首页    期刊浏览 2025年04月12日 星期六
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Applicant perceptions of the gender effect on the selling process and on targeting prospective customers: does gender matter?
  • 作者:Pinar, Musa ; Hardin, J. Russell ; Eser, Zeliha
  • 期刊名称:Academy of Marketing Studies Journal
  • 印刷版ISSN:1095-6298
  • 出版年度:2011
  • 期号:January
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:The DreamCatchers Group, LLC
  • 摘要:In recent years, as more women have joined the sales force, women have proven to be as successful as men in the traditionally male-dominated field of selling; yet they still face some barriers in gaining entry to some selling jobs (Fugate et al., 1988; "Pink Ghetto in Sales," 1988). Some barriers are caused by sales managers' beliefs in gender stereotypes (Kanuk, 1978). Several prior studies (Comer and Jolson, 1991; Russ and McNeily, 1988; Swan and Futrell, 1978; Swan, Rink, Kiser and Martin, 1984) of women in sales suggest that stereotypes of women in selling still exist from both managers and potential customers. A study by Comer and Jolson (1991) showed that, according to sales managers' perceptions, the more a saleswoman's behavior resembles the negative gender stereotype, the less effective her selling performance. The current study examines two fundamental issues: (1) whether negative (or positive) gender stereotypes are predictors of, or even associated with, selling performance, and (2) whether gender similarity/dissimilarity between salespersons and potential buyers impacts sales efforts during the selling process and in targeting efforts in finding new customers. These issues have apparently not been addressed by prior research. The current study could have important managerial implications for recruiting and developing effective sales force training programs and strategies.
  • 关键词:Business enterprises;Selling;Sex differences (Psychology);Target marketing;Women consumers

Applicant perceptions of the gender effect on the selling process and on targeting prospective customers: does gender matter?


Pinar, Musa ; Hardin, J. Russell ; Eser, Zeliha 等


INTRODUCTION

In recent years, as more women have joined the sales force, women have proven to be as successful as men in the traditionally male-dominated field of selling; yet they still face some barriers in gaining entry to some selling jobs (Fugate et al., 1988; "Pink Ghetto in Sales," 1988). Some barriers are caused by sales managers' beliefs in gender stereotypes (Kanuk, 1978). Several prior studies (Comer and Jolson, 1991; Russ and McNeily, 1988; Swan and Futrell, 1978; Swan, Rink, Kiser and Martin, 1984) of women in sales suggest that stereotypes of women in selling still exist from both managers and potential customers. A study by Comer and Jolson (1991) showed that, according to sales managers' perceptions, the more a saleswoman's behavior resembles the negative gender stereotype, the less effective her selling performance. The current study examines two fundamental issues: (1) whether negative (or positive) gender stereotypes are predictors of, or even associated with, selling performance, and (2) whether gender similarity/dissimilarity between salespersons and potential buyers impacts sales efforts during the selling process and in targeting efforts in finding new customers. These issues have apparently not been addressed by prior research. The current study could have important managerial implications for recruiting and developing effective sales force training programs and strategies.

Prior literature concerning gender effects in the sales field have focused on two main areas: one area of research has examined the effect of gender on interview processes and recruiting outcomes; and another area of research examined the impact of buyer-seller gender on sales performance. Several studies have investigated gender bias during the employment interview and attempted to separate the effects of applicant gender and recruiter gender on recruiters' evaluations (Arvey and Faley, 1988; Powell, 1987). These prior studies produced mixed results concerning the effect of the similarity of applicant gender and recruiter gender on interview outcomes. For example, a study by Graves and Powell (1988) found that applicant gender had no significant effect on the interview outcome; while the results of another study by Graves and Powell (1995) showed that female recruiters saw male applicants as more similar to themselves and more qualified than female applicants.

Other research that examined the gender effect in selling has covered such issues as female managers' leadership style (Comer et al., 1995; Yammarino et al., 1997); sex-role identity (Jolson and Comer, 1992); stereotypical behavior and perceptions of gender stereotyping (Comer and Jolson, 1991; Russ and McNeilly, 1988); and professional status (Gable and Reed, 1987). Concerning the gender effect on sales performance, past studies (Crosby, Evan, and Cowles, 1990; Smith, 1998) suggest that gender similarity between sales persons and customers is positively related to the quality of the sales person/customer relationship and sales performance. Crosby et al. (1990) found that same-gender relationships seem to be associated with greater relationship investment, more open communication, and greater trust and satisfaction within relationships. These findings support conventional wisdom that exchange relationships are easier to develop with similar others (Churchill et al., 1997). An earlier study by Churchill et al., (1975) found a significant relationship between visible similarity (i.e., gender, race, age, and nationality) and sales performance. However, other studies (Crosby et al., 1990; Weitz, 1981) suggest that the relationship that exists between dyadic similarity and salesperson performance is weak at best.

