Attitude toward brand: an integrative look at mediators and moderators.
Najmi, Manoochehr ; Atefi, Yashar ; Mirbagheri, Seyed Alireza 等
INTRODUCTION
Delving into the processes through which advertising efforts
stimulate consumers to buy a particular brand has been an area of
interest among marketing researchers for a long time (MacKenzie, Lutz,
& Belch, 1986). In this way, attitude, the cynosure of social
psychologists, has been a cornerstone of consumer research as well. In
most studies in this area, attitudes have served as dependent variables
and the impact of different ads, their repetition, and other factors on
attitude formation and change have been studied (Berger & Mitchell,
1989). Cognitive perspective of attitude was the dominant viewpoint in
the years when consumer behavior was a fledgling body of knowledge,
owing much to Fishbeinian theories and multi-attribute models (e.g.
Fishbein, 1963). However, the pioneering works of Mitchell and Olson
(1981) and others (such as Zajonc, 1980; Shimp, 1981) gave rise to a new
stream of research in which alternative paths to persuasion were
explored. The influential Elaboration Likelihood Model (Petty, Cacioppo,
& Schumann, 1983) and the series of studies by Lutz, MacKenzie, and
Belch (1983), MacKenzie, Lutz, and Belch (1986), and MacKenzie and Lutz
(1989) further expanded our knowledge of attitude formation and change
to include peripheral paths, especially through attitude toward the ad.
While some studies were carried out to probe into the nature of this new
construct (e.g. Gresham & Shimp, 1985), purely affective influences
of advertisements on attitudes also came to light (Batra & Ray,
1986; Edell & Burke, 1987).
However, attitude researchers have focused on selected areas
developing the now voluminous attitude literature in different and even
disconnected directions. Along with a need for integration, current
theories include relationships between some constructs that do not
effectively explain the nature of effects or neglect the role of
moderator variables. Argument quality, for example, has been noted in
many studies (e.g. Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983; Lord, Lee,
& Sauer, 1995) in terms of its direct effect on brand attitudes;
however the mediating role of brand cognitions has not received worthy
attention. The moderating role of irrelevant thoughts on Arg-[C.sub.b]
relationship and the importance of availability of cognitive resources
in moderating that relationship are also less accentuated. Our study
aims at providing an integrative framework that encompasses both
cognitive and affective determinants of brand attitudes in the field of
advertising effectiveness. This framework also includes the different
processes and constructs that become activated as a result of emotional
versus informational ads, a comparison neglected in previous studies. We
developed our framework from a more detailed model which we propose
later in the paper and provided several propositions for future
validation.
In the following sections of this paper, first, we will discuss
attitude formation and change by distinguishing between attitude toward
the ad and attitude toward the brand. Antecedents and moderators of
these two constructs will be explained in depth. Subsequently, we will
discuss the relationship between attitude toward the ad and attitude
toward the brand, and finally, we will propose an integrative model
uncovering the processes which underlie the formation and change of
these two constructs.
ATTITUDE FORMATION AND CHANGE
There are two major perspectives with regards to attitude
structure. First, we can view attitudes as evaluative responses
influenced merely by beliefs (e.g., Wyer, 1970). This view of attitude
reached its pinnacle with the famous expectancy-value models of which
theory of reasoned action (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) is of paramount
importance. The well-known formula (A=[summation] [b.sub.i][e.sub.i])
indicates that the attitude is the sum of all evaluative beliefs
regarding the attitude object where [b.sub.i] is the consumer's
belief defined as the extent to which the object possesses attribute i,
and [e.sub.i] is the evaluation of attribute i.
Second, the three-component model of attitudes asserts that along
with beliefs (cognitive component), affective and behavioral components
also underlie attitudes (Maio, Esses, Arnold, & Olson, 2004). For
example, one might form a positive attitude towards classic music, in
that one believes that listening to this type of music will enhance
one's appreciation and understanding of music (cognitive part), it
reminds one's fond memories of the past (affective part), and
he/she remembers that he/she was an avid fan of (or at least used to
listen to) classic music in his/her youth (behavioral part).
Moreover, researchers discriminate between consumers' response
to marketing stimuli such as advertisement and their response to the
brand (Kirmani & Campbell, 2009). Shimp (1981) posits that audiences
of a particular ad have different degrees of involvement with
advertisement based on the degree of attention and the processing
strategy. Therefore, four types of attitude formation possibly arise
from processing of an ad (Shimp, 1981): (1) if both brand and non-brand
information of an ad are processed, both attitude toward the ad
([A.sub.ad]) and attitude toward the brand ([A.sub.b]) will be formed;
(2) if merely brand information of an ad is processed, only [A.sub.b],
and (3) if merely non-brand information of an ad is processed, only
[A.sub.ad] will be shaped; (4) if neither brand information nor
non-brand information is processed, no attitude will be formed.
Two kinds of appeals can be used to form the content of an ad
message (Keller, 2001; Belch & Belch, 2004): (1) Rational appeals
which focus on tangible aspects of brand such as physical product
attributes and benefits; (2) Emotional appeals which emphasize social
and psychological needs of customers and also focus on intangible
aspects of product such as user imagery, usage imagery, and brand
personality. Creating favorable attitudes toward the brand ([A.sub.b])
can be done by designing ads that influence beliefs and evaluations
regarding the desired outcomes of consuming the brand (Shimp, 1981).
Formation of favorable attitude toward the brand increases the
probability of trial or repetition of purchasing the advertised brand
(Shimp, 1981).
The objective of advertising is not to exert influence on
consumers' beliefs toward the attributes or benefits of a specific
brand per se. Instead, marketers are more fervently trying to create a
favorable attitude toward the advertisement in order to induce a
positive feeling in the consumers after processing the ad (Shimp, 1981).
It is widely accepted that feelings evoked by marketing communications
(e.g. advertisements) have important effects on consumers' response
to the brand (Kirmani & Campbell, 2009). We can, accordingly, assume
that the audience exposed to an advertising message builds up an
attitude toward the ad ([A.sub.ad]) which influences attitude toward the
brand ([A.sub.ab]), purchase intentions (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch,
1983), and consumers' buying behavior (Gresham & Shimp, 1985).
ATTITUDE TOWARD THE AD
"Attitude toward the ad" can be defined as the set of
thoughts and feelings consumers have about an ad (Kirmani &
Campbell, 2009); however, some researchers define it as consumer's
affective responses (such as likable-dislikable, favorable-unfavorable,
and interesting-uninteresting) to the ad itself (Lutz, MacKenzie, &
Belch, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986) during a particular
exposure time (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). Also, two different aspects
of attitude toward the ad, cognitive and emotional, can be recognized
(Shimp, 1981).
At least four potential antecedents of attitude toward the ad
([A.sub.ad]) have been identified (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983):
(1) ad cognitions ([C.sub.ad]), (2) attitude toward the advertiser
([A.sub.adv]), (3) the recipient's mood during the exposure, and
(4) peripheral cues (PC) (Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995). It was also
postulated in previous studies that attitude toward advertising in
general ([A.sub.ag]) exerts an automatic effect on [A.sub.ad] (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983), whereas empirical data showed that
[A.sub.ag] has no effect on [A.sub.ad] (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989).
