure, as in (18).
(18) Minimal structures for simplex roots
Word
Stem
Root
[ti]
While the structural categories presented here for this item (root,
stem, word) are those admitted by the system introduced in (5), it is
possible that not all of them are actually needed. In particular, an
intermediate node such as stem that does not branch may well be pruned
from the structure (see Ito and Mester 1998: section 5 for a similar
argument with respect to German compounds). In other words, for the
simplex word Tisch only the root and the word are visible, and
constraints referring to the stem are irrelevant, as this category is
invisible or virtually nonexistent. If such a view can be defended, the
need for linguistic items to obey all constraints (an impossible
achievement anyhow) is greatly reduced.
3. Constraints on constraints
There is a more general conclusion to be derived from the analyses
presented in this paper: constraints as conceptualized in optimality
theory are not necessarily independent of each other.(12) Rather, the
existence of one constraint can impose a heavy restriction on the
existence of some other constraint. This interaction of constraints
within the grammar will be discussed in this final section.
Note first that the findings summarized in (4) for stress and (15)
for consonants and vowels at morphological edges can be captured
somewhat abstractly by a principle of contrast. I propose a formulation
of such a principle in (19), assuming that such a principle is indeed
more general than our two examples have shown. As contrast is surely one
of the most deeply rooted notions in phonology, such a principle makes a
lot of functional sense.(13)
(19) Principle of contrast (CONTRAST-P):
For any two categories A and B that are syntagmatically or
paradigmatically adjacent:
If category A bears a phonological property P, B should NOT bear P.
Crucially, the formulation of CONTRAST-P generalizes over
structures that are either syntagmatically or paradigmatical
(hierarchically) arranged. Therefore, we note that this principle is
akin to a notion well known in recent phonology, namely the principle of
obligatory contour (OCP; see Leben 1973; McCarthy 1986; Odden 1986, and
others). The OCP demands contrast, or, rather, disallows the absence of
contrast; see the schematic example for a situation disallowed by the
OCP in (20). This constraint says that two units A, B should not bear
the feature [Labial] twice, at least not if the two units are adjacent
on their tier and are not parts of different linguistic units.
(20) OCP as an instantiation of CONTRAST-P:
*A B
[Lab] [Lab]
The close similarity between the two German cases of paradigmatic
contrast discussed above and the OCP is only blurred by the fact that
OCP cases typically involve syntagmatic relations (like the syntagmatic
adjacency between A and B in [20]), whereas CONTRAST-P, see (19),
generalizes to all types of adjacency, syntagmatic or paradigmatic.
But what is the status of the principle formulated in (19)? Several
options are available: CONTRAST-P could be a metaconstraint, a principle
regulating the relationship between two or more constraints, or it could
be a standard constraint, of the OCP type, or it could be something
completely different. What I would like to suggest here is that the
current evidence suggests that a metaconstraint is indeed operative
here.(14) Consider again the possible OCP constraint disallowing two
adjacent labial segments (or other structures). What the formulation in
(20) hides is the fact that each of the two labial elements is already a
constraint violation. In optimality theory, each occurrence of a feature
in a representation counts as the violation of the constraint militating
against its occurrence, in the most general version the constraint
*STRUC. That is, an OCP constraint such as (20) already contains
reference to two independently needed constraints. More generally,
CONTRAST-P, (19), refers to two independent constraints, which of course
may entertain different rankings in the language-specific hierarchy.
(21) is an attempt to reformulate CONTRAST-P in this fashion.
(21) Principle of contrast as a metaconstraint
CONTRAST-P (CAT-A, P; CAT-B, NON-P)
Again, given that the marking of category A by some phonological
property P is expressed by means of a constraint (schema), the
constraint militating against the marking of category B by the same
property is a metaconstraint regulating the relationship between two
constraints. Furthermore, the formulation in (21) adopts the view that
metaconstraints must be included into the set of constraints that are
operative in a grammar.