An empirical study by Dwyer, Orlando, Shephard (1998) showed that female salespeople were just as effective as male salespeople, and gender similarity was not a significant factor in sales performance. In addition, their study found that male-female and female-male mismatched dyads significantly outperformed gender-matched dyads. In fact, their results showed that women selling to men (a mismatch) performed higher than the matching female-female dyads, and also exceeded the performance of male-male and male-female dyads. In addressing the question of "Does difference matter," Jones, Moore, Stanaland and Wyatt (1998) found that consumers appear to be more accepting of salespeople who are "dissimilar" to themselves, which contradicts some assertions in the popular press (Lucas 1996). They concluded that "difference" made no difference.

The literature concerning the gender effect on sales performance provides mixed results. As some studies (Churchill et al., 1997, 1975; Crosby et al., 1990; Smith, 1998) suggest benefits of matching buyer-seller genders, others studies (Dwyer et al., 1998; Jones et al., 1998) contradict these benefits. This study is aimed at investigating applicant perceptions (business students) regarding gender effects on different stages of the selling process and selling efforts. Unlike prior studies in the selling field where the focus was on the gender effect on recruiting processes and/or sales performance, this paper examines the gender effect at each step of the selling process as well as the gender effect on targeting of potential buyers. This study has two sets of specific objectives. One set of objectives deals with whether applicants perceive that they will: (a) confidently introduce themselves to male versus female buyers; (b) make presentations to male versus female buyers; (c) answer questions effectively from male versus female buyers; (d) overcome objections from male versus female buyers, and (e) engage in a trial close and successfully close the sale with male versus female buyers. The second set of objectives deal with whether the applicant: (a) would more strongly target male versus female potential buyers; (b) would be more successful selling to male versus female buyers, (c) would more strongly prefer selling to males versus females; and (d) would prefer to focus more of their efforts on males versus females when finding new buyers.

Selling is a process with successive stages, which includes meeting the prospect, making a presentation, answering questions, overcoming objections, attempting a trial close, and closing the sale (Futrell, 2006; Manning and Reece, 1995). Understanding the gender effect on each stage of selling process could provide insight into the nature of gender effects in the sales field. Also, before the actual selling process, it would be beneficial to understand the gender effect on targeting and selling efforts of salespeople when they are selecting potential buyers. In order to examine applicant perceptions of gender effects on the various stages of the selling process and targeting efforts, comparisons will be made for: all respondents, for male respondents, for female respondents, and for male versus female respondents. This paper is important because it appears to be the first paper to address gender effects on selling and targeting from the applicant's point of view. Understanding applicants' perceptions of these issues could assist sales organizations in recruiting for sales positions and in training the sales force.

THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND RESEARCH HYPOTHESES

Several theories or paradigms that have been developed or proposed to explain the gender effect on sales performance. These theories or paradigms, which have served as a foundation for empirical gender studies in the selling field, are Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel and Turner, 1986); Self-Categorization Theory (Turner, 1982, 1985); and the Similarity-Attraction Paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne and Neuman, 1992; Graves and Powell, 1995). The similarity-attraction paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne and Neuman, 1992; Graves and Powell, 1995) suggests that individuals tend to be attracted to, or seek membership with, those (demographically) similar to themselves, leading to perceived attraction. Similarity is the degree to which members of a group are alike in terms of personal (or demographic) characteristics or other attributes (Byrne and Neuman, 1992; Smith, 1998). Thus, similarity constitutes an important basis of interpersonal attraction and of social integration and cohesion (Baron and Pfeffer, 1994). Byrne and Neuman (1992) state that gender similarity seems to have a very strong influence on perceived similarity and interpersonal attraction. This theory implies that there would be a perceived attraction between a salesperson and a buyer, based on perceived similarity.

Social identity theory (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel and Turner, 1986) advocates that belonging to a group creates a psychological state that confers social identity or a collective representation of self-identity and behavior. Under this theory, an individual's self-identity formation is partly a function of group membership and demographic similarity will have positive effects on performance by increasing interpersonal attraction and increasing cognitive biases. Tajfel (1982) states that based on social identity theory an individual's self-identity formation is partly a function of group membership. People in groups share experiences and attitudes, they like each other and understanding increases, and this interaction reinforces or ratifies one's own self image (McNeilly and Russ, 2000). In essence, social identity theory predicts that demographic similarity will have positive effects on performance by increasing interpersonal attraction and increasing cognitive biases (Linville and Jones 1980), which could lead to more open communication and decreased interpersonal tension. Social identity theory also provides some insight for the unexpected outcomes of some previous gender studies (Graves and Powell, 1995; Hardin et al., 2002).

An important and integral aspect of social identity theory involves self-categorization. Self-categorization theory (Turner, 1982, 1985)

suggests that individuals take socially defined categories into account when making evaluations about others, where those characteristics that are similar to self would likely be considered as positive and vice-versa. McNeilly and Russ (2000) point out that since demographic characteristics such as age and gender are observable and accessible, they are useful for self-categorization. This aspect of self-categorization theory indicates that social categories such as gender, age, and race (Messick and Mackie, 1989), can cause one to perceive oneself as similar to other members of a category or group and can trigger stereotyping of the out-group. Self-categorization also takes place during the formation of dyadic relationships (Benkhoff, 1997), where similarity facilitates communication, the development of greater trust, and satisfaction within relationships (Kanter, 1977; Smith, 1998).

