Effects of message sensation value in bird flu TV stories on audience arousal and perception of story quality.
Wei, Ran ; Zhou, Shuhua
Although human beings are characterized as voracious information
seekers, not all of us are "informavores" (Dennett, 1991).
Research shows that individuals use mass media and attend to media
messages for a variety of reasons and to fulfill very different needs
(Blumler & Katz, 1974). With an explicit focus on message exposure
as need-fulfillment, the activation theory of information exposure
(Donohew, Lorch, & Duncan, 1980; Donohew, Lorch, & Palmgreen,
1998) underscores the importance of the need for stimulation in
motivating individuals to attend to media messages. The thesis is that
motivation to message exposure is driven by the need for stimulation
rather than cognitive need for information alone (Donohew, 1990).
Specifically, the theory proposes that sensation-seekers tend to view
media stimuli to satisfy their desire for stimulation or arousal (Frankenberger, 2004; Stephenson, 2002). The generalization is that high
arousal messages gain attention and is preferred by high-sensation
seekers when attending to a message (Stephenson & Southwell, 2006).
The activation theory of information exposure was applied mostly in
researching substance abuse among youth. The focus was on testing what
kind of media campaign messages, such as public service announcements
(PSAs), are most effective in persuading high-sensation seekers from
using drugs. In this study, the activation theory of information
exposure was applied in a different context--health communication about
the threat of a bird flu pandemic in the United States.
The threat of a bird-infected flu known as H5N1 (avian flu)
pandemic has garnered attention from the news media in the past few
years. Since 2003, a total of 407 people in 15 Asian African and
European countries were infected; more than half of them (254) died.
Over 200 million birds died or were destroyed. Just a couple of months
into 2009, four deaths were reported in China. Fearing the virus may
mutate and cause human-to-human infections, the World Health
Organization (WHO) warns about a potential avian flu pandemic worldwide.
In such a case, the pandemic will possibly kill 51 to 81 million people
(WHO, 2009). Facing the threat, former President Bush unveiled a $7.1
billion emergency plan on November 2, 2005 to make antiviral drugs available for more Americans. By October 2008, US international support
to fight the pandemic totaled $949 million (US Department of Health and
Human Services, 2009). Thus, bird flu news stories provide a desirable
context because the threat of a bird flu pandemic to America is real.
In past activation research, a great deal is known about the
relations between message sensation value and sensation-seeking on
outcome measures such as attention, arousal, fear, and behavior
intention (Donohew, Palmgreen, & Duncan, 1980; Everett &
Palmgreen, 1995; Lorch et al., 1994; Palmgreen, Donohew, Lorch, Hoyle,
& Stephenson, 2001), but relatively little is known about the
relations between message sensation value and viewer perception of media
messages. Scholars have raised the question whether message sensation
value in media messages may distract target audiences in communicating
health threats (Stephenson & Southwell, 2006). They have a concern
that some audience members may be stimulated by arousing messages but
fail to get the message. Unfortunately, there is no research to address
this concern to date.
To fill the gap, the dependent measures of message sensation value
were expanded in this study by including perception of news stories.
That is, how stories with varying levels of message sensation value in
bird flu TV news stories were evaluated by the target audience. The
perception of the stories focused on the dimensions of clarity,
understandability, and professionalism. Practically, little is known
about Americans' preparedness for a bird flu pandemic. How do
Americans process bird flu news? Will they view bird flu as a serious
threat to America after viewing the bird flu news stories? And will they
likely seek information to get prepared as a result of exposure to the
stories? Findings will provide valuable information for public health
agencies in policymaking.
The Activation Theory of Information Exposure and Hypotheses
As mentioned earlier, the activation theory of information exposure
holds that, when attending to a message, one seeks to satisfy one's
need for stimulation and information. But, one will seek to fulfill the
need for stimulation prior to the need for information (Donohew,
Palmgreen, & Duncan, 1980). The theory is grounded in the assumption
that individuals have levels of arousal at which they are most
comfortable. It further proposes that a message will likely attract an
individual's attention if the message provides optimal arousal to
satisfy his or her need for stimulation (Donohew, 1990; Donohew, Lorch,
& Palmgreen, 1998). Kroeber-Riel (1979) defined activation as part
of human behavior that elicited arousal and inner tension to enable
"psychological and motor activity of the organism (p. 241)."