Contrary to ad cognitions which affect [A.sub.ad] via central processing
route and require the consumer's elaborated cognitive processing of
the ad, the three remaining antecedents of [A.sub.ad] (i.e. [A.sub.av],
PC, and mood) affect [A.sub.ad] via peripheral processing route by the
simple transfer of affect instead of elaborated cognitive processing
(Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
AD COGNITIONS
It is widely accepted that ad cognitions, also called ad
perceptions, have a direct positive effect on attitude toward the ad
(Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986;
MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989; Brown & Stayman, Antecedents and
Consequences of Attitude toward the Ad: A Meta-analysis, 1992). Ad
cognitions ([C.sub.ad]) can be defined as "a multidimensional array
of consumer perceptions of the advertising stimulus" (MacKenzie
& Lutz, 1989, p. 51)--i.e. the audiences' beliefs and
perceptions of the ad (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983).
The determining factors of ad cognitions are (1) the ad
characteristics, (2) the consumer's attitude toward the advertiser
(MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989), and (3) conscious processing of
executional elements (Shimp, 1981). Moreover, it is observed that
consumer's attitude toward the advertiser is a positive, strong,
and reliable mediator of ad cognitions (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989).
This attitudinal determinant of ad cognitions implies that affect can
potentially influence the perceptual process (MacKenzie & Lutz,
1989).
After being exposed to an advertisement, audiences form a
perception about weakness or strength of arguments presented in the ad;
accordingly, quality of message argument (Arg) is one example of ad
cognitions. Lord et al revealed that the quality of message arguments
(Arg) exerts significant impact on both consumers' [A.sub.ad] and
[A.sub.b] (Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995). As the opportunity for
processing the ad message is enhanced, the impact of message arguments
(Arg) on [A.sub.ad] is considerably strengthened (Lord, Lee, &
Sauer, 1995). Also it is discovered that informative contents of an SMS
ad (i.e. rational appeals), focusing on factual information such as
product features and benefits, exert positive influence on
consumers' attitude toward the ad (Drossos, Giaglis, Lekakos,
Kokkinaki, & Stavraki, 2007; Tsang, Ho, & Liang, 2004;
Mirbagheri, 2010).
Another example of ad cognitions is ad credibility which received
some attention in many previous studies (e.g. Lutz, MacKenzie, &
Belch, 1983). Ad credibility affects weakly both [A.sub.ad] and
[A.sub.b] (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). Lutz and his colleagues define
ad credibility as the extent to which recipients perceive brand-related
claims in the ad to be honest and convincing (Lutz, MacKenzie, &
Belch, 1983; MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). It is hypothesized that
consumers evaluate credibility of a given ad based on three independent
determinants (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie & Lutz,
1989): perceived ad claim discrepancy, advertiser credibility, and
credibility of advertising in general. In addition, an empirical study
revealed that credibility of advertising in general has also an indirect
influence on ad credibility through the advertiser credibility
(MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989) Perceived ad claim discrepancy is defined
as the extent to which brand-related claims in the ad are in conflict
with the consumer's existing brand perceptions (Lutz, MacKenzie,
& Belch, 1983). The more ad claims are discrepant, the more ad
credibility will hurt. Advertiser credibility is the degree to which a
consumer perceives that the sponsor of the ad is truthful or honest
(MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). This construct is based on customer's
prior information and experience often across numerous different ads
(MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). Credibility of advertising "indicates
consumers' perceptions of the truthfulness and believability of
advertising in general, not simply the particular ad in question"
(MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989, p. 51). In addition to these three
determinants, medium type is another determinant of ad credibility.
Interactive media (such as Internet and mobile) can facilitate the trust
building through mutual communication, while one-way traditional
advertising media have limited capacity to reinforce the consumers'
trust (Stewart, Pavlou, & Ward, 2002).
In general, it is proposed that there is a considerable positive
relationship between [C.sub.ad] and [A.sub.ad] (P1); in addition,
message argument (Arg) and ad credibility are examples of ad cognitions
that have a direct influence on [A.sub.ad] (P2 & P3 respectively).
Opportunity for processing the ad message moderates the Arg- [A.sub.ad]
relationship. The higher the opportunity to process, the stronger the
Arg- [A.sub.ad] relationship (P2a).
ATTITUDE TOWARD THE ADVERTISER
Although some theoretical studies claim that attitude toward the
advertiser ([A.sub.adv]) is a weak mediator of attitude toward the ad
([A.sub.ad]) (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie & Lutz,
1989), some empirical studies hold the view that [A.sub.adv] is a strong
predictor of [A.sub.ad] (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). We propose that
the relationship between [A.sub.adv] and [A.sub.ad] is significant
enough to be considered (P4). Attitude toward the advertiser
([A.sub.adv]) can be defined as recipient's acquired tendency to
respond favorably (or unfavorably) and approvingly (or disapprovingly)
to the advertiser company (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). Attitude toward
the advertiser influences [A.sub.ad] roughly automatically and with
almost no cognitive thought (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983;
MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989). Consumers' perceptions of the
advertiser, formed based on their previous knowledge and experience of
the advertiser, are the underlying sources of [A.sub.adv] (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983).
Recipient's mood
Mood is defined as "the consumer's affective state at the
time of exposure to the ad stimulus" (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch,
1983, p. 538; MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989, p. 54). These positive or
negative feelings transfer to [A.sub.ad] (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch,
1983). It is also revealed that one in a good mood has a tendency to not
only interpret a stimulus optimistically but also respond positively to
it; in addition, it is more probable that one in a good mood retrieves
positive than negative thoughts and feelings from memory (Shimp, 1981).
A well designed advertisement can also put a recipient in a good mood,
which can affect his decision for choosing the advertised brand in the
future (Shimp, 1981).As a result, we propose that recipient's mood
has a direct and positive effect on [A.sub.ad] (P5).
The three determinants of mood are ad characteristics, individual
differences, and context of reception (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989).
Individual differences refer to the consumer's tendency to evaluate
situations positively or negatively (MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989).
Individual differences also refer to differences in consumers'
fields of interest. For instance, it is revealed that sending SMS ads
relevant to consumers' fields of interest can have a significant
influence on attitude toward SMS ad (Xu, Liao, & Li, 2008;
Mirbagheri, 2010).
Moreover, the context of reception includes all the factors
external to the communication that influence marketing communication
effectiveness (Keller, 2001). Context of reception is comprised of
several dimensions such as (1) time, (2) location and condition of
physical surroundings (e.g. lighting, clatter, and temperature), and (3)
task, that is, what the consumer is doing while receiving communication
message (Park, Shenoy, & Salvendy, 2008). For example, if SMS ad is
sent at the appropriate time and location such as a lunch suggestion
while recipient is near a restaurant at noon, it will affect
recipient's attitude positively (Carroll, Barnes, Scornavacca,
& Fletcher, 2007; Xu, Liao, & Li, 2008; Mirbagheri, 2010). Two
remaining dimensions of context are (4) surrounding ad clutters and (5)
the nature of the exposure to ads--i.e. consumer-unsolicited exposure
(e.g. TV commercials) versus consumer-solicited exposure (for
information search) (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983). If audiences
of a mass medium (e.g. TV, radio, and magazine) are exposed to ads that
are inconsistent and incompatible with their interests and
predilections, these ads may spoil the recipient's mood (Stewart,
Pavlou, & Ward, 2002).