These theories suggest that individuals maintain a positive self-identity by seeking to maximize inter-group distinctiveness and perceive out-group members as being less attractive (Jackson et al., 1992; Kramer, 1991). The implication of these theories is that salespeople will feel more confident during sales presentations and will primarily target prospects that are similar to themselves (i.e., same gender). Based on the above theories, Figure 1 presents a framework to investigate the potential effects of applicant gender (as a future salesperson) and buyer gender on each step of selling process and on targeting efforts. According to the Similarity-Attraction Paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne and Neuman, 1992; Graves and Powell, 1995), Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel and Turner, 1986), and Self-Categorization Theory (Turner, 1982, 1985), there should be a gender effect (or bias) on each selling step and on targeting (prospecting) efforts. These theories suggest that applicants, as potential salespeople, would perceive that they would be more successful at each step of the selling process (introducing themselves, giving presentations, answering questions, overcoming objections, engaging in a trial closing, and successfully closing the sale) with same gender buyers (gender matched buyer-seller) than with different gender buyers (gender mismatched buyer-seller). Based on these relationships, presented in Figure 1, and the first set of study objectives stated above, the following hypotheses are developed for each step of selling process:

H1: There will be a gender effect where (H1a) male applicants perceive they will more confidently introduce themselves to male buyers, and (H1b) female applicants perceive they will more confidently introduce themselves to female buyer.

H2: There will be a gender effect where (H2a) male applicants perceive they will more confidently make presentations to male buyers, and (H2b) female applicants perceive they will more confidently make presentations to female buyers.

H3: There will be a gender effect where (H3a) male applicants perceive they will be more confident in answering questions from male buyers, and (H3b) female applicants perceive they will be more confident in answering questions from female buyers.

H4: There will be a gender effect where (H4a) male applicants perceive they will be more confident in overcoming objections for not buying from male buyers, and (H4b) female applicants perceive they will be more confident in overcoming objections for not buying from female buyers.

H5: There will be a gender effect where (H5a) male applicants perceive they will be more confident in attempting a trial close with male buyers, and (H5b) female applicants perceive they will be more confident in attempting a trial close with female buyers.

H6: There will be a gender effect where (H6a) male applicants perceive they will be more successful in closing the sale with male buyers, and (H6b) female applicants perceive they will be more successful in closing the sale with female buyers.

Similarly, based on the relationships presented in Figure 1 and the second set of study objectives, the following hypotheses are developed for targeting prospective buyers and selling efforts:

H7: There will be a gender effect where (H7a) male applicants perceive they will more strongly attempt to sell to male buyers, and (H7b) female applicants perceive they will more strongly attempt to sell to female buyers.

H8: There will be a gender effect where (H8a) male applicants perceive they will be more successful in selling to male buyers, and (H8b) female applicants perceive they will be more successful in selling to female buyers.

H9: There will be a gender effect where (H9a) male applicants perceive they will prefer selling to male buyers, and (H9b) female applicants perceive they will prefer selling to female buyers.

H10: There will be a gender effect where (H10a) male applicants perceive they will prefer to focus their efforts on male buyers, and (H10b) female applicants perceive they will prefer to focus their efforts on female buyers.

METHODOLOGY

In order to accomplish the study objectives, a research instrument (questionnaire) was developed, which was adapted from Hardin et al. (2002) and further modified and improved to meet the objectives of this study. Specifically, the survey instrument included a number of questions concerning both sets of study objectives to examine student (as potential salespeople) perceptions of the gender effect during the selling process and targeting and selling efforts. For the first set of objectives, students were told that the selling process consisted of several steps. They were instructed to assume that they were giving a sales presentation and were asked several questions concerning their perceptions of how successful they thought they would be in dealing with male versus female potential buyers in each step of the selling process. For the second set of objectives, the instrument asked students whether they would more likely target to sell to, and be more successful in selling to, male versus female buyers, and finally, they were asked to indicate their preference for selling to, and focusing their efforts on, prospecting for male versus female buyers.

The research instrument included scale statements covering both sets of the study objectives. The instrument was submitted to several academicians with significant experience in the sales field and in scale development. After the instrument was improved with their suggestions, it was pre-tested with several students. The pretest provided very useful input for improving and clarifying the wording of the questions and establishing the face validity of the constructs (Churchill, 1979; Churchill and Iacobucci, 2005; Narver and Slater, 1990). The questions were measured on a semantic differential type of scale ranging from -5 to 5, where a score of -5=definitely males, a score of 0=equally likely, and a score of 5=definitely females. This scale offered some benefits. One benefit was that there was one scale item for each objective to cover both males and females. This reduced the number of scale items and made the instrument shorter. The second benefit is that each item somewhat forced the respondents to indicate their perceptions of the gender effect in a clear way. On the actual research instrument, the negative signs were omitted in order to eliminate any potential confusion and/or association with negative numbers. The instrument also included several demographic variables, such as gender, age, year in school (class) and major.