Ito and Mester (1998) propose to interpret the OCP by means of more
elementary, independently needed, mechanisms. They develop a
formalization of the OCP in terms of constraint conjunction in the sense
of Smolensky (1995). For example, the ban against two labials as in (20)
is expressed, according to their proposal, as the local conjunction of
the constraint against the feature labial with itself, crucially within
a given domain (morpheme, word, etc.). The cases discussed in the
present paper are different as they deal with dissimilarity BETWEEN
domains. I do not see at present how these cases can be expressed via
local constraint conjunction. Rather, a treatment in terms of a
metaconstraint seems necessary.
The right-to-left switch observed for German stress patterns can
equally well be captured by a principle of contrast. In current OT,
edge-related stress is captured by an alignment of strong elements
(heads) to a left or right edge of their mother constituent; see (22)
for a constraint schema of this sort, with PRC as a variable over
prosodic constituents (cf. EDGEMOST; Prince and Smolensky 1993).
(22) L/R alignment
ALIGN (PRC, L/R, HEAD (PRC), L/R)
The CONTRAST-P, (21), would require that the value for the edge
parameter in this schema is not identical for any two vertically
adjacent constituents. Left and right will alternate, as seen above for
German stress.
4. Conclusions
The main point of this paper is that left-right asymmetries (see
stress) and vertical asymmetries (see the switch in feature values
between morphological levels) are pervasive. The presupposition is that
the lexical categories as outlined in (2) for stress domains and in (5)
for morphological domains exist to give structure to the (German)
vocabulary. The more structure these categories give to the lexical
items, the less the vocabulary is just a long list of arbitrary items.
One empirical conclusion from the studies presented above is that
lexical categories in German are rather strongly signalled by boundary
signals, in particular by consonants and vowels. This fact can easily be
overlooked in linguistic frameworks that have no means of integrating
quantitative evidence of the type adduced above. But once a more direct
connection between markedness and representation is made, as in direct
OT, patterns emerge. It should be added here that the patterns found for
German do not seem at all restricted to this language. That roots of
Indo-European languages generally behave in this way ([cons] initially
and finally) is rather well known; that it is also true for
typologically or genetically distant languages such as Bantu languages
is less obvious, but equally true.
In the present bird's-eye studies of German stress and of edge
marking for morphological or lexical categories, we have seen that
phonology is functional and delimitative, to a larger extent than the
functional view of Trubetzkoy and Jakobson could foresee. A wider
perspective for these considerations would consider whether phonemic
representations, which are implicitly or explicitly conceived as strings
of segments, can be completely reformulated by means of notions of
delimitation. In direct OT, such a move is quite natural. As Golston
(1996a) demonstrates, a representational account relying solely on
constraint violation is possible and does away with most, if not all, of
the linear order of phonemic representations as strings of segments. In
any case, a representational theory replacing arbitrary strings of
segments by edge marking can express the functional significance of the
asymmetries noted in this paper more directly.
Notes
(1.) The ideas expressed in this paper were first presented at a
colloquium celebrating the 60th birthday of Dieter Wunderlich in
Dusseldorf. I also thank audiences at colloquia in Marburg and Santa
Cruz, California, for their input, and Birgit Alber and Susanne
Niedeggen-Bartke, Marburg, and Chris Golston, Fresno, for their
contributions. Finally, two anonymous reviewers helped to improve this
paper. Correspondence address: Institut fur Germanistische
Sprachwissenschaft, Philipps-Universitat Marburg, D-35032 Marburg,
Germany. E-mail: wiese@mailer.uni-marburg.de.
(2.) Trubetzkoy's phonology can be characterized as a
functional approach largely because it identifies the three
above-mentioned functions of sound structure and views sound structure
in relation to these functions. The cumulative function is completely
ignored in the following.
(3.) "These two functions of sound, the distinctive and the
delimitative, must be strictly distinguished. The distinctive function
is a necessary ingredient of language: the individual complexes of sound
that correspond to units of meaning must of necessity be kept distinct
from each other so that they will not be confused. (...) On the other
hand, the external boundaries of meaningful complexes of sounds are not
in any way necessary. It is possible for these sound complexes to follow
each other in an uninterrupted flow of speech without any hint at their
boundaries" (translated by the author).