Conceptually, the need for stimulation is a kind of personal trait like
sensation-seeking (Christ, 1985; Donohew, 1990). Sensation seeking,
according to Zuckerman (1979), is, "a trait defined by the need for
varied, novel, and complex sensations and experiences, and the
willingness to take physical and social risks (p. 10)." Christ
(1985) proposed that arousal need is one of the psychological mechanisms
which motivates individuals to process information.
Therefore, the activation theory of information exposure is
concerned with audiences' psychological processes; it also focuses
on audience characteristics (e.g., differences in need for arousal) and
message features (e.g., stimuli) in producing effective media messages
(Cappella, 2006; Donohew, 1990). Empirically, Donohew, Palmgreen, and
Duncan (1980) examined the relationships among needs for activation,
importance of information, discrepancy, and effects of that information.
Using Pearson's Novelty Experiencing Scale as a measure of arousal
need, they found that among participants with a high need for
activation, unimportant information in newspapers produced a negative
effect; but, information on an important newspaper story, that simulated
subjects to meet their activation needs, generated a positive effect.
Lorch and her colleagues (1994) conducted an experiment to examine
the effects of message and program sensation value, sensation seeking,
and drug use on visual attention to televised anti-drug PSAs. The
experiment examined adolescents. Results showed that attention to an
anti-drug PSA was higher if the PSA was placed in a program context
where the sensation value of the program was optimal for subjects (both
high-sensation seekers and low-sensation seekers). Applying the
activation theory to an anti-cocaine campaign, Everett and Palmgreen
(1995) investigated the effectiveness of sensation seeking, message
sensation value in PSAs, and program sensation value to free/cued recall
of messages. They also investigated attitudes toward cocaine and
behavioral intention to use cocaine. Results demonstrated that
high-sensation seekers appeared to be more affected by the PSAs'
high-sensation value than low-sensation value; they tended to recall
more, have more negative attitudes to cocaine, and self-reported less
likelihood to try cocaine. Lowsensation seekers were more affected by
the PSAs' low-sensation value message. They also recalled more, had
a negative attitude toward cocaine, and indicated a less likelihood to
use cocaine. Recent studies (Lang, Schwartz, Chung, & Lee, 2004;
Morgan et al., 2003; Palmgreen et al., 2001; Palmgreen, Donohew, Lorch,
Hoyle, & Stephenson, 2002) show new evidence that PSAs, with
high-sensation value targeting teen high-sensation seekers, would be
effective in drug prevention.
The review of the literature provided the grounding based on which
the following hypotheses were formulated:
H1: Participants would pay more attention to bird flu TV news
stories with high message sensation value than those with low message
sensation value.
H2: Participants would register more arousal to bird flu TV news
stories with high message sensation value than those with low message
sensation value.
H3: Participants would report greater fear to bird flu TV news
stories with high message sensation value than those with low message
sensation value.
H4: Participants think that a bird flu outbreak would affect more
lives in response to bird flu TV news stories with high message
sensation value than to those with low message sensation value.
H5: Participants would report higher behavioral intention in
response to bird flu TV news stories with high message sensation value
than those with low message sensation value.
Finally, message sensation value in broadcasting news stories has
the potential to contribute to higher ratings. It will be interesting to
study if, and how, stories with varying levels of message sensation
value affect participants' perception of the stories. A research
question was raised:
RQ: Will participants perceive differently bird flu TV news stories
with high message sensation value from those with low message sensation
value?
Method
This study employed a repeated measure 2 (MSV) x 2
(Sensation-seeking) x 4 (stories), within subject factorial design, to
test the hypotheses. The three factors of this experiment were message
sensation value (high and low), sensation-seeking (high, low), and four
bird flu TV news stories, which were held as a repeated measure. Message
sensation value is a within-subject factor, whereas sensation-seeking is
a between-subject factor. The advantage of this mixed design was to rule
out topic influence by having all participants viewing all four stories.
In addition, two presentation orders were created to randomize primacy
and recency effects.
Participants
A number of bird flu TV news stories were acquired from CNN PathFire. All of them focused on bird flu as a public health threat.