The above described instrument was administered to business students in the colleges of business at five universities in the United States; three were state universities while the other two were private universities. Before conducting the study, marketing professors at several U.S. universities were contacted and asked if they would be interested in participating in the study by administering the instrument in their classes. The professors who agreed to participate in the study were asked to randomly select two of their classes and administer the instrument to all the students in those classes. Special effort was made to select general business classes so that the study would include students with different majors as well as different classification levels. These classes were considered as clusters, and every student in each selected class completed the instrument. This type of sampling is called one-stage cluster sampling (Churchill and Iacobucci, 2005). This process produced 385 useable responses. The five universities, located in various parts of the United States, should offer a fairly good representation of business students across the United States. Since business (especially marketing) students are more likely to seek sales jobs, the responses from these students should provide useful information to help accomplish the research objectives of this study.

Respondent Profiles

The demographic profiles of the respondents show that 51.4% of the respondents are male and 48.6% are female, with an average age of 23.7 years. A distribution of student classification indicates that 1.0% are freshmen, 5.5% are sophomores, 28.2% are juniors, 39.9% are seniors, 24.3% are graduate students, and 1.0% are other. In terms of majors, 27.9% are majoring in marketing, 24.8% in management, 13.1% in accounting, 11.0% in finance, 1.6% in economics, 1.0% in computer science, 1.8% in management information systems, and 18.2% in other. While the survey was intended to include students from different business majors by targeting general business courses, the distribution of majors shows that marketing and management majors are the largest groups of respondents followed by accounting. These majors seem to be the most popular majors at many U.S. business schools.

RESULTS

The main objective of this study was to investigate applicant perceptions of the gender effect on selling performance during each step/stage of the selling process and targeting efforts for prospective buyers. In order to test whether there is a gender effect during any of the selling stages, a one sample t-test was conducted where the test value = 0. Separate analyses were conducted for all students, male students and female students, and the results are presented in Table 1. Since the scale used in the study ranged from "-5=definitely males" to "5=definitely females" with "0=equally likely," the sign of the mean shows the direction of any gender effect, and the t-significance would indicate if the gender effect was significant. Based on the results of the one sample test for all responses, students perceive that they would be more successful in making presentations to female buyers (mean of .41, p < .01), and answering questions from female buyers (mean of .30, p < .01). They also feel they would be somewhat more successful in overcoming objections from female buyers (mean of .19, p < .10). The results also indicate that students feel they would be equally successful with male and female buyers in introducing themselves, engaging in a trial close, and in closing the sale. The results suggest the existence of a perceived gender effect (bias) during some stages of selling process where students feel they would be more successful with female buyers than male buyers.

The mean scores of selling efforts in Table 1 show that students, if hired as sales persons, would prefer to attempt to sell to female buyers (mean of .43, p < .01) and believe they would be more successful selling to female buyers (mean of. 37, p < .01). These findings suggest the existence of a gender effect in targeting and selling to female buyers. In terms of targeting, the respondents indicated they would prefer selling to female buyers (mean of .34, p < .01). Also, when finding new buyers, the respondents would prefer to focus their efforts on female buyers (mean of .34, p < .01), assuming an equal number of male and female buyers are available. These findings further support the existence of a significant gender effect in favor of female buyers concerning the selling efforts/outcome of potential applicants.

In addition to a gender effect for all students (potential salespeople), separate analyses were conducted to examine whether there were gender effects for male students and female students, and the results are presented in Table 1. The results for male students indicate that the mean scores are not significant for any of the selling steps. These findings suggest that male students feel they will be equally successful in their selling efforts during each step of the selling process to both male and female buyers. These results do not support hypotheses H1a, H2a, H3a, H4a, H5a, and H6a. In Table 1, similar analyses for female students shows that females seem to feel they would be more successful in making presentations to female buyers (mean of .77, p < .01) and answering questions from female buyers (mean of .68, p < .01). Since the mean scores for the other selling steps are not significant, these results suggest that female students feel equally successful in their selling efforts during the overall selling process to both male and female buyers. Concerning female applicants, hypotheses H2b and H3b are supported, but H1b, H4b, H5b, and H6a are not supported.

Similarly, male and female students' perceptions of targeting efforts were analyzed and presented in Table 1. The positive signs of the means to all the responses to targeting questions suggest that male students would prefer to attempt to sell to female buyers (mean of .61), would be more successful in selling to female buyers (mean of .72), would prefer selling to female buyers (mean of .81), and would prefer to focus more of their efforts on female buyers (mean of .49). All of these results are significant at the p < .01 level. These findings reveal a significant gender effect for male students in their targeting efforts in favor of female buyers. However, the results are opposite of the predictions of the above theories and hypotheses; therefore, H7a, H8a, H9a, and H10a are not supported.

The results of a similar analysis for female students are also included in Table 1. Since none of the means for targeting questions are significant, female students don't appear to have any preference for; targeting male versus female buyers. These findings show that there is no gender effect (from female students) on targeting efforts for potential customers. As a result, hypotheses H7b, H8b, H9b, and H10b are not supported.