Peripheral cues
An attitude toward an ad may develop merely because the ad evokes
an emotional response, such as a feeling of love, joy, nostalgia, or
sorrow, without any conscious processing of executional elements (Shimp,
1981). Peripheral cues (PC) are sources of information unrelated to the
actual message--e.g. the source attractiveness (based on physical
appearance, personality, or social status) or context of reception
(Solomon, 2009). Peripheral cues (PC) exert a significant direct impact
on consumers' [A.sub.ad] and [A.sub.b] (Lord, Lee, & Sauer,
1995). As a case in point, consumers may like a specific advertisement
because it uses an attractive endorser (e.g. a celebrity), a humorous
appeal (Shimp, 1981), or likable background music (Lord, Lee, &
Sauer, 1995). Also, entertainments such as funny contents and
interactive games sent via an SMS ad have influence on [A.sub.ad]
(Tsang, Ho, & Liang, 2004; Xu, Liao, & Li, 2008; Mirbagheri,
2010).
Furthermore, as the opportunity for processing the ad message is
enhanced, the impact of peripheral cues (PC) on [A.sub.ad] is
considerably weakened (Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995). Increasing the
number of exposures to an ad (e.g. from one to three exposures) can
enhance the opportunity for processing and learning the ad message
(Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995). It is thus proposed that peripheral cues
(PC) significantly affect [A.sub.ad] (P6); moreover, the higher the
opportunity to process, the weaker the PC- [A.sub.ad] relationship
(P6a).
Attitude toward the Brand
Attitude toward the Brand ([A.sub.b]) can be defined as
audiences' affective reaction to the advertised brand (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983). That is, to what extent audiences feel
purchasing the brand is good-bad, favorable-unfavorable, and
wise-foolish (Lutz, MacKenzie,
& Belch, 1983). In contrast with this affective definition of
attitude, Martin Fishbein and his colleagues have given a more important
role to cognitive processes in attitude formation and change, while by
cognition, belief structures are meant (Fishbein M. , An investigation
of the relationship between beliefs about an object and the attitude
toward that object, 1963). So attitude toward the brand is mediated by
recipients' brand related cognitive structures (Lutz, MacKenzie,
& Belch, 1983; Gresham & Shimp, 1985; MacKenzie, Lutz, &
Belch, 1986). Brand Cognitions ([C.sub.b]) can be defined as the
audiences' perceptions of the advertized brand (Lutz, MacKenzie,
& Belch, 1983)--e.g. perceived brand attributes and benefits.
In addition to cognition, affect is also another element widely
studied in terms of its effect on attitude. Affect can be defined as
"evaluative reactions that can be embodied" (Clore &
Schnall, 2005, p. 438). There has long been a debate over the way
attitudes are influenced by affect (Bodur, Brinberg, & Coupey, 2000;
Homer, 2006). While some theorists held cognitive structure fully
responsible for attitude formation and change and considered the
influence of affect on attitude to be mediated by cognitive structure,
others have posited that affect has also a direct independent influence
on attitude (Bodur, Brinberg, & Coupey, 2000).
Furthermore, the way affect impacts attitude is said to stem from
the focus of attitude whether attitude is toward an action or it is
toward an object (Clore & Schnall, 2005). If an object is the center
of evaluation, then the positive or negative affect can be transferred
to the object; however, when the focus is on tasks and actions, affect
influences information processing approach (Clore & Schnall, 2005).
We proceed by shedding more light on determinants of these two elements
([C.sub.b] and affect).
BRAND COGNITIONS
Since the beginning of attitude research, cognitive processes have
been at the heart of the focus (Wegener & Carlston, 2005). Three
types of cognitions have been recognized and studied so far in attitude
research in terms of their role in attitude formation and change: brand
cognitions, ad cognitions, and irrelevant thoughts (Lutz, MacKenzie,
& Belch, 1983; Gresham & Shimp, 1985; Homer, 1990; Coulter &
Punj, 2007). We defined brand and ad cognitions earlier. We define
irrelevant (idiosyncratic) thoughts as non-message or non-brand-related
thoughts generated in response to a persuasive message (Coulter &
Punj, 2007).
As previously defined, brand cognitions are the set of thoughts and
salient beliefs regarding the advertised brand. Persuasive messages
prompt the receiver to relate the new information to the existing
brand-related information, knowledge, attitude, etc., and this
juxtaposition generates brand cognitions (Greenwald, 1968). The net
result of these brand cognitions determines the new attitude toward the
brand. Another perspective (information integration theory (see Frey
& Kinnear, 1980)) proposes that new information about a brand is
integrated with those brand beliefs already held in memory after
attributes of that brand have been evaluated and weighted. This view
appears like a variation of the popular expectancy-value model of
Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) discussed earlier. In the expectancy-value
framework, brand cognitions consist of beliefs that the considered brand
possesses certain attributes ([b.sub.i]) and the values attached to
those attributes (ei) (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). The sum of these
cognitions shapes the overall attitude toward the object. Thus, to
change the attitude towards a brand, either the beliefs of brand
attribute possession should be altered, or the values attached to those
attributes should be modified (Lutz, 1975). While applying the first
strategy seems straightforward (e.g. Volvo is safe), the second strategy
requires more endeavor. For instance, drawing more attention to certain
attributes has proved beneficial for increasing the perceived value
("importance") of those attributes (MacKenzie, 1986; MacInnis
& Jaworski, 1989). Moreover, MacKenzie (1986) found that ad
characteristics (concrete versus abstract copy) affect the amount of
attention absorbed to the advertisement. Thus, we propose that attention
drawn to certain features mediates brand cognitions via increasing the
value attached to those attributes (P7).
Surprisingly, the [C.sub.b]--[A.sub.b] relationship has shown to be
weak or unsubstantiated in a number of studies (Lutz, MacKenzie, &
Belch, 1983), especially when [A.sub.ad] was included in the model
(Batra & Ray, 1986). Some explanations of this poor relation allude
to flaws in the research design (Homer, 1990) and some indicate that the
measurement of [C.sub.b] maybe a possible source (MacKenzie, Lutz, &
Belch, 1986). But the most compelling explanation is the domination of
peripheral route through [A.sub.ad] over the central route through
[C.sub.b] (MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986; Rose, Miniard, &
Bhatla, 1990). There is ample literature providing comparison between
two different paths to information processing--central, systematic,
effortful, etc versus peripheral, heuristic, less effortful, etc (Petty,
Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986;
Chen, Duckworth, & Chaiken, 1999) much of which stem from the
Elaboration Likelihood Model (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983) and
Dual Mediation Hypothesis (MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986). The gist
of all these models is that in the absence of motivation to process or
low involvement condition, brand attitude is more likely to be formed or
changed by heuristic processing and [A.sub.ad] is more predictive of
[A.sub.b]. Most researchers define consumer's involvement as
consumer's motivation to process the message (Celsi & Olson,
1988). The strong relationship between [A.sub.ad] and [A.sub.b] in the
studies which [C.sub.b]--[A.sub.b] was not supported indicates that
peripheral processing was in charge of attitude change (MacKenzie, Lutz,
& Belch, 1986). Even those studies demonstrating supportive evidence
for [C.sub.b]--[A.sub.b] lacked peripheral cues such as music and were
processed with high cognitive involvement (Rose, Miniard, & Bhatla,
1990). In brief, though less pronounced in emotional ad settings, the
[C.sub.b]--[A.sub.b] relation cannot be neglected especially in
informational contexts where cognitions exert their impact on [A.sub.b].