Four stories were chosen based on the varying level of five factors:
graphic effects, emotional intensity, unusualness, dramatic impact, and
sound effect. Two stories were judged to be high in message sensation
value, whereas, the other two were low in message sensation value. To
ensure successful stimuli production, manipulation checks were conducted
using a separate pool of participants from the main study. A total of 46
undergraduates evaluated the stories using the 17-item perceived message
sensation value scale (Palmgreen, Stephenson, Everett, Baseheart, &
Francies, 2002). T-tests revealed that message sensation value between
the two sets of stories was significant (t=35.52, p < .01). Stories
considered to be high in sensation value were rated significantly higher
(M=4.20, SD=.62) than stories considered to be low in sensation value
(M=3.62, SD=.69). (1)
For the experiment, 54 participants were recruited from a large
Southeast university research participant pool. Participants in this
study ranged from 18 to 42 in age, with a mean of 21 years. In terms of
gender, 35% of the participants were male, and 63% were female, with one
person's gender data missing.
Procedures
Upon arrival, participants were greeted and taken into the lab.
They were told that they would be completing an experiment about how
people process news stories. The procedures for measuring heart rate and
skin conductance were explained at this point. All participants were
given informed consent statements to read and sign. The participants
completed the 40-item SS (Sensation-seeking) scale designed to measure
predisposition toward sensation seeking. The participants then watched
the four featured CNN TV news stories about bird flu, ranging in
duration from two minutes and 20 seconds to two minutes and 41 seconds.
Physiology data was collected while the participants watched. Following
each news story, the stimulus and physiology data collection was
stopped. Participants filled out self-report measures of story
perception, fear, perceived effects, valence, and arousal, as well as
behavioral intention in response to the story just viewed. When
participants were ready to move to the next story, physiological data
collection began again. This procedure was repeated for all four
stories. After participants completed measures for all four stories,
they were thanked, debriefed, and dismissed. Physiological data was
recorded with the BioPac MP150 modular data system. ECG (electrocardiogram) and skin conductance or electrodermal activity (EDA)
input was amplified by the MP150 and fed into a Dell Inspiron 1150
laptop computer. Heart rate was measured initially as milliseconds per
beat, and then transformed into average heart rate per second. Skin
conductance was collected as an analog signal with a sample rate of 20
times per second.
Dependent Measures
Attention
Attention was measured by heart-rate monitoring of cardiac
deceleration. Heart rate can reveal details about a subject's
attention and cognitive states, as the heart is under the control of the
sympathetic and parasympathetic systems of the autonomic nervous system.
When one pays attention, the parasympathetic system slows down the
heart. In this study, cardiac deceleration was the basis for measuring
short-term changes in attention to stimuli. Arousal
Arousal was measured by self-reported data using the pictorial SAM
(self-assessment mannequin) scale (Lang, Dhillon, & Dhong, 1995), as
well as with skin conductance data (Hopkins & Fletcher, 1994). SAM
is a three-dimensional, pictorial, emotional-rating scale with nine
rating points. Participants rated their emotional responses in terms of
arousal (calm-excited), valence (positive-negative) and dominance
(small-in control). Only the arousal dimension was used. Electrodermal
activity (EDA) in test participants was used to measure arousal in
response to a stimulus (Stern, Ray, & Quigley, 2001). Skin
conductance has been recorded in numerous psychophysiological studies
(Stern et al., 2001), dating as far back as research conducted by Carl
Jung in 1907. Changes of skin conductance in participants occur as they
respond to the stimuli and the variation is in direct proportion to the
message's intensity of emotion. Conductance variation in amplitude
exceeding .5 uS (micro Siemen) was counted as a response.
Fear
Four items using a 7-point semantic differential scale were created
to measure fear. The items measured whether the participant felt
overwhelmed, fearful, uneasy, and apprehensive, after watching the
story. Reliabilities (Cronbachs' [alpha]) for the scales across the
four stories ranged from .87 to .91, with an average of .89. For each
story, responses were therefore summed and divided by five to create a
mean response for each participant. In addition, a single item was also
included in the study to gauge participants' fear of how serious a
bird flu pandemic would be.