Comparison of Male versus Female Applicants

Also, the study compared male vs. female student perceptions of the gender effect during each step of the selling process, and the results are presented in Table 2. Comparisons of the mean scores for the selling steps suggest that there is no significant difference between male and female students in: confidently introducing themselves to either male or female buyers (p > .05); overcoming objections from male or female buyers (p > .05); engaging in a trial closing (p > .05); and successfully closing the sale (p > .05). However, the study did reveal a significant difference between male and female applicants for two steps of the selling process. The positive signs of the means for making a presentation indicate that both male and female students feel they would be more successful in giving sales presentations to female buyers. It appears that female students also feel they will be more successful (mean of .77) than male students (mean of .09) in making sales presentations to female buyers (p < .01). The results also indicate a significant difference between male and female students in answering questions from buyers (p < .01). A negative sign of the mean for male students indicates that feel they would be more successful in answering questions from male buyers, whereas the positive sign of the mean for female students indicate that they feel they would be more successful in answering questions from female buyers.

Concerning targeting efforts, the mean scores of .61 for male students and .23 for female students suggest that both genders would more strongly attempt (or target) to sell to female buyers. Male students seem to have a stronger preference to target female buyers than female students, where the difference is somewhat significant (p < .10). In terms of selling success, male students perceive they will be more successful in selling to female buyers (mean of .72), whereas female students perceive they will be more successful in selling to male buyers (-.03), where the means are significantly different (p < .01).

In addition, the results illustrate a significant difference between the selling preferences of male and female students (p < .01). If they had a choice, male students would prefer selling to female buyers (mean of .81), while female students would prefer to focus their efforts on finding new male buyers (mean of -.13). When finding a new buyer, both male and female students prefer to focus most of their effort on prospecting female buyers; however, the difference was not significant (p > .10). The results regarding selling efforts contradict the predictions of the Similarity-Attraction Paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne & Neuman, 1992; Graves & Powell, 1995). In fact, these findings are the opposite of the predictions of the theories used in this and prior studies. As suggested by Dwyer et al. (1998), it appears that gender similarity would not provide any advantage, and may even reduce sales opportunities.

DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS

Unlike prior gender studies in the sales field where gender effects were investigated from the recruiters' point of view or from the point of view of salespeople, this study examined gender effects on each step of the selling process and targeting potential customers from the applicant's perception (business student) as a potential salesperson. The results of the study indicate that the entire sample of students feel they would be more successful with female buyers than male buyers in making sales presentations, answering questions, and overcoming objections. Regarding the other steps of the selling process, students feel they would be equally successful in their selling efforts to both male and female buyers. Concerning targeting and prospecting efforts, the study found that all students would more strongly prefer to target female buyers, would be more successful selling to and prefer selling to female buyers and would prefer to focus more of their selling efforts on female buyer. These results indicate a significant gender effect (bias) for all students in favor of preferring female buyers in their selling process and targeting efforts. These findings imply that both male and female students may need to be trained in selling and targeting male buyers. This could be important in increasing sales.

The results by gender indicate that male students believe they will be equally successful with both male and female buyers at each stage of the selling process. Unlike the predictions of the Similarity-Attraction Paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne and Neuman, 1992; Graves and Powell, 1995), Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel and Turner, 1986); Self-Categorization Theory (Turner, 1982, 1985), these findings suggest a gender effect for male students during the selling process. These findings contradict the results of some previous studies (Crosby et al., 1990; Smith, 1998; Churchill et al, 1997; 1975), which suggested that male students should be more successful during each step of the selling process to same gender (male) buyers. However, the results are consistent with the findings of some other studies (Dwyer et al., 1998; Jones et al., 1998) that show that gender similarity does not seem to be a factor for sales performance. The implication is that male students would not need special training to be successful in selling to male or female buyers; rather they might be trained in improving their overall selling effectiveness.

However, the current study did find the existence of a gender effect for female students during two steps of the selling process. It seems that female students feel they would be more successful with female buyers than male buyer in making sales presentations and answering questions. These findings are consistent with the predictions of the Similarity-Attraction Paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne and Neuman, 1992; Graves and Powel, 1995), Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel and Turner, 1986) and Self-Categorization Theory (Turner, 1982, 1985). These theories suggest that individuals tend to be attracted to, or seek membership in, groups that are (demographically) similar to themselves, indicating that there would be a perceived attraction between a salesperson and a buyer based on perceived similarity. On the other hand, the study found no gender effect for female students during the other steps of the selling process. The results, therefore, provide limited support for these theories. The findings for both male and female students suggests that in selling to male and female buyers, male students would be successful during all stages of the sales process, while female students would be successful during certain stages. These findings could be helpful for managers in their recruiting and training efforts.

The comparisons of male students vs. female students found significant differences between the two genders for only two stages of the selling process. The results show that both male and female students feel they would be more successful in giving sales presentation to female buyers. However, female students have a stronger preference for female buyers. The study also found a significant gender difference between male and female students regarding answering buyer questions. Specifically, while male students feel they would be more successful in answering questions from male buyers versus female buyers, female students feel they would be more successful answering questions from female buyers than male buyers. There were no significant differences between male and female students for other steps of the selling process. Some of these findings are consistent with the predictions of the above gender effect theories, others are not. Also, some of these findings are consistent with the findings of Crosby et al. (1990), Dwyer et al. (1998), and Jones et al. (1998), while others are not consistent with their findings. The results of the current study show that the gender effect on each step of selling process does not follow any predictable, consistent pattern. As reported by Dwyer et al. (1998) and Jones et al. (1998), female salespeople seem to be just as effective as male salespeople, and gender similarity does not appear to be a significant factor in sales performance. In fact, contrary to the Access and Legitimacy Paradigm (Thomas and Ely, 1996), these results suggest that companies should probably not try to match seller gender with buyer gender.

