Does teaching enhance research in economics? A few more thoughts.
Siegfried, John J.
William Becker and Peter Kennedy have assembled a series of
entertaining anecdotes about ways teaching has complemented the research
of successful scholars. The examples are numerous, varied, and
interesting. There is no doubt that the research productivity of many
serious scholars has been stimulated by their teaching responsibilities.
There is a danger, however, of drawing unwarranted conclusions from
these interesting vignettes. The successful scholars surveyed by Becker
and Kennedy may be individuals of such innate curiosity and creativity
that they are stimulated by virtually anything and everything. These may
be people, to steal unabashedly from the title of Dan Hamermesh's
(2004) wonderful little book, who see economics everywhere. If that is
the case, the role of teaching in their research cannot be distinguished
from the roles of reading, listening, watching, pondering, eating,
drinking, smelling, singing, running, golfing, driving, and perhaps even
dreaming.
The individuals surveyed by Becker and Kennedy may have been
stimulated at least as much, or perhaps even more, by spending time in
informal discussions with colleagues, scanning the New York Times and
Wall Street Journal, viewing CNN and C-SPAN, or gazing out the window of
their office, but Becker and Kennedy did not ask them how they perceived
the research productivity of discussing, reading, watching, and looking.
These may be people, like Hamermesh, who, if he spent a week at the zoo,
probably soon would be writing about the economics of butterfly beauty,
the labor supply of elephants, and the economics of bear hibernation.
A control group with a level of curiosity and imagination similar
to those surveyed would be needed to tease out the distinctive effect of
teaching on research. Matching curiosity and imagination across
individuals would be challenging because there is no accepted measure of
curiosity or imagination. It might be reasonable to assume that
attributes like curiosity and imagination remain constant in individuals
across time and then to compare the stimulation successful scholars
received from different working environments--some teaching and some
not--they have experienced throughout their careers. The experiences of
Herb Grubel and Peter Lloyd reported by Becker and Kennedy suggest the
potential value of asking economists who have spent a considerable
amount of time in environments both with and without teaching
responsibilities, say at a university and at a research think tank, to
reflect on which portions of their careers were more intellectually
productive, and whether they believe the productivity differences were
caused by their environment.
One cynical interpretation of the survey results is that they show
that there are smart, exciting, and intellectually curious students at
Harvard, Yale, MIT, and Stanford, where many of those surveyed work. Of
course, Becker and Kennedy understand this as thoroughly as the rest of
us. The interesting question, however, is whether the same scholars,
were they not to have enjoyed the privilege of teaching the highest
quality students in the world, would have had as productive research
careers as they did while rubbing shoulders with the best students in
the world. If not, one wonders if teaching is sufficiently valuable that
the scholars would have been willing to pay for the privilege of
associating with their students?
Becker and Kennedy find that successful scholars believe teaching
has enhanced their research. This evidence, however, is insufficient to
answer some relatively important questions about the relationship
between teaching and research productivity. Becker and Kennedy's
evidence shows that teaching is an input into some research success.
But, does teaching always lead to successful research? I doubt it. If we
find, as I suspect would be true, that sometimes teaching enhances
research, whereas at other times it does not, the interesting question
then devolves to the frequency with which teaching enhances research. On
that question we remain in the dark, because the survey (intentionally)
was not random. It did not include teachers who have not been successful
scholars.
Another important question is whether successful scholarship can be
produced without any exposure to teaching, that is, is teaching an
essential input for producing scholarship? If teaching is not an
essential input, what are substitutes for it and how much do they cost
relative to teaching? In other words, what is the relative scholarly
marginal productivity of an hour's worth of teaching vis-a-vis an
hour spent pursuing other activities that might also complement
research?
A bit unexpected is the way in which the survey respondents seemed
to uniformly interpret teaching to mean undergraduate teaching. Most
respondents work at universities with large economics Ph.D. programs. It
is surprising, therefore, that there is so little mention from the
respondents of cross-fertilization between Ph.D. thesis supervision and
faculty research, especially so since the knowledge and level of
analysis conducted by Ph.D. students is closer to that undertaken in
faculty research than is the content of typical undergraduate
instruction.
Finally, I offer a category of how teaching enhances scholarship
that is not mentioned in the paper, namely, the future research
collaboration between scholars and their students. Several examples
illustrate the productivity of such connections: Robert Shiller and his
former student, John Campbell; Edward Prescott and his former student,
Finn Kydland; James Tobin and his former student, William Brainard.
Scholarly progress resulting from a collegial relationship initiated
through student-teacher contact deserves no less acclaim than the
numerous happy and productive marriages between former students and
their teachers.
Reference
Hamermesh, Daniel S. 2004. Economics is everywhere. New York:
McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
John J. Siegfried, Department of Economics, Box 1819-B, Vanderbilt
University, Nashville, TN 37235, USA. Received March 2005; accepted May
2005.