Word-of-author advertising in textbooks: the role of brand familiarity and placement repetition on recall and recognition.
Brennan, Ian ; McCalman, David
INTRODUCTION
Brand placement in the media of popular culture has burgeoned in
recent years, projected to reach over 6.3 billion dollars in spending
for 2008 (Education Policy Studies Laboratory 2005). In a Bruce
Springsteen music video, Born in the USA, the music is accompanied by
visual images of Miller beer. Mountain Dew has been liberally sprinkled
throughout movies such as Secret Window, while Aquafina caught attention
in the hit movie, National Treasure. References to branded products are
not, however, confined to the media of entertainment; brand names and
logos have also infiltrated academic textbooks. Indeed, a recent
mathematics text contains visual or verbal references to Sony, Spalding,
Disney, Warner Bros., Burger King and McDonalds (Hayes, 1999).
Friedman (1985, 1986, 1987, 1991) uses the term word-of-author
advertising (WOA advertising) to describe the appearance of brand names
in screenplays, television dramas, novels, lyrics and other
"popular cultural products." WOA advertising may be motivated
by creative considerations, such as the desire to lend verisimilitude to
a drama. In contrast, when WOA advertising results from commercial
considerations (i.e., brand owners are charged for a brand's
appearance), the practice is considered brand placement (Karrh 1998).
WORD-OF-AUTHOR ADVERTISING IN TEXTBOOKS
Authors of textbooks often include practical applications of
theoretical content; consequently, many texts are replete with WOA
advertising. For example, the text Global Marketing Strategies_(1998)
contains thirty-two pages that refer to Ford or Ford products, while
references to Wal-Mart adorn thirteen pages of the text Electronic
Commerce: A Managerial Perspective (2000). Hewlett Packard is featured
on eight of the pages of the text Organizational Behavior (1998). Brand
names also appear in non-business texts; for instance, Mathematics:
Applications and Connections (1999) is sprinkled with references to
Sony, Spalding, Disney, Warner Bros., Burger King and McDonalds (Hays
1999). Extant research has not addressed the potential benefits that
might accrue to firms that receive WOA advertising. In the present paper
we examine the effects of WOA advertising on consumer memory.
Recent research in marketing considers recall and recognition to be
measures of explicit memory (Law and Braun 2000, Shapiro and Krishnan
2001). In contrast, implicit memory describes changes in task
performance that are induced by exposure to a stimulus without the task
performer being able to recollect the exposure episode (Schacter 1987).
Thus an implicit memory of an advertising exposure may influence brand
choice even when the consumer is unable to recognize the advertiser
(Shapiro and Krishnan 2001). Consequently, we consider the impact of WOA
advertising on measures of both explicit (advertiser recall and
recognition) and implicit (brand choice) memory.
Researchers have examined the levels of brand recall and
recognition generated by brands placed in movies (Babin & Carder,
1996; Brennan, Dubas & Babin, 1999; Gupta & Lord, 1998;) and
television programs (Law & Braun, 2000). The extant literature on
audience recollections of brand placements in movies has concentrated on
the effects of variations in the presentational characteristics of the
placement. These presentational characteristics include the mode of
placement exposure--audio-visual versus visual-only (Gupta & Lord,
1998; Law & Braun, 2000, Brennan and Babin 2004), the time on-screen
(Brennan, Dubas & Babin, 1999), and the relative prominence of the
brand-placement (Brennan, Dubas & Babin, 1999; Gupta & Lord,
1998; Law & Braun, 2000). Given the tendency for WOA references to
be repeated within a text, the present paper also considers the effects
on recall, recognition and choice of one presentational
characteristic--the extent of WOA advertising repetition. Our study also
considers the effects of (pre-study) brand familiarity with a WOA
advertiser.