Regarding targeting efforts, comparisons of male students vs. female students found significant differences between the two genders. The results show that both male and female students would more strongly attempt to sell to female buyers, with male students having a stronger preference. In addition, male students feel they would be more successful, and would prefer, selling to female buyers, whereas female students feel just the opposite. Again, these findings are not consistent with the predictions of the theories and prior research (Crosby et al., 1990; Dwyer et al., 1998; Jones et al., 1998).

Managerial Implications

This study examined applicant (business student) perceptions of gender effects during the selling process and targeting efforts. Understanding student perceptions of gender effects at each stage of the selling process and targeting efforts, rather than the sales outcome, provided a different perspective on gender effects. The results of the current study could have several managerial implications for companies in recruiting their sales force and in designing sales training programs. The first managerial implication deals with sales efforts during the selling process. As both male and female students believe they would be successful with both male and female buyers during all but a few steps of the selling process, matching the gender of the target market with the gender of a salesperson should not be a major factor in the recruiting/hiring decision. Companies may not gain any advantage by matching the gender of salespeople with buyer gender as suggested by previous studies (Crosby et al., 1990; Smith, 1998; Churchill et al, 1997; 1975) and by the Access and Legitimacy Paradigm (Thomas and Ely, 1996). To the contrary, as suggested by Dwyer et al. (1998) and Jones et al. (1998), companies could attract and recruit applicants from both genders resulting in hiring the best qualified persons for sales jobs. This could have a positive impact on the firm's sales force productivity and sales performance.

The second managerial implication is that companies could identify potential gender effects for each stage of the sales process, rather than considering the gender effect on overall sales outcomes as suggested by prior research (Dwyer et al., 1998, Jones et al., 1998). This would allow companies to have a better understanding of the gender effect on certain steps of selling process so that companies can design special training programs to deal with specific problem areas. For example, the results show a significant gender effect for female students who seem to feel more comfortable with female buyers in making sales presentations and answering questions. Once this is identified, companies could design training programs to improve female students success in dealing with male buyers.

The third managerial implication deals with targeting and selling success with potential buyers. It seems that students prefer to target female buyers, feel they would be more successful in selling to female buyers and prefer to sell to female buyers. The reason for such preference was not within scope of this study, but the implication is that companies might focus their prospecting efforts on potential female buyers in order to increase sales. Alternatively, they might have to train new recruits to improve their effectiveness in prospecting and selling to male buyers. These findings imply that companies may not gain any advantage by matching the gender of salespeople with buyer gender as suggested by the Access and Legitimacy Paradigm (Thomas and Ely, 1996). To the contrary, both male and female students appear to prefer to target to and sell to female buyers.

Finally, the results of this study show that, unlike the predictions of the Similarity-Attraction Paradigm (Byrne, 1971; Byrne & Neuman, 1992; Graves & Powell, 1995), Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1972, 1982; Tajfel & Turner, 1986), and Self-Categorization Theory (Turner, 1982, 1985), there is no consistent gender effect where same gender relationships would be more effective and preferable during the targeting and selling process. In order to create a successful sale force, companies must train their new recruits on targeting and successfully completing sales to male buyers. This is the same for targeting and for all stages of the selling process. These findings could be very valuable for companies in developing training programs for the specific problem areas identified.

REFERENCES

Arvey, R.D., & R.A. Faley (1988). Fairness in selecting employees (SecondEdition). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Baron, J.N. & J. Pfeffer (1994). The social psychology of organizations and inequality. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, (September), 190-209.

Benkhoff, B. (1997). A test of the HRM model: Good for employers and employees. Human Resource Management Journal, 7(4), 44-60.

Byrne, D. (1971). The Attraction Paradigm. New York: Academic Press.

Byrne, D. & J.H. Neuman (1992). The implications of attraction research for organizational issues. In K. Kelley (Ed.) Issues, Theory, and Research in Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 29-70). New York: Elsevier.

Churchill, G.A. Jr. (1979). A paradigm for developing better measures of marketing constructs. Journal of Marketing Research, 16(February), 64-73.

Churchill, G.A., Collins, R.H. & W.A. Strang (1975). Should retail salespersons be similar to their customers? Journal of Retailing, 51(Fall), 29-42.

Churchill, G.A., Jr., Ford, N.M & O.C. Walker, Jr. (1997). Sales Force Management (Fifth Edition). Chicago: Irwin.

Churchill, G.A. Jr., & D. Iacobucci (2005). Marketing Research: Methodological Foundations (Ninth Edition). United States: Thomson / South-Western.

Comer, L.B. & M.A. Jolson (1991). Perceptions of gender stereotypic behavior: An exploratory study of women in selling. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 11(Winter), 43-59.