It is thus proposed that the [C.sub.b]--[A.sub.b] relation is
considerable (P8).
Argument quality is another concept worth of further attention. We
defined message argument as one of the components of [C.sub.ad]. Strong
argument (Arg) in an ad has shown to have positive effect on [A.sub.b]
under both high and low involvement conditions (Petty, Cacioppo, &
Schumann, 1983). Moreover, influence of Arg on [A.sub.b] becomes
significantly stronger by moving from low to high involvement conditions
(Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983; Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995).
However, there are some ideas that lead us to revise the aforementioned
hypotheses.
The first important point here is that in neither of these studies
[C.sub.b] is present in the model--herein, we restate the definition of
brand cognition as the set of perceptions and brand-related thoughts
(including attribute beliefs and value--the cognitive elements of
Fishbein formula). In addition, consumers hold previously formed
cognitions for familiar brands. According to cognitive response theory
(Greenwald, 1968) and information integration theory (Anderson, 1971),
new information should be compared with previous beliefs in order to
develop brand cognitions. In other words, information processing to
which argument quality is a major input, is a cognitive process that
impacts attitude through renewing brand cognitions and perceptions.
Therefore we propose that argument quality mediates [C.sub.b] (P9). We
also propose that the effect of strong argument on brand cognitions is
strengthened under high involvement rather than low involvement (P9a).
The mediation effect of [C.sub.ad] on [C.sub.b] has been previously
supported (Praxmarer & Gierl, 2009) but further work should be done
on the Arg--[C.sub.b] relationship to prove whether this component of
[C.sub.ad] influences [C.sub.b] directly.
The extent to which argument quality mediates [C.sub.b] differs
substantially under different involvement conditions and
recipient's moods. Batra & Stayman (1990) demonstrated that
positive mood decreases the amount of required elaboration giving rise
to a more heuristic processing and biasing the evaluation of the
argument quality (which generates a more favorable evaluation of the
argument) (Batra & Stayman, 1990). They also found that positive
moods seem to reduce counterarguments when the subjects are exposed to
weak messages (Batra & Stayman, 1990). Also, this moderation role of
positive mood has shown to be most decisive under low involvement
conditions (Batra & Stephens, 1994). In another study, the level of
information processing (deep vs. shallow) as well as the type of
information processing (schema based vs. data driven) with regards to
the effect of mood on them has been explored (Shapiro, MacInnis, &
Park, 2002). The findings indicate that positive mood is in charge of
activating prior schemas by signaling that there is no need for a
detailed information processing, leading to broader categorization and
confidently use of accessible cognitions such as stereotypes (Shapiro,
MacInnis, & Park, 2002; Clore & Schnall, 2005). The interesting
point here is that brand names can also operate like stereotypes (Clore
& Schnall, 2005) thus applying an advertising strategy which evokes
positive moods can be a powerful tool for established brands. All these
studies suggest that positive mood negatively moderates the
Arg-[C.sub.b] relationship by impeding thorough evaluation of argument
quality (P9b).
The notion that the argument quality is the primary driver of
message acceptance has not been supported in a number of studies
(Coulter & Punj, 2004). Instead, the integral role of cognitive
resources required (RR) and those available for processing (RA) have
been a subject of attention in cognitive resource matching (CRM)
hypothesis (Coulter & Punj, 2004). According to this hypothesis,
regardless of the quality of the argument, the persuasiveness of any
message entails a match between resources required and those available
for processing. In other words, although according to ELM, higher levels
of elaboration will enhance (decrease) persuasion when the argument is
strong (weak), increasing elaboration beyond the level where RR equals
RA will negatively moderate that effect (Coulter & Punj, 2004). It
is therefore proposed that when the RR is greater than RA, lack of
consumer's cognitive capacity to process an argument hampers the
Arg-[C.sub.b] relationship; on the other hand when RR=RA the
Arg-[C.sub.B] relationship is strengthened (P9c).
Furthermore, since irrelevant thoughts also occupy a proportion of
cognitive capacity, their role has proved to be detrimental to
persuasiveness when the message has a strong argument under low
involvement condition (Coulter & Punj, 2007). These irrelevant
thoughts take over support arguments, thus negatively moderate
Arg--[C.sub.b]. On the contrary, when the message has weak argument,
they supplant counterarguments, thus negatively moderate Arg--[C.sub.b]
resulting in a more positive brand attitude (Coulter & Punj, 2007).
We propose that irrelevant thoughts indirectly by limiting available
cognitive resources adversely affect Arg-[c.sub.b] (P10). The summary of
these discussions can be illustrated as in Figure 2:
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Non-cognitive regard to attitude can be traced back to the
'80s when the mounting importance of attitude toward the ad and its
peripheral role in determining [A.sub.b] abounded a plethora of studies
(e.g. Mitchell & Olson, 1981). Zajonc's influential work
(Zajonc, 1980) in which affect and cognition were concluded to be
separate and independent systems was maybe the fountainhead of this
separation from purely cognitive literature of attitude and the
embarking on dual-path and affect-laden attitude research. Further
research (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, &
Belch, 1986; MacKenzie & Lutz, 1989) attested to the significant
impact of [A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] and de-emphasized a purely cognitive
process in attitude formation and change, notwithstanding the fact that
some other researchers called these findings into question by naming
them "methodological artifacts" stemming from flawed use of
criteria or invalid predictors (Fishbein & Middlestadt, 1995). Such
criticisms were also replied by others (for more details on the series
of responses to this issue see Haugtvedt & Kasmer, 2008). However,
the rising wave of affect research in the late '80s (Batra &
Ray, 1986; Edell & Burke, 1987; Stayman & Aaker, 1988)
highlighted the independent path of persuasion through ad-induced
affect, and ushered in a new perspective in which affect and cognition
were both inextricable mediators of attitude.
Several types of affective states have been studied. Here, we are
interested in those triggered by the ad and distinguish them from moods,
which can exist before the recipient is exposed to the ad (Burke &
Edell, 1989) and are not focused on a specific object (Clore &
Schnall, 2005).
There is evidence that ad-evoked feelings influence [A.sub.b]
through indirect path by [A.sub.ad] (Batra & Ray, 1986; Burke &
Edell, 1989). Burke & Edell (1989) suggest that feelings have both a
direct and an indirect effect (via judgments of ad's
characteristics) on [A.sub.ad].