(4.) I assume that the existence of the categories in (3) is
established prior to (and partly independent of) the stress rules.
Otherwise, as a reviewer suggests, there would be the danger of
circularity: units and stress rules motivate each other. While one of
the main points to be made here is that stress alternates because of its
function of marking linguistic units of a particular type, stress is not
absolutely necessary to differentiate between, for example, compounds
and phrases. This is witnessed by the fact that exceptions exist in both
directions: German has right-strong compounds and left-strong phrases.
The distinction between such compounds and phrases can still be made,
for example on the grounds of separability on the syntactic level. In
general, there is a (hopefully converging) set of syntactic,
morphological and phonological regularities for the existence of the
units under discussion.
(5.) Example taken from Alber (1998: 117).
(6.) Very little discussion can be found on the stem category in
German. Neef (1996: section 2.2) uses the stem in a sense different from
the one used here, namely as the formal representation of any lexical
item.
(7.) The vowel is given as /e/, but there is a further rule in this
account reducing this vowel to schwa. In general, Wurzel's account
is very much within the spirit of SPE (Chomsky and Halle 1968). My
argument is based on the assumption that a number of insights given in
this work hold independently of the formal framework in which it is
couched.
(8.) The search was performed over the so-called stem lexicon in
CELEX. The resulting lists of forms reveal some errors (for example with
respect to segmentation in morphemes). As these errors probably dc, not
introduce a bias, they were not corrected.
(9.) There is a long-standing uncertainty whether to treat final
schwa in German nouns as a morpheme or not. Wurzel's proposal
basically is that it is a minimal morpheme marking nothing but the
category of its base as a noun stem.
(10.) It is not clear to me why the stem marker r (as in kinder,
`child, p1.') is not listed in the second column of (11).
(11.) Data were taken from the computer-readable telephone
directory D-Info 1996. Given that the names are in orthographic form,
there are some uncertainties involved, for example for foreign names.
However, the quantitative impact of these problematic cases is
negligible. Counting was in terms of tokens and not types here, in
contrast to the immediately following data.
(12.) Of course, in OT, constraints also interact with each other
in that they enter constraint ranking. In this other sense, they are
never independent of each other.
(13.) There is another principle of contrast, as used by Clark
(1987, 1993) and Carstairs-McCarthy (1994), which postulates that any
two forms should differ in meaning. The present principle, instead,
requires contrast WITHIN a single form. This is not to say that there
may not be a deeper connection between the two observations.
(14.) McCarthy and Prince (1995: 364-365) introduce another
example. They argue for a metaconstraint in which the faithfulness of
roots is higher ranked than the faithfulness of affixes. The task of
this metaconstraint is to fix the, otherwise variable, ranking between
two constraints. In the type of metaconstraint discussed here, the
function of the metaconstraint is different: it fixes feature values in
an otherwise open constraint schema.
References
Alber, Birgit (1997). Quantity sensitivity as the result of
constraint interaction. In Phonology in Progress -- Progress' in
Phonology, Geert Booij and Jeroen van de Weijer (eds.), 1-45. HIL
Phonology Papers III. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics.
--(1998). Stress preservation in German loan words. In Phonology
and Morphology of the Germanic Languages, Wolfgang Kehrein and Richard
Wiese (eds.), 113-141. Tubingen: Niemeyer.
Baayen, Harald R.; Piepenbrock, Richard; and van Rijn, H. (1993).
The CELEX Lexical Database [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data
Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
Benware, Wilbur A. (1987). Accent variation in German nominal
compounds of the type (A(BC)). Linguistisehe Berichte 108, 102-127.
Carstairs-McCarthy, Andrew (1994). Inflection classes, gender, and
the principle of contrast. Language 70, 737-788.
Chomsky, Noam (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
--; and Halle, Morris (1968). The Sound Pattern of English. New
York: Harper and Row.
Cinque, Guglielmo (1993). A null theory of phrase and compound
stress. Linguistic Inquiry 24(2), 239-297.