Comer, L.B., Jolson, M.A., Dubinsky, A.J., & F.J. Yammarino (1995). When the sales manager is a woman: An exploration into the relationship between salespeople's gender and their responses to leadership styles. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 15(Fall), 17-32.

Crosby, L.A., Evans, K.R. & D. Cowles (1990). Relationship quality in services selling: An interpersonal influence perspective. Journal of Marketing, 54(July), 68-81.

Dwyer, S., Orlando, R. & C.D. Shepherd (1998). An exploratory study of gender and age matching in the salesperson-prospective customer dyad: Testing similarity-performance predictions. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 4(Fall), 55-69.

Fugate D.L., Decker, P.J. Decker & J.J. Brewer (1988). Women in professional selling: A human resource management perspective. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 8 (November), 33-41.

Futrell, C.M. (2006). Fundamentals of Selling (Ninth Edition). New York: McGraw-Hill/Irwin.

Gable, M. & B.J. Reed (1987). The current status of women in professional selling. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 7(May), 33-39.

Graves, L.M. & G.N. Powell (1988). An investigation of sex discrimination in recruiters' evaluations of actual applicants. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 20-29.

Graves, L.M. & G.N. Powell (1995). The effect of sex similarity on recruiters' evaluations of actual applicants: A test of the similarity attraction paradigm. Personnel Psychology, 48 (Spring), 85-98.

Hardin, R.J., Reding, K.F. & M.H. Stocks (2002). The effect of gender on the recruitment of entry-level accountants. Journal of Managerial Issues, 14(2), 251-266.

Jackson, S.E., Stone, V.K. & E.B. Alvarez (1992). Socialization amidst diversity: The impact of demographics on work team old timers and newcomers. Research on Organizational Behavior, 15, 45-109.

Jolson, M.A. & L.B. Comer (1992). Predicting the effectiveness of industrial saleswomen. Industrial Marketing Management, 21(February), 69-76.

Jones, E., Moore, J., Stanaland, A. & R. Wyatt (1998). Salesperson race and gender and the access and legitimacy paradigm: Does difference make a difference. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 18(4), 7188.

Kanter, R. (1977). Some effects of proportions on group life: Skewed sex ratios and responses to token women. American Journal of Sociology, 82, 965-990.

Kanuk, L. (1978). Women in industrial selling. Journal of Marketing, 42(January), 87-91.

Kramer, R.M. (1991). Intergroup relations and organizational dilemmas: The role of categorization process. Research on Organizational Behavior, 13, 191-228.

Linville, P. & E. Jones (1980), Polarized appraisal of outgroup members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 689-703.

Lucas, A. (1996). Race matters. Sales & Marketing Management, 148(September), 50-62.

Manning, G.L. & B.L. Reece (1995). Selling Today. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

McNeilly, K. M. & F. Russ (2000). Does relational demography matter in a personnel selling? Journal of Personnel Selling & Sales Management, 4(Fall), 279-288.

Messick, D.M. & D.M. Mackie (1989). Intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 45-81.

Narver, J. & S.F. Slater (1990). The effects of a market orientation on business profitability. Journal of Marketing, 54(October), 20-35.

Pink Ghetto' in Sales for Women. (1988). Sales and Marketing Management, 140(July), 80.

Powell, G.N. (1987). The effects of sex and gender on recruitment. Academy of Management Review, 12, 731-743.

Russ, F.A. & K.M. McNeilly (1988). Has sex stereotyping disappeared? A study of perceptions of women and men in sales. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 8(November), 43-54.

Smith, J.B. (1998). Buyer-seller relationships: Similarity, relationship management, and quality. Psychology and Marketing, 15(January), 3-21.

Swan, J.E. & C.M. Futrell (1978). Men versus women in industrial sales: A performance gap. Industrial Marketing Management, 7(December), 369-373.

Swan, J.E., Rink, D.R., Kiser, G.E. & W.S. Martin (1984). Industrial buyer image of the saleswomen. Journal of Marketing, 48(Winter), 110-116.

Tajfel, H. (1982). Social Identity and Intergroup Relations. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Tajfel, H. & J.C. Turner (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W.G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

Thomas, D.A. & R.J. Ely (1996). Making differences matter: A new paradigm managing diversity. Harvard Business Review, 74(September-October), 79-91.

Turner, J.C. (1982). Toward a cognitive redefinition of the social group. Social Identity and Intergroup Relations, 12, 15-40.

Turner, J.C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social-cognitive theory of group behavior. Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research, 2, 77-121.

Yammarino, F.J., Dubinsky, A.J., Comer, L.B., & M.A. Jolson (1997). Women and transformational and contingent reward leadership: A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 40(February), 205-222.

Weitz, B. A. (1981). Effectiveness in sales interactions: A contingency framework. Journal of Marketing, 45(Winter), 85-103.