Furthermore, in recent years researchers have been more inclined to
distinguish between positive and negative affect. Edell and Burke (1987)
find that positive and negative feelings co-occur while according to
Brown et al (1998) they are bi-dimensional not bipolar ("different
constructs not merely opposite poles of the same construct") and
the way they influence attitude is different (Burke & Edell, 1989;
Brown, Homer, & Inman, 1998; Homer, 2006). Negative affect directly
inflicts unfavorable effects upon attitude toward the brand (Burke &
Edell, 1989), whereas positive affect mediates [A.sub.b] both directly
and indirectly not only through brand cognitions (Homer, 2006) but also
via attitude toward the ad (Stayman & Aaker, 1988). As Burke and
Edell (1989) importantly remark, although eliciting negative effects
(such as fear) may have positive effects on consumer purchase
intentions, they act to the detriment of attitude toward the ad and
attitude toward the brand. Therefore, it is proposed that negative
affect directly influences [A.sub.b] (P11), while positive affect both
directly (P12) and indirectly via [C.sub.b] (P13) exerts its effect on
brand attitudes. In addition, both positive and negative affect
influence [A.sub.ad] (P14, P15).
Another concept primarily studied in decision making and brand
preference literature is brand familiarity. Brand familiarity can be
defined as "a uni-dimensional construct directly related to the
amount of time that has been spent processing information about the
brand, regardless of the type or content of the processing that has
occurred" (Baker, Hutchinson, Moore, & Nedungadi, 1986, p.
637). As to attitudes, brand familiarity is posited to moderate the
influence of [A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] (Derbaix, 1995; Brown, Homer, &
Inman, 1998; Campbell & Keller, 2003). In other words, consumers
with prior brand familiarity will rely more on their existing knowledge
about the brand than on [A.sub.ad] in forming their [A.sub.b] (Derbaix,
1995; Campbell & Keller, 2003). Moreover, Derbaix (1995) found that
affect had stronger impact on [A.sub.b] for novel brands, but this
moderating role of brand familiarity was later found to hold true only
for positive affect (Brown, Homer, & Inman, 1998; Homer, 2006). This
means that negative affect has a direct and strong impact on brand
attitude under both familiar and unfamiliar brand conditions, but
positive affect influences [A.sub.b] through [C.sub.b] for familiar
brands and directly for novel brands (Homer, 2006). Homer's study
(2006) indicates that even facing a completely emotional ad devoid of
any product attribute information, the receiver's attitude toward a
familiar brand is changed mainly via brand cognitions when positive
feelings are evoked (consumers "update" their existing
evaluations), but negative affect directly ruins attitude even toward a
familiar brand. All in all, brand familiarity acts as a moderator in all
mentioned relationships except that between negative affect and brand
attitudes. For familiar brands, the strength of
positive-affect--[A.sub.b] is reduced while the effect of positive
affect on brand cognitions are intensified; for novel brand vice versa
happens. Thus brand familiarity negatively moderates
positive-affect--[A.sub.b] relationship (P12a) and positively moderates
positive-affect--[C.sub.b] relationship (P13a).
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
As it is shown in Figure 3, brand familiarity does not moderate the
direct effect of negative affect on [A.sub.b], on the grounds that
regardless of the level of brand familiarity, this direct influence
dominates other indirect routes (Homer, 2006). In contrast, for all
other mediations of affect and [A.sub.ad], familiarity acts as a
moderator. For unfamiliar brands, (positive and negative) affect
directly and indirectly through [A.sub.ad] mediates [A.sub.b] (Homer,
2006), but for familiar products, the cognitions play the major role
(Campbell & Keller, 2003; Derbaix, 1995) except for negative affect
whose destructive effect on [A.sub.b] is straight and direct (Homer,
2006).
The Relationship between attitude toward the ad and attitude toward
the brand
The relationship between attitude toward the ad and attitude toward
the brand is a controversial issue (Gresham & Shimp, 1985). Based on
prior theoretical and empirical research, three possible explanations
for relationship between attitude toward the ad ([A.sub.ad]) and
attitude toward the brand ([A.sub.b]) are identified (Lutz, MacKenzie,
& Belch, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986).
First, the affect transfer hypothesis (ATH) supposes a
unidirectional causation from [A.sub.ad] to [A.sub.b] (MacKenzie, Lutz,
& Belch, 1986). In other words, it has been hypothesized that
affective reactions toward an advertisement of a brand (i.e. [A.sub.ad])
should have impacts on audiences' attitudes toward the brand
without changing their brand cognitions (Cb) (Gresham & Shimp,
1985). Affect transfer hypothesis has been supported by some past
studies (Mitchell & Olson, 1981); some empirical data (Gresham &
Shimp, 1985), however, have not completely supported this hypothesis.
Gresham and Shimp (1985) also found that potential effect of negative
attitudes toward the ad on weakening the consumers' attitudes
toward the brand is much bigger than influence of positive attitudes
toward the ad on strengthening the consumers' attitudes toward the
brand (Gresham & Shimp, 1985). Recently, a research for online
environment found that for consumers who are less motivated to consider
a lot of information (i.e. low need for cognition), and have little
contact with cyberspace, affect transfer hypothesis is the superior
model in explaining the relationship between attitude toward the ad and
attitude toward the brand (Sicilia, Ruiz, & Reynolds, 2006).
Second, the reciprocal mediation hypothesis (RMH) which can be
inferred from Balance theory, asserts that there is a mutual causal
relationship between [A.sub.ad] and [A.sub.b] (Gresham & Shimp,
1985; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986). Because the correlation
between the ad and the advertised brand is clearly positive, balance
theory predicts that a recipient of an ad promoting a specific brand
will attempt to obtain a balanced relationship by either liking both the
ad and the brand or disliking both (MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986).
In addition, for a recently introduced brand, the influence of
[A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] should be stronger than the opposite direction;
in contrast, for a mature brand with which consumers have more
experience, the flow from [A.sub.b] to [A.sub.ad] would govern (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986).
Gresham and Shimp implicitly presented the evidences supporting above
statement about new and mature brands (Gresham & Shimp, 1985).
Finally, the dual mediation hypothesis (DMH) supposes that
[A.sub.ad] affects [A.sub.b] not only directly but also indirectly
through its impact on brand cognitions (Cb) (MacKenzie, Lutz, &
Belch, 1986). We can compare this hypothesis with Elaboration Likelihood
Model (ELM) presented by Petty and Cacioppo (1981). According to ELM,
attitude can be changed via one of two routes: central route via which a
recipient's extensive thought about content of an advertisement
leads to attitude change; and peripheral route via which a
recipient's attitude is shaped by positive and negative cues (e.g.
an expert source) associated with advertisement or inferences based on
simple cues in the context of the ad (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann,
1983). Accordingly, [A.sub.b] changes via peripheral route when it is
directly influenced by [A.sub.ad]; besides, [A.sub.b] changes via
central route when [A.sub.ad] affects [C.sub.b] then [C.sub.b]
influences on [A.sub.b] (Lutz, MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983). It is also
stated that high involvement ad messages, having greater personal
relevance and outcomes than low involvement messages, affect the
attitude via central route, but low involvement ad messages influence on
attitude via peripheral route (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983).
