Eating disorders and advertising effects: an exploration.
Peterson, Robin ; Jun, Minjoon
ABSTRACT
The purpose of this manuscript is to describe a study designed to
furnish insights into various relationships between advertising and
anorexia nervosa and bulemia among university students. The inquiry
measured self image and ideal self image and correlated these
assessments to the occurrence of the eating disturbances and to measures
of advertising and merchandising. The results led to a number of
recommendations for marketers who choose to assist in preventing and
abating eating disorders.
INTRODUCTION
Marketing and consumer researchers have devoted considerable effort
in search of serviceable answers for pressing societal difficulties
(Porter & Kramer, 2002; Christie, Fisher, Kozup, & Smith, 2001;
Wagner & Hansen, 2002; Murphy, 1998). A significant proportion of
this endeavor has applied conventional research methodologies to
macro-level questions (Rentschler & Wood, 2001; Karande, Rao, &
Singhapakdi, 2002). Another field of concentration for researchers has
been micro-issues facing organizations with societal goals. Hibbert,
Piacentini and Al Dajani (2003), for instance have examined volunteer
motivation for participation in community-based food cooperatives.
Carrigan and Attalla (2001) and Sen and Bhattacharya (2001) have focused
on whether or not customers care about ethical behavior and
socially-responsible actions to the degree that they significantly
affect their purchasing behavior. In a somewhat similar study, Miles and
Covin (2000) considered the relationship between environmental marketing
and reputational, competitive, and financial advantages accruing to the
firm.
A considerable number of research endeavors have concentrated on
the effects of advertising and other promotion vehicles on phenomena
that are related to social responsibility (Bloch & Richins,1992).
Kreth (2000), for instance, has examined the degree to which marketers
exercise social responsibility in making advertised health claims for
dietary supplements (Pechmann, Zhao, Goldberg, & Reibling, 2003)
have conducted research to assist in identifying effective message
themes to be used in antismoking advertisements for adolescents.
Peterson (2002) considered the extent and degree of favorableness of the
depiction of African American Childrens' activities in television
commercials. Odekerken-Schroder, De Wulf, and Hofstee (2002) have
examined the extent to which gender stereotyping in advertising is
related to the incidence of masculinity in the culture of various
countries.
A social responsibility issue which marketing and consumer behavior
researchers have largely disregarded is the widespread existence of
eating disorders among young people, especially females. Research
indicates that this is a major social problem (Hoskins, 2002; Johnson
& Larson, 1982). Large numbers of individuals have fallen prey to
anorexia nervosa and bulemia. Research estimates vary, but generally
they suggest that nearly twenty percent of the individuals in the 17-20
age grouping are victims of the two eating disorders, while other age
groupings possess smaller percentages (Steiger, Bruce, & Israel,
2003; Jacobson, 1985). Increasingly, larger numbers of college, high
school, middle school, and even elementary school students are becoming
victims (Schur, Sanders, & Steiner, 2000). The largest proportion,
however, falls in the college-age female category (Schwitzer, Bergholz,
& Terri, 1998). The effects on the emotional, physical, and mental
health of these individuals and their families have been considerable
(DiConsiglio, 2000).
Research has been undertaken in recent periods on the underlying
conditions, causes, main victims, and potential cures for these
disorders. Social pressure to be thin has emerged as a potential causal
factor (Stice, Maxfield, & Wells, 2003). Other studies have
uncovered social dependency, need for approval, and fear of rejection as
influences (Hayaki, Friedman, Whisman, Delinsky, & Brownell, 2003).
Significant correlations between volume of exercise, vegetarianism, low
esteem, and stress, on the one hand, and the two eating disorders, on
the other, have been found (McLean & Barr, 2003). In addition,
studies have revealed an association between perfectionism motives and
anorexia/bulemia (Sutandar-Pinnock, Woodside, Carter, Olmsted, &
Kaplan, 2003). There is some evidence that body image distress is a
factor (Devlin & Zhu, 2001). Eating disturbances are as common among
Hispanic and African Americans as they are with Caucasians (Walcott,
Pratt, & Patel, 2003). Further, numerous female collegiate athletes
are subject to these disturbances (Beals, & Manore, 2002).
Exposure to outside influence appears to be a possible contributor
to the two afflictions. The amount of television viewing is such a
factor(Kennedy, 2000). Another is readership of health, sports, and
fitness magazines, where young people are able to make social
comparisons and develop what they feel are critical body image
perceptions (Botta, 2003). Further, there is a demonstrated and
significant relationship is between the two disturbances and exposure to
advertising (Zucker, Harrell, Miner, Kathi, & Abigail, 2002).