Word-of-Author Advertising and Explicit Memory
Prior research in advertising suggests that familiar brands enjoy a
recall advantage over brands that are less familiar (Johar & Pham,
1999; Nedgunadi, 1990). The recall advantage for familiar WOA
advertisers over their less familiar counterparts may occur at the time
of retrieval and/or during encoding. With respect to retrieval, the
spreading-activation model of memory (Anderson, 1983; Collins &
Loftus, 1975) asserts that brand retrieval may be triggered by the
memory of a node (e.g., product category) with which it is associated,
with stronger associations appearing to enhance brand retrieval (Lee
& Sternthal, 1999; Negunadi, 1990). Accordingly, a familiar brand
should have a greater likelihood of being retrieved than an unfamiliar
brand, given the stronger associations of the former with the product
category (Negunadi, 1990).
With respect to encoding, subjects who encounter a familiar name
(e.g., a familiar WOA advertiser) are also more likely to encode the
encounter into memory than is the case when they encounter an unfamiliar
name. In a series of experiments, Watkins, LeCompte & Kim (2000)
demonstrate that a list of words which occur more frequently in everyday
language (e.g., letter, earth, captain, hotel and flower) are more
recallable than a list of words which occur more rarely (e.g., proctor,
kilt, mango, scooter and cobra),, even when neither list shares an
association with an underlying node that would facilitate retrieval.
Thus, WOA advertisers are more likely to enjoy both an encoding and
retrieval advantage over their less familiar less familiar counterparts.
Advertising research also suggests that the repetition of an
advertisement tends to increase brand familiarity (Berger & Mitchel,
1989) and brand name recall (Unnava & Burnkrant, 1991). Clearly, the
additional opportunity to encode a WOA advertising reference should
enhance its retrieval over a comparable reference that receives only a
single execution. Whether WOA advertiser repetition magnifies the
anticipated recall advantage for familiar WOA advertisers over their
less unfamiliar counterparts is an issue of calibration. The additional
encoding opportunity enhances the possibility that the encoding
advantage of the familiar WOA advertiser is realized; for example,
irrespective of familiarity, single WOA advertising references may not
be encoded by a reader with inconsistent concentration. In contrast,
potential ceiling effects on the recall of familiar WOA advertisers may
limit the gains associated with repetition. In experiment one we test
the following hypotheses:
H1: The unaided-recall of familiar WOA advertisers will be greater
than the unaided-recall of less familiar WOA advertisers.
H2: WOA advertising repetition will increase the unaided-recall of
WOA advertisers.
EXPERIMENT 1
Method
Subjects and design
The forty-one subjects who participated in quasi-experiment one
(experiment 1) received extra-credit in an undergraduate marketing
course. The subjects, undergraduates at a southeastern university, were
asked to bring their Principles of Marketing textbooks to the
experiment. Upon arrival they were told that they would take part in a
study designed to measure the effectiveness of alternative distance
learning programs. In phase one of the experiment, subjects were
informed that the study was designed to measure the average level of
knowledge that was likely to be gained by a class that a textbook
chapter prior to an on-line instructional session. Consistent with the
guise, the subjects were informed that after reading the New Product
Development chapter from their text, they would be asked a series of
questions designed to measure their knowledge of new product
development. The content contained in the reading had not previously
been discussed in the course, and subjects were screened to ensure that
they had not previously read the relevant chapter. The subjects were
told that they had about 35 minutes to read the chapter. At the end of
the reading period (phase one), the subjects completed a ten-minute
filler task that was consistent with the guise of the study. In phase
two of the experiment, the subjects were asked to write down the names
of all the companies that had been referred to in the reading.
Coding
A pretest required subjects, who did not take part in the
experiments, to rate the familiarity of each firm that received one or
two WOA advertisements in the chapter which was to serve as the
experimental stimulus in experiment one. Subjects were asked to consider
whether each firm was an industry leader. A post experimental test
confirmed that subjects rated firms deemed to be "leaders in their
industry" as significantly higher in familiarity.