Clark, Eve (1987). The principle of contrast: a constraint on
language acquisition. In Mechanisms of Language Acquisition, Brian Mac
Whinney (ed.), 1-33. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
--(1993). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
de Blois, Kornelis F. (1975). Bukusu Generative Phonology and
Aspects of Bantu Structure. Tervuren: Musee Royal de L'Afrique
Centrale.
Fery, Caroline (1998). German word stress in optimality theory.
Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics 2, 101-142.
Giegerich, Heinz J. (1985). Metrical Phonology and Phonological
Structure. German and English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Golston, Chris (1996a). Direct optimality theory: representation as
pure markedness. Language 72(4), 713-748.
(1996b). Prosodic constraints on roots, stems and words. In
Interfaces in Phonology, Ursula Kleinhenz (ed.), 172-193. Berlin:
Akademie-Verlag.
--; and Wiese, Richard (1998). The structure of the German root. In
Phonology and Morphology of the Germanic Languages, Wolfgang Kehrein and
Richard Wiese (eds.), 165-185. Tubingen: Niemeyer.
Hall, Tracy Alan (1992). Syllable Structure and Syllable-Related
Processes in German. Tubingen: Niemeyer.
Hanson, Kristin; and Kiparsky, Paul (1996). A parametric theory of
poetic meter. Language 72(2), 287-335.
Ito, Junko; and Mester, Armin (1998). Markedness and word
structure: OCP effects in Japanese. Unpublished manuscript, Santa Cruz.
Jessen, Michael (1999). Word stress in Germanic languages: German.
In Word Prosodic Systems in the Languages of Europe, Harry van der Hulst
(ed.), 515-545. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Kiparsky, Paul (1966). Uber den deutschen Akzent. Studia Grammatica
7, 69-98.
--(1979). Metrical structure assignment is cyclic. Linguistic
Inquiry 10, 421-441.
Leben, William R. (1973). Suprasegmental phonology. Unpublished
Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.
McCarthy, John J. (1986). OCP effects: gemination and
antigemination. Linguistic Inquiry 17, 207-263.
--; and Prince, Alan S. (1993a). Prosodic Morphology I. Constraint
Interaction and Satisfaction. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science.
--; and Prince, Alan S. (1993b). Generalized alignment. Yearbook of
Morphology 1993, 79-153.
--; and Prince, Alan S. (1995). Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in
Linguistics 18: Papers in Optimality Theory, Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh
Dickey, and Suzanne Urbanczyk (eds.), 249-384. Amherst, MA: Graduate
Linguistics Student Association.
Meeussen, Alfred E. (1967). Bantu grammatical reconstructions.
Africana Linguistica III, Ann. Ling. 61, 79-121.
Neef, Martin (1996). Wortdesign. Eine deklarative Analyse der
deutschen Verbflexion. Tubingen: Stauffenburg.
Odden, David (1986). On the role of the obligatory contour
principle in phonological theory. Language 62, 353-383.
Olawsky, Knut (1999). Aspects of Dagbani Grammar. With Special
Emphasis on Phonology and Morphology. Munich: Lincom Europa.
Ortmann, Wolf Dieter (1993). Kernmorpheme im Deutschen. 4561
allgemeinsprachliche deutsche Morphemgestalten im Vergleich zwischen
zehn Quellen. Munich: Goethe-Institut.
Prince, Alan S.; and Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory.
Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
University. Boulder: University of Colorado.
Selkirk, Elisabeth O. (1982). The Syntax of Words. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Smolensky, Paul (1995). On the internal structure of the constraint
component Con of UG. Unpublished manuscript.
Trubetzkoy, Nikolaj S. (1967). Grundzuge der Phonologie, 2nd ed.
G6ttingen: Vandenhoeck and Ruprecht.
Watkins, Calvert (1985). The American Heritage Dictionary of
Indo-European Roots. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Wiese, Richard (2000 [1996]). The Phonology of German. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Wurzel, Wolfgang Ullrich (1970). Studien zur deutschen
Lautstruktur. Studia grammatica 8. Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
RICHARD WIESE Philipps University, Marburg
Received 11 July 1999 Revised version received 13 September 2000