Story Perception
Eight items using a 7-point semantic differential scale were
created to measure participants' perception of bird flu TV news
stories. The items measured whether the story was appealing, clear,
understandable, enjoyable, stimulating, credible, engaging, and
informative, with a low score representing low perception of the story.
Reliabilities (Cronbachs' [alpha]) for the scales across the four
stories ranged from .76 to .88, with an average of .82. For each story,
responses were therefore summed and divided by eight to create a mean
response for each participant.
Behavioral Intention
Four, 7-point, Likert-type items were created to measure
participants' behavioral intention. The four items were, "I
need to learn more about bird flu," "I am going to pay close
attention to bird flu," "I am going to do everything possible
to help my community prepare for bird flu," and "I am going to
do everything possible personally to stay off the pandemic."
Reliabilities (Cronbach'[alpha]) for the scales across stories were
rather high, ranging from .82 to .88, with an average of .85. For each
story, responses were therefore summed and divided by four to create a
mean response for each participant.
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Results
To test the hypotheses, six, two-way, repeated ANOVA tests were
performed. H1 predicted that participants would pay more attention to
bird flu TV news stories with high message sensation value than those
with low message sensation value. The main effect for attention was
significant, F (1, 52) = 5.77, p < .05, [[epsilon].sup.2] = .10 (see
Figure 1). Participants did pay more attention to bird flu TV news
stories high in message sensation value, as indicated by lower heart
rates (M = 73.67, SD = 1.42) than to those lower in message sensation
value (M = 76.89, SD = 2.4). H1 was supported.
H2 predicted that participants would register more arousal to bird
flu TV news stories with high message sensation value than those with
low message sensation value. Arousal was inferred from physiological
measurement of skin conductance. The main effect for arousal was
significant, F (1, 52) = 4.57, p < .05, [[epsilon].sup.2] = .08.
Participants were more aroused when watching stories higher in message
sensation value, as indicated by the higher number of skin conductance
responses (M = 9.12, SD = 1.02), than watching those lower in message
sensation value (M = 7.38, SD = 0.83). Self-reported arousal as measured
by SAM, however, did not triangulate the physiological responses: F (1,
52) = 3.67, p = .06, [[epsilon].sup.2] = .06. Participants'
reported arousal when watching the stories high (M = 9.12, SD = 1.02)
and low (M = 7.38, SD = 0.83) in message sensation value did not differ
significantly, even though it was approaching significance. H2 was
partially supported. (2)
H3 predicted that participants would report greater fear of bird
flu with TV news stories of high message sensation value than those with
low message sensation value. The main effect for fear was significant, F
(1, 52) = 35.73, p < .01, [[epsilon].sup.2] = .40. Participants were
more apprehensive of bird flu after watching stories higher in message
sensation value (M = 3.90, SD = 0.15) than after watching those lower in
message sensation value (M = 3.20, SD = 0.13). H3 was supported.
H4 proposed that participants would think the bird flu would affect
more lives in response to bird flu TV news stories with high message
sensation value than to those with low message sensation value. The main
effect for such perception was significant, F (1, 52) = 12.00, p <
.01, [[epsilon].sup.2] = .19. Participants considered bird flu would
affect many lives in response to stories higher in message sensation
value (M = 4.23, SD = 2.21) than after watching those lower in message
sensation value (M = 3.70, SD = 2.22). H4 was supported.
H5 predicted that participants would report higher behavioral
intention in response to bird flu TV news stories with high message
sensation value than those with low message sensation value. The main
effect for behavioral intention was significant, F (1, 52) = 5.17, p
< .05, [[epsilon].sup.2] = .09. Participants intended to learn more
about and better prepare for bird flu after watching the stories high in
message sensation value (M = 4.40, SD = 0.17) than after watching those
low in message sensation value (M = 4.16, SD = 0.17). H5 was supported.
Finally, the RQ asked if participants would perceive differently
bird flu TV news stories with high message sensation value from those
with low message sensation value. The main effect for perceptions was
significant, F (1, 52) = 66.03, p < .01, [[epsilon].sup.2]2 = .56.
Participants reported better and more positive perception of stories
high in message sensation value (M = 5.24, SD = 0.65) than those low in
message sensation value (M = 4.57, SD = 0.43). The perceptions included
clarity, understandability, credibility, enjoyability, engagement, and
informative ness.