Each correct, unaided, WOA advertiser recollection reported by each
subject in phase two of the experiment was coded into one of four
categories developed in the pretest: familiar-firm, one WOA
advertisement; familiar-firm, two WOA advertisements; unfamiliar-firm,
one WOA advertisement; unfamiliar-firm, two WOA advertisements. The
number of WOA advertiser recollections in each category was recorded for
each subject. Next, to counteract the potential for the presence of the
higher base-rate of familiar (versus unfamiliar) WOA advertisers in the
stimulus to contaminate the results, an unaided-recall score for each
subject was created by dividing the number of WOA advertisers recalled
by each subject in each category by the total number WOA advertisers
appearing in the assigned chapter in that category (familiar-firm, one
WOA advertisement = 13 firms; familiar-firm, two WOA advertisements = 4
firms; unfamiliar-firm, one WOA advertisement = 5 firms;
unfamiliar-firm, two WOA advertisements = 2 firms). Thus, the coding
procedure for phase two resulted in a 2 x 2 (WOA advertiser familiarity
x WOA ad repetition) design in which all four evaluations were collected
within-subjects.
Results
Umesh, Peterson McCann-Nelson and Vaidyanathan (1996) note that
when research questions call for comparisons of cell means (rather than
a comparison of residual effects present in cell means after main
effects have been extracted), that it is inappropriate to conduct
comparisons of cell means on the basis of a significant omnibus test of
interaction in the ANOVA model. Accordingly, a planned-comparison
analysis was performed on the combinations of the four cell means
relevant to the hypotheses tests [Wilks' Lambda = .41; F(3, 38) =
18.14, p < .001].
H1 contends that the unaided-recall for familiar WOA advertisers
will be superior to the unaided-recall obtained by unfamiliar
advertisers. The results support H1. Subjects exhibited a higher recall
of WOA advertisers when the WOA advertisement referred to a familiar
firm than when the WOA advertisement referred to a unfamiliar firm
[M's = 9.99 versus 0.61; F(1, 40) = 46.85, p < .001].
H2 contends that unaided-recall for a WOA advertiser will be
positively related to WOA advertising repetition. Consistent with H2,
subjects exhibited a higher recall of WOA advertisers when the
advertisement was repeated in comparison with the single execution
[M's = 7.32 versus 3.28; [F.bar](1, 40) = 9.36, p < .004]. An
analysis of H2 within WOA advertiser-size, however, revealed that the
increase in the recall of WOA advertisers that could be attributed to
advertisement repetition was significant only for familiar WOA
advertisers [M's = 13.41 versus 6.57; [F.bar](1, 40) = 8.14, p <
.007]. In contrast, recall of unfamiliar WOA advertisers was not
significantly increased by WOA advertisement repetition [M's = 1.22
versus 0.00; [F.bar](1, 40) = 1.00, p > .323, see Table]. Thus,
familiarity appears to moderate the impact of WOA advertisement
repetition on unaided-recall.
Discussion
The results of experiment one indicate that familiar WOA
advertisers enjoy an unaided-recall advantage over unfamiliar WOA
advertisers. The effects of WOA advertisement repetition appear to be
moderated by familiarity. Subjects who were given an additional
opportunity to process the WOA advertisement significantly increased
their unaided-recall of familiar WOA advertisers. In contrast, the
unaided-recall of unfamiliar WOA advertisers was not significantly
increased by the repetition of the WOA advertisement.
The results of experiment one are not inconsistent with the notion
that familiar WOA advertisers enjoy both encoding and retrieval
advantages over their unfamiliar counterparts. The experiment does not,
however, demonstrate that familiar WOA advertisers enjoy an encoding
advantage over their unfamiliar counterparts; the superior recall
exhibited by the familiar WOA advertisers may have resulted entirely
from superior retrieval.
The notion that superior retrieval alone could explain the recall
advantage enjoyed by familiar WOA advertisers would require the
frequency effect to disappear. A recent study by Watkins, LeCompte &
Kim. (2000) indicates that the frequency effect disappears and, may
indeed reverse itself-- the mixed list paradox--when two conditions are
fulfilled. First, the familiar and rare words must be presented on the
same (mixed) list. Second, subjects must process the words under the
expectation that they will be subjected to a recall test (Delosh &
McDaniel, 1996; Watkins, LeCompte & Kim, 2000). Ostensibly, the two
conditions that give rise to the mixed-list paradox are likely to be
present when subjects process a text that contains WOA advertisers.