We can, therefore, hypothesize that under high involvement condition,
there is a relatively weak influence of [A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] as well
as a relatively strong influence of [C.sub.b] on [A.sub.b] (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983). Unlike our expectations, empirical data
show that influence of [A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] dominates the influence
of [C.sub.b] on [A.sub.b], irrespective of involvement condition (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; Muehling & Laczniak, 1988; Homer,
1990; Brown & Stayman, 1992). Hence, the major difference between
DMH and ELM is that DMH views central and peripheral processes as
intertwined processes instead of substitutes for each other (MacKenzie,
Lutz, & Belch, 1986).
Two structural equations analyses (one for TV commercials and the
other for print ads) showed that the dual mediation hypothesis (DMH) is
superior to the other two hypotheses (MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch,
1986; Homer, 1990). Although MacKenzie et al hold the view that there
are some limitation for generalization of their finding, the
generalizability of these findings were checked in a meta-analysis by
Brown and Stayman, and dual mediation model was strongly supported
(Brown & Stayman, 1992). Recently, a research for online environment
discovered DMH is the superior model, especially for consumers willing
to deepen their understanding of each phenomenon (i.e. high need for
cognition) (Sicilia, Ruiz, & Reynolds, 2006). Accordingly, it is
proposed that [A.sub.ad] directly mediates [A.sub.b] (P16); moreover,
[A.sub.ad] indirectly mediates [A.sub.b] through its impact on brand
cognitions ([C.sub.b]) (P17). That is, consumer's attitude toward
the ad has an impact on their attitude toward the brand, through either
a simple affect transfer (i.e., the recipient likes the brand since the
ad appeals to him or her) or a more intricate cognitive processing
(i.e., one likes the brand due to effective tactics used by advertiser)
(Kirmani & Campbell, 2009). Furthermore, under situations benefiting
from high amount of message elaboration (e.g. print ads), [A.sub.ad]-
[C.sub.b] relation is greater than that under situations not having this
advantage (e.g. TV commercials) (Brown & Stayman, 1992). In
addition, when the advertised brand is novel and less familiar, the
correlation between [A.sub.ad] and [A.sub.b] becomes stronger (Brown
& Stayman, 1992). Therefore, it is proposed that brand familiarity
negatively moderate the [A.sub.ad]--[A.sub.b] relationship (P16a).
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
CONCLUSION
After in depth reviewing of literature, we proposed several
propositions that show not only the relationship between each two
constructs but also the most important moderators of each relationship.
These relationships and moderators are summarized in Table 1.
According to our propositions, [A.sub.ad] influences [A.sub.b] not
only directly but also indirectly through its impact on brand cognitions
([C.sub.b]); besides, when the advertised brand is novel and less
familiar, the correlation between [A.sub.ad] and [A.sub.b] strengthens.
Major antecedents of attitude toward the ad ([A.sub.ad]) are ad
cognitions, attitude toward the advertiser, mood, and peripheral cues.
Ad cognitions have a direct impact on attitude toward the ad; two
examples of these cognitions are quality of message argument (Arg) and
ad credibility. As the opportunity for processing the ad message is
increased, the impact of message arguments (Arg) on [A.sub.ad] is
considerably strengthened. In addition, both attitude toward the
advertiser ([A.sub.adv]) and peripheral cues (PC) as well as
consumer's mood are strong mediators of attitude toward the ad
([A.sub.ad]). As the opportunity for processing the ad message is
increased, the influence of PC on A ad is weakened.
Brand cognitions were the first elements in the attitude literature
to be studied in terms of their effect on [A.sub.b]. Attention drawn to
certain product attributes bolds the value (importance) of those
attributes; hence, we propose that attention influences [C.sub.b]. A
match between cognitive resources required for message processing and
those available along with the level of involvement and recipient's
mood moderates the effect of argument quality on generation or
modification of favorable brand-related thoughts and perceptions. The
irrelevant thoughts produced during the exposure to the ad, indirectly
by restricting resources available for processing, influence brand
cognitions. Negative affect directly and adversely affects [A.sub.b].
However, the path through which positive affects impact [A.sub.b] is
subject to brand familiarity. For novel brands positive affect (together
with [A.sub.ad]) is the major determinant of attitude, while for
familiar brands, the indirect path through brand cognitions dominates
all other routes.
Figure 5 joints several parts of our model each of which described
earlier:
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
Removing moderators from Figure 5 leaves the simplified model shown
in Figure 6:
[FIGURE 6 OMITTED]
The attitude literature is fraught with studies and frameworks
which focus either on the information processing paradigms (e.g.
MacInnis & Jaworski, 1989) or on affective perspectives (e.g. Batra
& Ray, 1986). Also, there are a number of experiments that either
discriminate between informational and emotional ads or set only one of
the two types above (affect vs. cognition) as the context of their study
(e.g. Yoo & MacInnis, 2005; Homer, 2006). By emotional ads, those
"designed to appeal to the receiver's emotions by using drama,
mood, music, and other emotion-eliciting strategies" and by
informational ads, those "designed to appeal to the rationality of
the receiver by using objective information describing a brand's
attributes or benefits" are meant (Yoo & MacInnis, 2005, p.
1397). The rationale for such classification is somehow clear; with
respect to the level of involvement (motivation), according to the above
definition, each type calls for either cognitive or affective ways of
attitude formation and change. Although these may seem two extremes of
the real advertising world, they effectively depict conditions under
which one of the routes would be dominant over the other. We, therefore,
use this typology to show where information processing is more involved
and where an affect-laden process is in work. By this strategy as it is
shown in Figure 7, we distinguish between the evaluative section of the
model (including brand cognition) and the non-evaluative section
(affect). Furthermore, it is noteworthy that although affective
responses are non-evaluative (Batra & Ray, 1986), [A.sub.ad] is
neither purely evaluative nor purely non-evaluative (MacKenzie &
Lutz, 1989).
[FIGURE 7 OMITTED]
Implications and Further Research
Whatever the prime goal of marketers is (boosting sales, forming
long-term profitable relationships, attracting support and membership
for nonprofit businesses, etc), customer persuasion is an integral step
towards that goal and both [A.sub.b] and [A.sub.ad] play major roles in
persuading prospects to buy a particular brand or support a
not-for-profit organization. There are substantial empirical evidences
that support existence of a direct causal relationship from attitude
toward the brand ([A.sub.b]) to purchase intention (PI) (Lutz,
MacKenzie, & Belch, 1983; MacKenzie, Lutz, & Belch, 1986).
Furthermore, some other researchers have identified that [A.sub.ad] has
a moderate direct influence on purchase intention (PI) (Brown &
Stayman, 1992; Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995).