Discussion and Conclusion
This investigation applied the activation theory of information
exposure to understand how message sensation value in the public health
context affected the effectiveness of the news stories about the bird
flu pandemic. All five of the hypotheses were supported. Message
sensation value seemed to be the key factor influencing participants
processing of the stories. It had main effects on all of the five,
dependent, outcome measures, including attention, arousal, fear,
apprehension about the massive impact of a bird flu outbreak, and
behavioral intention.
More importantly, new research ground was broken in the current
study. Some scholars are concerned that message sensation value may be a
distraction in communicating public health threats to target audiences
(Stephenson & Southwell, 2006). Audience perception of stories was
incorporated as a dependent measure (e.g., perception of the clarity,
understandability, and professionalism of the stories), and how it was
related to message sensation value was examined. Findings show that
message sensation value affected story evaluation in a positive fashion:
the higher the message sensation value, the more positive the perception
of the story in terms of clarity, understandability, credibility, being
enjoyable, engaging, and informativeness. Thus, message sensation value
is not a distraction in bird flu TV news stories, but a facilitator of
participants' processing. This finding contributes to the research
of activation theory of information exposure.
But the mechanism that explains how message sensation value, story
evaluation, and other message effects impact one another was not
examined in this study. For instance, does message sensation value
affect story perception, which in turn impacts arousal? Or, does message
sensation value affect arousal, which then impacts story perception?
Ascertaining the interrelationships among these variables is clearly a
new direction for future research.
The activation theory of information exposure explains how bird flu
news with message sensation value, a device in communicating public
health threats, has had effects on audiences in getting their attention
to the pandemic, being aroused, and fearing the impact of an outbreak.
In addition, the effect of message sensation value on subjects'
behavioral intention was found. After watching the bird flu news stories
that were high in message sensation value, participants are more likely
to get prepared. These findings suggest that Americans took the bird flu
threat seriously and would get prepared. News organizations play a
critical role in helping the American public prepare for bird flu
pandemic.
Findings of this study are subject to limitations of use of a
student sample, which was relatively homogenous and skewed toward
females. Future research can expand the current study to a larger sample
drawn from the general public, which will likely provide more
appropriate data to test the interaction effect between the message
sensation value in bird flu news stories and the sensation-seeking trait
of subjects. In addition, more news stories from varied sources can be
used to replicate the current study.
References
Blumler, J., & Katz, E. (1974). The uses and mass
communications: Current perspectives on gratifications research. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Cappella, J. (2006). Integrating message effects and behavior
change theories: Organizing comments and unanswered questions. Journal
of Communication, 56, 265-279.
Christ, W. (1985). The construct of arousal in communication
research. Human Communication Research, 11, 575-592.
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little,
Brown & Co.
Donohew, L. (1990). Public health campaigns: Individual message
strategies and a model. In Eileen B. Ray and Lewis Donohew (Eds.),
Communication and health: Systems and applications (pp. 136-152).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Donohew, L., Lorch, E., & Palmgreen, P. (1998). Applications of
a theoretical model of information exposure to health interventions.
Human Communication Research, 24, 454-468.
Donohew, L., Palmgreen, P., & Duncan, J. (1980). An activation
model of information exposure. Communication Monographs, 47, 295-303.
Everett, M., & Palmgreen, P. (1995). Influences of sensation
seeking, message sensation value, and program context on effectiveness
of anti-cocaine public service announcements. Health Communication, 7,
225-248.
Frankenberger, K. (2004). Adolescent egocentrism, risk perceptions,
and sensation seeking among smoking and nonsmoking youth. Journal of
Adolescent Research, 19, 576-590.
Hopkins, R., & Fletcher, J. E. (1994). Electrodermal
measurement: Particularly effective for forecasting message influence on
sales appeal. In A. Lang (Ed.), Measuring psychological responses to
media messages (pp. 113-132). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Kroeber-Riel, W. (1979). Activation research: Psychobiological
approaches in consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 5,
240-250.
Lang, A., Dhillon, K., & Dong, Q. (1995). The effects of
emotional arousal and valence on television viewers' cognitive
capacity and memory. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 39,
313-327.