First, rather than appearing in separate parts of a text, references to
familiar and unfamiliar WOA advertisers are likely to be mixed within
the text. Second, although the recreational reading of educational texts
by students is not unknown, the reading of most students is likely to be
motivated by an impending examination. Notwithstanding such arguments,
we suspect that the mixed-list paradox is unlikely to emerge when
students anticipate being tested on the arguments or principles
discussed in a text (as is likely to be the case in practice), rather
than on their ability to recall the illustrators (i.e., WOA advertisers)
of those arguments. Accordingly, we contend that familiar WOA
advertisers will enjoy an encoding advantage over their unfamiliar
counterparts. In experiment two, by employing a recognition test that
attempts to control for the effects of retrieval, we attempt to
illuminate the effects of WOA advertiser-size on encoding.
EXPERIMENT 2
The extent to which the results obtained in experiment one may
generalize to WOA advertiser recognition is addressed in experiment two.
We anticipate that the unaided-recall advantages demonstrated for
familiar WOA advertisers over unfamiliar WOA advertisers in experiment
one will also extend to recognition. Such a view is consistent with the
idea that familiar WOA advertisers will enjoy encoding advantages over
their unfamiliar counterparts (i.e., the frequency effect), and
inconsistent with the mixed-list paradox. The effects of repetition on
WOA advertiser recognition are also expected to be consistent with those
reported for the effects of repetition on unaided-recall in experiment
one.
H3: A familiar WOA advertiser will attain a higher level of
recognition than that attained by a less familiar WOA advertiser.
H4: WOA advertisement repetition will induce higher levels of WOA
advertiser recognition.
Method
Subjects and design
Thirty-one subjects participated in a 2 x 2 (WOA advertiser
familiarity x WOA ad repetition) within-subjects design. The
experimental guise and text stimulus from experiment one were
re-employed in quasi-experiment two. (experiment 2). In addition, two
firms from each of the four experimental conditions were employed to
create the WOA advertiser familiarity and WOA ad conditions. Subject
recognition scores were calculated by averaging the recognition scores
for the two WOA advertisers in each treatment condition.
The font and font-size employed in the WOA reference for each of
the eight firms was identical. Although the experimental stimulus
contained a number of graphs and illustrations, none referred to the WOA
advertisers selected for experiment two. In an attempt to control for
the effects of primacy and recency in the recollection task, each target
WOA reference was both preceded and succeeded by at least six other WOA
references that were not involved in the recognition task. When
selecting the WOA advertisers for experiment 2, care was taken to
balance the valence of the author's comments associated with the
WOA references (across familiarity and within each repetition
treatment). The single WOA advertisement execution conditions each
contained a firm with one positive and one neutral reference. Within
each WOA repetition condition, each firm received one positive and one
neutral reference. The neutral references typically associated a firm
with a particular new product development technique (e.g., a firm used a
concept test to create a brand of dog food). The positive references
associated a firm with a successful new product or a pleasant
characteristic (e.g., a firm's product was described as
"litter free").
Decoy-foils
A number of studies have examined the extent to which self-reported
recollections are influenced by question characteristics (Menon, 1993;
Schwarz, 1990; Schwarz, Hippler, Deutsch & Strack, 1985). Recent
research in event-sponsor recognition (Johar & Pham, 1999) suggests
that recognition based on direct retrieval of the event-sponsor
association from memory may be supplemented by the use of constructive
recognition strategies. These constructive recognition strategies rely
upon inferences derived from the relative size of the sponsor vis-a-vis
the decoy foil that is present at the time of recognition (Johar &
Pham, 1999). For example, a subject who cannot retrieve an event-sponsor
from memory may make an inference based upon the relative size of the
two candidates. In particular, Johar & Pham contend that the larger
of the two candidate event-sponsors may be selected on the basis that
the larger firm is more likely to have the financial resources that
would permit event sponsorship.