Lord et al also found that under low (high) involvement conditions,
correlation between [A.sub.ad] and PI is considerably greater (lower)
than correlation between [A.sub.b] and PI (Lord, Lee, & Sauer,
1995). In other words, [A.sub.b] can be a superior predictor of
consumer's intention to buy a brand under high than under low
involvement conditions (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983). Also,
for novel or less familiar brands, the positive correlation between
[A.sub.ad] and PI becomes stronger (Brown & Stayman, 1992). Thus,
[A.sub.ad] influences purchase intention not only directly but also
indirectly via [A.sub.b] (Lord, Lee, & Sauer, 1995).
Our proposed model can provide marketers with a framework to
understand how their marketing plans, especially advertising and
branding plans, influence their consumers. Marketers can use our model
both to justify the effectiveness of their plans and to employ its
variables as indices for assessing those plans. For example, our model
implies that different advertising practices should be applied to brands
with different brand familiarity. Marketers of novel brands should focus
more on emotional ads to make positive affective reactions transfer to
[A.sub.ad] and consequently enhance PI. Also ads for these products
should be designed in such ways as to create a positive attitude toward
the ad itself. Advertisements for familiar brands, however, should
maintain a balance between emotional and informational approaches.
Although the cognitive path is dominant for familiar brands, a highly
informative ad under low involvement can even hurt the attitude in some
circumstances (Coulter & Punj, 2007). In addition, marketers of a
familiar brand should not neglect the importance of emotional ads, in
that emotional ads put a recipient in a positive mood which decreases
the amount of required elaboration and generates a more favorable
evaluation of the argument quality.
Our research has some limitations. First, although we relied on the
research from the last 30 years to develop our propositions, further
research is required to validate the generalizability of these
propositions by a series of empirical studies. Second, the advent of new
advertising media such as mobile and internet may bring evolutionary
changes in the hypotheses on which our model is built. Thus, further
research should be conducted to unveil whether differences in media type
can influence our integrative model. Finally, future studies are needed
to address cross-cultural differences that can alter some of the
propositions and relationships.
REFERENCES
Anderson, N.H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change.
Psychological Review, 78(3), 171-206.
Baker, W., J.W. Hutchinson, D. Moore & P. Nedungadi (1986).
Brand familiarity and advertising: Effects on the evoked set and brand
preference. Advances in Consumer Research, 13, 637-642.
Batra, R. & M.L. Ray (1986). Affective responses mediating
acceptance of advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(2), 234-249.
Batra, R. & D.M. Stayman (1990). The role of mood in
advertising effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(2), 203-214.
Batra, R. & D. Stephens (1994). Attitudinal effects of
ad-evoked moods and emotions: The moderating role of motivation.
Psychology & Marketing, 11(3), 199-215.
Belch, G.E. & M.A. Belch (2004). Advertising and promotion: An
integrated marketing communication perspective (Sixth International
Edition). Singapore: McGraw-Hill.
Berger, I.E. & A.A. Mitchell (1989). The effect of advertising
on attitude accessibility, attitude confidence, and the
attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(3),
269-279.
Bodur, H.O., D. Brinberg & E. Coupey (2000). Belief, affect,
and attitude: Alternative models of the determinants of attitude.
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 9(1), 17-28.
Brown, S.P. & D.M. Stayman (1992). Antecedents and consequences
of attitude toward the ad: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer
Research, 19(1), 34-50.
Brown, S.P., P.M. Homer & J.J. Inman (1998). A meta-analysis of
relationships between ad-evoked feelings and advertising responses.
Journal of Marketing Research, 35(1), 114-126.
Burke, M.C. & J.A. Edell (1989). The impact of feelings on
ad-based affect and cognition. Journal of Marketing Research, 26(1),
69-83.
Campbell, M.C. & K.L. Keller (2003). Brand familiarity and
advertising repetition effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(2),
292-304.
Carroll, A., S.J. Barnes, E. Scornavacca & K. Fletcher (2007).
Consumer perceptions and attitudes towards SMS advertising: recent
evidence from New Zealand. International Journal of Advertising, 26(1),
79-98.
Celsi, R.L. & J.C. Olson (1988). The role of involvement in
attention and comprehension processes. Journal of Consumer Research,
15(2), 210-224.
Chen, S., K. Duckworth & S. Chaiken (1999). Motivated heuristic
and systematic processing. Psychological Inquiry, 10(1), 44-49.
Clore, G.L. & S. Schnall (2005). The influence of affect on
attitude. In D. Albarracin, B.T. Johnson, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), The
Handbook of Attitudes (pp. 437-489). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
Coulter, K.S. & G.N. Punj (2004). The effects of cognitive
resource requirements, availability, and argument quality on brand
attitudes. Journal of Advertising, 33(4), 53-64.
Coulter, K.S. & G.N. Punj (2007). Understanding the role of
idiosyncratic thinking in brand attitude formation. Journal of
Advertising, 36(1), 7-20.
Derbaix, C.M. (1995). The impact of affective reactions on
attitudes toward the advertisement and the brand: A step toward
ecological validity. Journal of Marketing Research, 32(4), 470-479.
Drossos, D., G.M. Giaglis, G. Lekakos, F. Kokkinaki & M.G.
Stavraki (2007). Determinants of effective SMS advertising: An
experimental study. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 7(2), 30-38
Edell, J.A. & M.C. Burke (1987). The power of feelings in
understanding advertising effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3),
421-433.
Fishbein, M. (1963). An investigation of the relationship between
beliefs about an object and the attitude toward that object. Human
Relations, 16(3), 233-240.
Fishbein, M. & I. Ajzen (1975). Belief, attitude, intention,
and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley.
Fishbein, M. & S. Middlestadt (1995). Noncognitive effects on
attitude formation and change: Fact or artifact? Journal of Consumer
Psychology, 4(2), 181-202.
Frey, C.J. & T.C. Kinnear (1980). Information integration
theory: An alternative attitude model for consumer behavior. Advances in
Consumer Research, 7, 350-355.
Greenwald, A.G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response to
persuasion, and attitude change. In A.G. Greenwald, T.C. Brock, &
T.M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological Foundations of Attitudes (pp.
147-170). New York: Academic Press.
Gresham, L.G., & T.A. Shimp (1985). Attitude toward the
advertisement and brand attitude: A classical conditioning perspective.
Journal of Advertising, 14(1), 10-17.
Haugtvedt, C.P., & J.A. Kasmer (2008). Attitude change and
persuasion. In C. P. Haugtvedt, P. M. Herr, & F. R. Kardes (Eds.),
Handbook of Consumer Psychology (pp. 419-435). New York: Taylor &
Francis Group.
Homer, P.M. (2006). Relationships among ad-induced affect, beliefs,
and attitudes: Another look. Journal of Advertising, 35(1), 35-51.
Homer, P.M. (1990). The mediating role of attitude toward the ad:
Some additional evidence. Journal of Marketing Research, 27(1), 78-86.
Keller, K.L. (2001). Mastering the marketing communications mix:
Micro and macro perspectives on integrated marketing communication
programs. Journal of Marketing Management, 17(7/8), 819-847.