Lang, A., Schwartz, N., Chung, Y., & Lee, S. (2004). Processing
substance abuse messages: Production pacing, arousing content, and age.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media48(1), 61-88.
Lorch, E, Palmgreen, P., Donohew, L., Helm, D., Baer, S., &
D'Silva, M. (1994). Program context, sensation seeking, and
attention to televised anti-drug public service announcements. Human
Communication Research, 20, 390-412.
Morgan, S., Palmgreen, P., Stephenson, M., Hoyle, R., & Lorch,
E. (2003). Associations between message features and subjective
evaluations of the sensation value of antidrug public service
announcements. Journal of Communication, 53(3), 512-526,
Palmgreen, P., Donohew, L., Lorch, E., Hoyle, R., & Stephenson,
M. (2001). Television campaigns and adolescent marijuana use: Tests of a
sensation seeking targeting. American Journal of Public Health, 91,
292-296.
Palmgreen, P., Donohew, L., Lorch, E., Hoyle, R., & Stephenson,
M. (2002). Television campaigns and sensation seeking targeting of
adolescent marijuana use: A controlled time-series approach. In R.
Hornik (Ed.), Public health communication: Evidence for behavioral
change (pp. 35-56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Palmgreen, P., Stephenson, M., Everett, M., Baseheart, J., &
Francies, R. (2002). Perceived message sensation value (PMSV) and the
dimensions and validation of a PMSV scale. Health Communication, 14,
403-428.
Stephenson, M. (2002). Sensation seeking as a moderator of the
processing of anti-heroin PSAs. Communication Studies, 53, 358-380.
Stephenson, M., & Palmgreen, P. (2001). Sensation seeking,
message sensation value, personal involvement, and processing of
anti-drug PSAs. Communication Monographs, 68, 49-71.
Stephenson, M., & Southwell, B. (2006). Sensation seeking, the
activation model, and mass media health campaigns: Current findings and
future directions for cancer communication. Journal of Communication,
56, 38-56.
Stern, R., Ray, W., & Quigley, K. (2001). Psychophysiological
recording. New York: Oxford University Press.
US Department of Health and Human Services (2009). Pandemic
planning update VI. Retrieved February 11, 2009. Access:
http://www.pandemicflu.gov/plan/panflureport6.html
WHO (2009). Cumulative number of confirmed human cases of Avian
Influenza a/(H5N1) reported to WHO. Retrieved February 11, 2009, from
WHO. Access: http://www.who.int/csr/disease/avian_influenza/country/
cases_table_2009_02_11/en/index.html.
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level
of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Notes
(1) One story is about how the US would fare and respond if a bird
flu pandemic broke. The reporter put on a facemask and led his viewers
to the USDA's biggest lab in Ames, Iowa, where scientists were
working frantically to come up with a vaccine. Another story featured
Indonesia as the center of the pandemic, where more birds died than at
any other place in the world. These two stories were in the high message
sensation value level. At the low end of this manipulation were a story
about how a Florida community was preparing to round up chickens running
loose in the city, where residents argued that the city council is just
using bird flu as an excuse to get rid of the chickens. Another story
opened with a chicken vendor in Hong Kong complaining about a hard time
making a living because people were afraid of eating chickens; the story
wrapped up by praising Hong Kong as being the gold standard by WHO to
combat bird flu.
(2) Two factors might be at work here: the effect size of arousal
is small anyway, so the nuances may be lost between these two measures.
Physiological measures were more sensitive than self-report data, as
participants may have a hard time evaluating their emotion. This issue
can be further explored in follow-up methodological studies.
Ran Wei, University of South Carolina
Shuhua Zhou, University of Alabama
Correspondence:
Ran Wei, Ph.D.:
Professor
School of Journalism & Mass Communications
University of South Carolina
Columbia, SC 29208
Tel: (803) 777-5762; Fax: (803) 777-4103
E-mail: wei2@mailbox.sc.edu
Shuhua Zhou, Ph.D., Associate Professor
Department of Telecommunication & Film
College of Communication & Info. Sciences
University of Alabama
Tuscaloosa, AL 35487-0152
Office: (205) 348-8653; Fax: (205) 348-5162
E-mail: szhou@bama.ua.edu