In the case of experiment two in the present study, we controlled
for the possibility that the relative familiarity of the WOA advertiser
vis-a-vis the decoy firm (foil) might serve as a retrieval cue. For
example, if the foil is dissimilar in familiarity (distinctive) to the
WOA advertiser, subject recognition of both familiar and unfamiliar WOA
advertisers may be artificially inflated. In particular, assume that in
spite of being unable to recall the name of a WOA advertiser, a subject
is nevertheless able to retrieve two cues: (1) the industry from which
the two candidates are drawn that was employed to illustrate an argument
in the chapter reading, (2) whether the firm used in the illustration
was familiar or unfamiliar. If a familiar WOA advertiser were pitted
against a distinctive (unfamiliar) foil, or an unfamiliar WOA advertiser
pitted against a distinctive (familiar) foil, the WOA advertiser could
be selected constructively (by a process of elimination). These
retrieval cues do not facilitate selection by elimination when WOA
advertisers are pitted against foils that are non-distinctive (i.e.,
similar in familiarity to the WOA advertiser). Accordingly, after the
results of a pretest, eight non-distinctive foils (from the same
industry as their respective WOA advertiser) appeared as decoys on the
recognition task.
Results
A planned comparison analysis was performed [Wilks' Lambda =
.47; F(3,28) = 10.29 p < .001]. H3 contends that familiar WOA
advertiser will attain higher levels of recognition than unfamiliar WOA
advertisers when the judgement environment involves non-distinctive
foils. The results of the planned comparison support H3. Subjects
recognized a significantly higher number of familiar (versus unfamiliar)
WOA advertisers [M's = 1.85 versus 1.58; F(1, 30) = 17.85, p <
.001, see Table].
H4 contends that the repetition of a WOA advertisement will result
in higher levels of recognition when the judgement environment involves
non-distinctive foils. The results support H4 [M's = 1.81 versus
1.63; F(1, 30) = 4.67, p < .04]. An analysis of H4 within WOA
advertiser-size, however, reveals that the increase in recognition
associated with repetition was marginally significant for familiar WOA
advertisers [M's = 1.94 versus 1.77; F(1, 30) = 2.95, p < .1].
In contrast, subjects recognition of unfamiliar WOA advertisers was not
significantly increased by WOA advertisement repetition [M's = 1.68
versus 1.48; F(1, 30) = 1.67, p > .205 see Table]. The moderating
influence of familiarity on the effects of WOA advertisement is
directionally similar to that observed in experiment one. Finally, it
should be noted that recognition accuracy differed from chance in all
four treatment conditions (p's < .05).
Discussion
The results of experiment two indicate that familiar WOA
advertisers obtain greater recognition than unfamiliar WOA advertisers.
The repetition of a WOA advertisement in a text also induces higher
levels of recognition in comparison with the single execution. The
moderating role of familiarity on the effect of WOA advertisement
repetition on recognition is directionally similar to that which was
observed for unaided-recall in the first experiment.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
For WOA advertisements that appeared in a college textbook, our
study investigated the influence of the familiarity of a firm appearing
in a WOA advertisement and the repetition of the advertisement on recall
and recognition.
The results of our experiments indicate that the WOA advertisement
of a familiar firm enjoys an advantage over that of an unfamiliar firm
with respect to both recall and recognition. Further, our results
indicate that the effect of WOA advertisement repetition on recall is
moderated by the size of the firm that is referenced in the WOA
advertisement. Specifically, familiar WOA advertisers benefited from a
significant increase in recall (and marginally significant increase in
recognition) when subjects were afforded an additional opportunity to
encode the WOA reference. In contrast, the recall and recognition of
unfamiliar WOA advertisers were unaffected by WOA advertisement
repetition. Furthermore, our results demonstrate that even when denied
the recall advantage that familiarity generates via superior retrieval
(Negunadi, 1990), the encoding advantage enjoyed by familiar WOA
advertisers is sufficient to induce a recognition advantage over
unfamiliar WOA advertisers.
Theoretical implications.