Kirmani, A. & M.C. Campbell (2009). Taking the target's
perspective: The persuasion knowledge model. In M. Wanke (Ed.), Social
Psychology of Consumer Behavior (pp. 297-316). New York, United States
of America: Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
Lord, K.R., M.S. Lee & P.L. Sauer (1995). The combined
influence hypothesis: Central and peripheral antecedents of attitude
toward the ad. Journal of Advertising, 24(1), 73-85.
Lutz, R.J. (1975). Changing brand attitudes through modification of
cognitive structure. Journal of Consumer Research, 1(4), 49-59.
Lutz, R.J., S.B. MacKenzie & G.E. Belch (1983). Attitude toward
the ad as a mediator of advertising effectiveness: Determinants and
consequences. Advances in Consumer Research, 10(1), 532-539.
MacInnis, D.J. & B.J. Jaworski (1989). Information processing
from advertisements: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of
Marketing, 53(4), 1-23.
MacKenzie, S.B. (1986). The role of attention in mediating the
effect of advertising on attribute importance. Journal of Consumer
Research, 13(2), 174-195.
MacKenzie, S.B. & R.J. Lutz (1989). An empirical examination of
the structural antecedents of attitude toward the ad in an advertising
pretesting context. Journal of Marketing, 53(2), 48-65.
MacKenzie, S.B., R.J. Lutz & G.E. Belch (1986). The role of
attitude toward the ad as a mediator of advertising effectiveness: A
test of competing explanations. Journal of Marketing Research, 23(2),
130-43.
Maio, G.R., V.M. Esses, K.H. Arnold & J.M. Olson (2004). The
function-structure model of attitudes. In G. Haddock, & G. R. Maio
(Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Psychology of Attitudes (pp.
9-34). New York: Psychology Press Inc.
Mirbagheri, S. (2010). SMS advertising business model: Toward
finding vital elements of its value model. Proceedings of the 2010
European Applied Business Research Conference (EABR), 815-824
Mitchell, A.A. & J.C. Olson (1981). Are product attribute
beliefs the only mediator of advertising effects on brand attitude?.
Journal of Marketing Research, 18(3), 318-32.
Muehling, D.D. & R.N. Laczniak (1988). Advertising's
immediate and delayed influence on brand attitudes: Considerations
across message-involvement levels. Journal of Advertising, 17(4), 23-34.
Park, T., R. Shenoy & G. Salvendy (2008). Effective advertising
on mobile phones: A literature review and presentation of results from
53 case studies. Behaviour & Information Technology, 27(5), 355-373.
Petty, R.E. & J.T. Cacioppo (1981). Attitudes and persuasion:
Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, Iowa: William C. Brown.
Petty, R.E., J.T. Cacioppo & D. Schumann (1983). Central and
peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of
involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(2), 135-146.
Praxmarer, S. & H. Gierl (2009). The effects of positive and
negative ad-evoked associations on brand attitude. Asia Pacific Journal
of Marketing and Logistics, 21(4), 507-520.
Rose, R.L., P.W. Miniard & S. Bhatla (1990). Brand cognitions
as determinants of brand attitudes: The influence of measurement and
processing involvement. Advances in Consumer Research, 17, 128-134.
Shapiro, S., D.J. MacInnis & C.W. Park (2002). Understanding
program induced mood effects: Decoupling arousal from valence. Journal
of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26.
Shimp, T.A. (1981). Attitude toward the ad as a mediator of
consumer brand choice. Journal of Advertising, 10(2), 9-48.
Sicilia, M., S. Ruiz & N. Reynolds (2006). Attitude formation
online: How the consumer's need for cognition affects the
relationship between attitude towards the website and attitude towards
the brand. International Journal of Market Research, 48(2), 139-154.
Solomon, M.R. (2009). Consumer behavior: buying, having, and being
(Ninth Edition). Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.
Stayman, D.M. & D.A. Aaker (1988). Are all the effects of
ad-induced feelings mediated by [A.sub.ad]?. Journal of Consumer
Research, 15(3), 368-373.
Stewart, D.W., P. Pavlou & S. Ward (2002). Media influences on
marketing communications. In J. Bryant & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media
Effects: Advances in Theory and Research (Second Edition) (pp.353-95).
New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Tsang, M.M., S.C. Ho & T.P. Liang (2004). Consumer attitudes
toward mobile advertising: An empirical study. International Journal of
Electronic Commerce, 8(3), 65-78.
Wegener, D.T. & D.E. Carlston (2005). Cognitive processes in
attitude formation and change. In D. Albarracin, B.T. Johnson, &
M.P. Zanna (Eds.), The Handbook of Attitudes (pp. 493-542). New Jersey:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Wyer, R.S. (1970). Quantitative prediction of belief and opinion
change: A further test of a subjective probability model. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 16(4), 559-570.
Xu, D.J., S.S. Liao & Q. Li (2008). Combining empirical
experimentation and modeling techniques: A design research approach for
personalized mobile advertising applications. Decision Support Systems,
44(3), 710-724.
Yoo, C. & D.J. MacInnis (2005). The brand attitude formation
process of emotional and informational ads. Journal of Business
Research, 58(10), 1397-1406.
Zajonc, R.B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no
inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151-175.
Manoochehr Najmi, Sharif University of Technology
Yashar Atefi, Sharif University of Technology
Seyed Alireza Mirbagheri, Sharif University of Technology
Table 1: Summary of relationships and moderators
Relationship Moderators
Brand Opportunity
Familiarity to Process
P1: [C.sub.ad] on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P2: Arg on [A.sub.ad] (+) P2a (+)
P3: ad credibility on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P4: [A.sub.adv] on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P5: mood on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P6: PC on [A.sub.ad] (+) P6a (-)
P7: attention on [C.sub.b] (+)
P8: [C.sub.b] on [A.sub.b] (+)
P9: Arg on [C.sub.b] (+)
P10: Irrelevant thoughts on RA (-)
P11: Negative affect on [A.sub.b] (-)
P12: Positive affect on [A.sub.b] (+) P12a(-)
P13: Positive affect on [C.sub.b] (+) P13a(+)
P14: Positive affect on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P15: Negative affect on [A.sub.ad] (-)
P16: [A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] (+) P16a (-)
P17: [A.sub.ad] on [C.sub.b] (+)
Relationship Moderators
Involvement Mood Capacity
P1: [C.sub.ad] on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P2: Arg on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P3: ad credibility on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P4: [A.sub.adv] on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P5: mood on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P6: PC on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P7: attention on [C.sub.b] (+)
P8: [C.sub.b] on [A.sub.b] (+)
P9: Arg on [C.sub.b] (+) P9a(+) P9b(-) P9c(+)
P10: Irrelevant thoughts on RA (-)
P11: Negative affect on [A.sub.b] (-)
P12: Positive affect on [A.sub.b] (+)
P13: Positive affect on [C.sub.b] (+)
P14: Positive affect on [A.sub.ad] (+)
P15: Negative affect on [A.sub.ad] (-)
P16: [A.sub.ad] on [A.sub.b] (+)
P17: [A.sub.ad] on [C.sub.b] (+)