Musa Pinar, Valparaiso University

J. Russell Hardin, University of South Alabama

Zeliha Eser, Baskent University
Table 1: Students' Perceptions of Gender Effect in Selling Process and
Targeting Potential Customers

               Selling Process                      All respondents

                                                Mean     St.      p
                                                        Dev.    value

Confidently introducing self to potential       -0.1    2.11    0.679
buyers

Making the sales presentation                   0.41      2       0

Answering any questions the client might have    0.3    1.79      0

Overcoming various objections to buying         0.19    1.98    0.07

Engaging in a trial closing                       0     1.97    0.834

Successfully closing the sale                   0.02    2.03    0.859

        Targeting Potential Customers           Mean     St.      p
                                                        Dev.    value

If hired as a salesperson, most strongly        0.43    2.15      0
attempt to sell to men or women

As a sales person, most successful selling to   0.37    2.59      0
men or women

If you had a choice, you would most strongly    0.34    2.55    0.01
prefer selling to?

When finding new buyers, you would

prefer to focus most of your efforts on males   0.34    2.02      0
vs. females, assuming an equal number of
buyers are available?

               Selling Process                     Male respondents

                                                Mean     St.      p
                                                        Dev.    value

Confidently introducing self to potential       -0.11   2.04    0.44
buyers

Making the sales presentation                   0.09    1.82    0.504

Answering any questions the client might have   -0.04   1.76    0.776

Overcoming various objections to buying         0.26    1.95    0.06

Engaging in a trial closing                     0.13    2.01    0.354

Successfully closing the sale                   0.09    2.06    0.55

        Targeting Potential Customers           Mean     St.      p
                                                        Dev.    value

If hired as a salesperson, most strongly        0.61     1.3      0
attempt to sell to men or women

As a sales person, most successful selling to   0.72    2.41      0
men or women

If you had a choice, you would most strongly    0.81    2.52      0
prefer selling to?

When finding new buyers, you would

prefer to focus most of your efforts on males   0.49    2.07      0
vs. females, assuming an equal number of
buyers are available?

               Selling Process                    Female respondents

                                                Mean     St.      p
                                                        Dev.    value

Confidently introducing self to potential       0.01    2.18    0.946
buyers

Making the sales presentation                   0.77    2.11      0

Answering any questions the client might have   0.68    1.75      0

Overcoming various objections to buying         0.13    2.01    0.398

Engaging in a trial closing                     -0.16    1.9    0.244

Successfully closing the sale                   -0.07     2     0.658

        Targeting Potential Customers           Mean     St.      p
                                                        Dev.    value

If hired as a salesperson, most strongly        0.23     2.3    0.182
attempt to sell to men or women

As a sales person, most successful selling to   -0.03   2.71    0.971
men or women

If you had a choice, you would most strongly    -0.13   2.48    0.461
prefer selling to?

When finding new buyers, you would

prefer to focus most of your efforts on males   0.17    1.94    0.244
vs. females, assuming an equal number of
buyers are available?

Scale: Definitely Males -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 Definitely Females

Table 2: Comparisons of Male vs. Female Applicant Perceptions of
Gender Effect in Selling Process and Targeting Potential Customers

                                            Male Applicant
             Selling Process
                                            Mean    St. Dev

Confidently introducing self to potential   -0.11    2.04
buyers

Making the sales presentation               0.09     1.82

Answering any questions the client might    -0.04    1.76
have

Overcoming various objections buying        0.26     1.95

Engaging in a trial closing                 0.13     2.01

Successfully closing the sale               0.09     2.06

      Targeting Potential Customers         Mean    St. Dev

If hired as a salesperson, most strongly    0.61       2
attempt to sell to man or women

As a sales person, most successful          0.72     2.41
selling to man or women

If you had a choice, you would most         0.81     2.52
strongly prefer selling to?

When finding new buyers, you would prefer
to focus

most of your efforts on males or females,   0.49     2.07
assuming an equal number of buyers are
available?

                                            Female Applicant   p value
             Selling Process
                                            Mean    St. Dev.

Confidently introducing self to potential   0.01      2.18      0.569
buyers

Making the sales presentation               0.77      2.11      0.001

Answering any questions the client might    0.68      1.75        0
have

Overcoming various objections buying        0.13      2.01      0.501

Engaging in a trial closing                 -0.16     1.9       0.14

Successfully closing the sale               -0.07      2        0.464

      Targeting Potential Customers         Mean    St. Dev    p value

If hired as a salesperson, most strongly    0.23      2.3       0.082
attempt to sell to man or women

As a sales person, most successful          -0.03     2.71      0.004
selling to man or women

If you had a choice, you would most         -0.13     2.48        0
strongly prefer selling to?

When finding new buyers, you would prefer
to focus

most of your efforts on males or females,   0.17      1.94      0.121
assuming an equal number of buyers are
available?

Scale: Definitely Males -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 Definitely Females

Figure 1: Effect of Salesperson and Buyer Gender on Selling Process
and Targeting Efforts      Applicant/Salesperson

                           Male               Female

Potential Buyer   Male     Gender Match:      Gender Mismatch:
                             Perceived          Perceived
                             Similarity         Dissimilarity

                  Female   Gender Mismatch:   Gender Match:
                             Perceived          Perceived
                             Dissimilarity      Similarity

                           Steps in Selling Process
                           Introducing
                           Presentation
                           Answering questions
                           Overcoming objections
                           Attempting trial close
                           Closing the sale
                           Targeting effort
                           Attempting to sell
                           More successful in selling
                           Prefer to sell to
                           Prefer to focus efforts on
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有