Within the domain of event-sponsor identification, research by
Johar & Pham (1999) indicates that sponsor recognition accuracy is
not significantly affected by sponsor-size when non-distinctive foils
are present at the time of both encoding and retrieval. Johar & Pham
exposed subjects to fictitious newspaper articles about sporting events
that referred not only to the sponsor of the event, but also to the
names of three other firms (from which foils were selected) from the
same industry as the event sponsor. Johar and Pham had expected the
recognition advantage associated with sponsor-size to diminish (but not
disappear) when non-distinctive foils replaced distinctive foils on the
recognition instrument. Their premise was that subjects unable to
directly retrieve the event-sponsor would be denied the opportunity to
constructively identify the event sponsor through the use of a
heuristic: namely, that familiar companies, with extensive promotional
budgets, would be more likely to be involved in sponsorship.
Nevertheless, Johar & Pham anticipated that, as a result of superior
encoding of the event-sponsor relationship, familiar sponsors would
still retain a direct retrieval advantage over their less familiar
counterparts. Accordingly, the authors were surprised to find that the
recognition advantage associated with sponsor-size disappeared when
event-sponsors were pitted against non-distinctive foils during the
recognition task. Our findings suggest that an encoding advantage for
familiar sponsors may emerge when non-distinctive foils are absent at
the time of encoding and present only when a judgement of recognition is
required.
Practical Implications
For managers involved in brand placement transactions, our findings
draw attention to the potential for the effectiveness of brand
placements to be influenced not only by presentational characteristics
(Brennan, Dubas & Babin, 1999; Gupta & Lord, 1998; Law &
Braun, 2000), but also by variation in an audience characteristic: brand
familiarity.
Our results suggest that marketing mangers with brand recall and
brand recognition objectives should explore opportunities to place WOA
advertisements in books. Walsh (2000) notes that some educational
institutions are partially funded by companies who are granted
advertising opportunities within the scholastic environment. For
example, with the exception of New York, all the school systems in the
USA participated in a program that incorporated twelve minutes of
Channel One programming and advertising into the daily curriculum; in
exchange, the distributors of Channel One programming provided the
school systems with free audio-visual equipment (Wyatt 2000b). School
systems have also bartered advertising for computers and have permitted
advertising on school buses, school menus, school hallways and textbook
covers (Rassmussen, 1997; Stark, 2000; Wyatt, 2000a). This evidence
suggests, that at least within the United States, a market for texts
containing sponsored WOA advertising could develop if the publisher or
author of the texts were to offer the adopting educational institutions
some form of consideration (e.g., funding for an educational program or
a reduction in the purchase price of affected textbooks).
Directions for Future Research
In our quasi-experiments differences in pre-study brand familiarity
were measured. Measured variables enhance the ecological validity of an
experimental design (Menon, 1993); however, such designs suffer from the
fact that treatment conditions may differ from one another on variables
other than those that were measured (i.e. the results are
correlational). Our use of multiple firms within each treatment
condition mitigates this threat to the internal validity of the
research; it could be further alleviated by replications that utilize
alternative sets of WOA advertisers.
Future research might examine WOA advertising within the realm of
the novel. Since World War II, the identification of non-fictional
products has become increasingly prevalent within the genre of fiction
(Friedman, 1985). References to branded products not only add
verisimilitude to the environments inhabited by the characters, but also
assist an author in expediting the communication of their social
backgrounds and personality traits (Karrh, 1998; Delorme & Reid,
1999). Accordingly, future research might consider the extent to which
the patronage of leading characters in a novel may condition attitudes
(Gorn, 1982) towards the companies and products that feature in WOA
advertisements. An assessment of such conditioning effects is likely to
be of particular interest to marketers with brands that compete in
markets that lack a dominant brand--markets where conditioning has been
shown to influence brand choice (Baker, 1999; Miniard, Sirdeshmukh &
Innis, 1992).
REFERENCES
Anderson, J. R. (1983) 'A spreading activation theory of
memory', Journal of Verbal Learning and Learning Behavior, 22,
261-295.
Babin, L. A., & Carder, S. T. (1996) 'Viewers'
recognition of brands placed within a film', International Journal
of Advertising, 15, 140-151.
Baker, W. E. (1999) 'When can affective conditioning and mere
exposure directly influence brand choice', Journal of Advertising,
28, 31-46.
Berger, I. E., & Mitchell, A. A. (1989) 'The effect of
attitude accessibility, attitude confidence, and the attitude behavior
relationship', Journal of Consumer Research, 16, 269-279.
Bornstein, R F., (1989) 'Exposure and affect: Overview and
meta-analysis of research, 1968-1987', Psychological Bulletin, 106,
265-289.
Brennan, I., Dubas K. M. & Babin L. A. (1999) 'The
influence of product-placement type and exposure-time on
product-placement recognition', International Journal of
Advertising, 18, 323-337.
Brennan, I & Babin L. A. (2004) Brand placement recognition:
The influence of presentation mode and brand familiarity," Journal
of Promotion Management, 10, 185-202.
Bazerman, M. H. (1994) Judgement in Managerial Decision Making (3rd
ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Collins, W., Dritsas , L., Frey, P., Howard, A. C., McClain, K.,
Molina, D., Moore-Harris, B., Ott, J. M., Pelfrey, R., Price J., Smith,
B., & Wilson P. S. (1999) Mathematics: Applications and Connections.
NewYork: Glencoe/McGraw-Hill.
Collins, A M., & Loftus, F. (1975) 'A spreading activation
theory of semantic processing', Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.
Delorme, D E., & Reid, L. N. (1999) 'Moviegoers
experiences and interpretations of brands in films revisited',
Journal of Advertising, 28, 71-95.
Delosh, E. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996) 'The role of order
information in free recall: application to the word frequency
effect', Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and
Cognition, 22, 1136-1146.
Dexter, C. (1983) The Riddle of the Third Mile. London: Macmillan.
Education Policy Studies Laboratory of Arizona State University
(2005) 'Product Placement Spending in Media 2005: Executive
Summary', http://epsl.asu.edu/ceru/Articles/CERU-0505-127-OWI.pdf.
Friedman, M. (1985) 'The changing language of a consumer
society: Brand name usage in popular American novels in the post war
era', Journal of Consumer Research, 11, 927-938.
Friedman, M. (1986) 'Commercial influences in the lyrics of
popular American music of the postwar era', Journal of Consumer
Affairs, 20, 193-113.
Friedman, M. (1987) 'Word of author advertising and the
consumer; an empirical analysis of the quality of product brands noted
by authors of popular cultural works', Journal of Consumer Policy,
10, 307-318.
Friedman, M. (1991) A Brand New Language. New York: Greenwood
Press.
Gupta, P. B., & Lord K. R. (1998) 'Product placement in
movies: The effect of prominence and mode on audience recall',
Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 20, 47-59.
Gorn, G. J. (1982) 'The effects of music in advertising on
choice behavior', Journal of Marketing, 46, 94-101.
Hays, C. L. (1999, 21 March) 'Math textbook salted with brand
names raises new alarm. New York Times', B1.
Hoyer, W. D., & Brown, S. P. (1990) 'The effects of brand
awareness on choice for a common, repeat-purchase product', Journal
of Consumer Research, 17, 141-148.
Jeannet, J. P., & Hennessey, H. D. (1998) Global Marketing
Strategies, NewYork: Houghton Mifflin.
Johar, G. V., & Pham, M. T. (1999) 'Relatedness,
prominence, and constructive sponsor identification', Journal of
Marketing Research, 36, 299-312.
Karrh, J. A. (1998) 'Brand placement: A review', Journal
of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 20, 31-49.
Law, S.,& Braun, K. A. (2000) 'I'll have what
she's having: Gauging the impact of product placement on
viewers', Psychology and Marketing, 17, 1059-1075.
Lee, A. Y., & Sternthal, B. (1999) 'The effects of
positive mood on memory', Journal of Consumer Research, 26,
115-127.
Meenaghan, T. (1994) 'Ambush marketing: Immoral or imaginative
marketing practice?', Journal of Advertising Research, 34, 77-78
Menon, G. (1993) 'The effects of accessibility of information
in memory on judgements of behavioral frequencies', Journal of
Consumer Research, 20, 421-440.
Miniard, P. W., Sirdeshmukh, D., & Innis, D. E. (1992)
'Peripheral persuasion and brand choice', Journal of Consumer
Research, 19, 226-239.
Moorhead, G., & Griffin, R. W., (1998) Organizational Behavior
(5th ed.). New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Negunadi, P. (1990) 'Recall and consumer consideration sets:
Influencing choice without altering brand evaluations', Journal of
Consumer Research, 17, 263-276.
Rasmussen, E. (1997)'Courting the classroom', Sales and
Marketing Management, 149, 20.
Rindfleisch, A., & Inman, J. J. (1998) 'Explaining the
familiarity-liking relationship: mere-exposure, information availability
or social desirability?', Marketing Letters, 9, 5-19.
Schwarz, N. (1990) 'Assessing frequency reports of mundane
behavior: Contributions of cognitive psychology to questionnaire
construction', In C. Hendrick & M. S. Clarke (Eds.), Research
Methods in Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 98-119). Newbury Park,
CA: Sage.
Schwarz, N., Hippler, H. J., Deutsch, B., & Strack, F. (1985)
'Response categories: Effects on behavioral reports and comparative
judgements', Public Opinion Quarterly, 49, 388-395.
Snyder, M. (1992) 'Movies and product placement: Is Hollywood
turning films into commercial speech?', University of Illinois Law
Review, 301-337.
Stark, A. (2000, March 31) 'Taste: Let's make a
deal-Commerce invades the classroom; A terrible thing? Not always',
Wall Street Journal, W17.
Turban, E., Lee, J., King, D., & Chung, H. M. (2000) Electronic
Commerce: A Managerial Perspective. New Jersey, NJ: Prentice Hall
Unnava, H. R., & Burnkrant, R. E. (1991) 'Effects of
repeating varied ad executions on brand name memory', Journal of
Marketing Research, 28, 406-416.
Walsh, M. (2000) 'USA: Commercials in the classroom', The
Unesco Courier, 53, 14-16.
Watkins, M. J., LeCompte, D. C., and Kim, K. (2000) 'Role of
study strategy in recall of mixed lists of common and rare words',
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26,
239-245.
Wyatt, E. (2000a, April 7) 'School board considers deal to
swap ads for computers', New York Times, p. A1.
Wyatt, E. (2000b, September 26) 'School board to turn to
outside advisors', New York Times, p. B3.
Zajonc, R. B. (1968) 'Attitudinal effects of mere
exposure', Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monegraph,
(9, Pt 2).
Ian Brennan, Colorado State University-Pueblo
David McCalman, University of Central Arkansas
Ian Brennan is associate professor of marketing at Colorado State
University--Pueblo. His previous research has appeared in Advances in
Consumer Research, Decision Support Systems, Marketing Intelligence and
Planning, The International Journal of Advertising, Psychology and
Marketing, The Journal of Promotion Management
D. G. McCalman is assistant professor of management at the
University of Central Arkansas. His previous research has appeared in
Business Horizons, Journal of Technology Transfer, and Competitiveness
Review
TABLE MEANS FOR RECALL AND RECOGNITION OF WOA ADVERTISERS
Placement = 1
Familiar Firms Unfamiliar Firms
Unaided-Recall (a) 6.57 0.00
Recognition (b) 1.77 1.48
Choice (c) advertising present 1.35 1.25
Choice (c) advertising present 1.2 1.2
Placement = 2
Familiar Firms Unfamiliar Firms
Unaided-Recall (a) 13.41 1.22
Recognition (b) 1.94 1.68
Choice (c) advertising present 1.15 1.3
Choice (c) advertising present 1.15 1.3
(a) Percent of firms correctly recalled within each condition
(experiment 1).
(b) Number of brands recalled within each condition (range 0-2)
(experiment 2).
(c) Number of target brands chosen (range 0-2) when WOA advertising is
present (experiment 3).
(d) Number of target brands chosen (range 0-2) when WOA advertising is
absent (experiment 3).