Overall, the studies suggest that the two eating disorders are most
common among the white middle and upper classes. Numerous victims are
overachievers who excel in academics, sports, and other activities. Many
are overactive and are heavily involved in recreational, work, and
social activities. In turn, a large number have obsessive and low esteem
personality traits, are subject to considerable stress, have negative
images of their bodies, and are perfectionist in their appearance and
performance. Many anorexics and bulemics are well-groomed, attractive,
and independent, and have achieved professional success. They tend to be
avid readers of health and fitness magazines and are heavily exposed to
the media, especially television.
The bulk of the studies relating to eating disorders have emanated
from the medical and social science literature (Botta, 2003).
Researchers in consumer behavior and marketing have not addressed this
problem in depth. However, it would appear that there is need for
studies emanating from these disciplines. It would be useful, for
instance, to examine the degree of influence on the formation and
maintenance of eating disorders that is brought about through
advertising. specifically, could use of emaciated models in advertising
create desires for body images and in turn lead to bulemia and anorexia?
Do advertisers portray ideal self physical images that are not realistic
for most young women and children? Answers to questions such as these
could be of value to those who seek prevention and treatment of the
eating disturbances. The study set forth here was undertaken to furnish
insights into these issues.
RESEARCH OBJECTIVES
The broad aim of the research was to furnish insights for
advertisers, advertising agencies, governmental authorities, and other
parties who have a vested interest in constraining potential advertising
influences on eating disorders. Three specific objectives flow from this
aim. These are:
1. To appraise the self images and ideal self images of college
juniors and seniors who are experiencing anorexia nervosa and bulemia
and to compare these with those who are not experiencing these
disorders.
Self image is a meaningful variable for study since the
"heavy" or "thin" self image held by individuals is
closely related to the probability that they will contract an eating
disorder (Botta, 2003).
2. To compare gauges of promotion exposure, shopping exposure,
weight consciousness, and the effect of advertising on the desire to be
thin between the two groups.
This objective could be useful in producing insights on the effect
of advertising and merchandising efforts on the development of eating
disorders.
3. Employing the self image and ideal self image measures, to
suggest guidelines to advertisers and other parties who are motivated to
reduce possible advertising effects on the incidence and maintenance of
eating disorders.
NULL HYPOTHESES TESTED
The present research was conducted in a manner which permitted
testing of the following null hypotheses:
H1: There is not a significant difference in (a) magazine advertising
exposure, (b) television advertising exposure, and (c) clothing
shopping exposure between (a) college students who are victims of
anorexia and bulemia and (b) students who are not victims.
Research suggests that victims of the eating disorders tend to be
heavy magazine readers, especially for health and fitness magazines
(Thomsen, Weber, & Brown, 2001; Botta, 2003). Further, there is
evidence that victims are heavy viewers of television and television
advertisements (Kennedy, 2000). In addition, studies indicate that
victims spend considerable time shopping for clothing (Leon, Carroll,
Chernyk, & Finn, 1984). Thus, there is theoretical support for the
alternative hypotheses.
H2: There is not a significant difference in the reinforcement by
advertisements of the desire to be thin between the two groups.
Some studies have found that the popular media place substantial
emphasis on thinness as an ideal of feminine beauty (Owen, &
Laurel-Seller, 2000). Further, high risk subjects-those who are more
likely to develop eating disorders-are more vulnerable to advertisements
which glorify thinness than are those who are less likely to become
victims (Zucker, Harrell, Miner, Kathi, & Abagail, 2001; Toro,
Salamero, & Martinez, 1994).
H3: There is not a significant difference in concern with personal
appearance between the two groups.
Past research has suggested that individuals with eating disorders
are more likely than those without these disorders to be highly
concerned with their personal appearance (McLean & Barr, 2003). They
are more likely, for instance, to become depressed when they are exposed
to videos stressing ideal personal appearance than are those without
eating disorders (Heinberg & Thompson, 1995). These findings suggest
a relationship between anorexia and bulemia, on the one hand, and
concern with personal appearance, on the other.
H4: There is not a significant difference between the self images of
the two groups.
Self images exert an important impact on behavior. In this regard,
various studies suggest that members of the two groups possess divergent self images. Those with the eating disorders, for instance, tend to
possess a higher degree of body image distress (Devlin & Zhu, 2001).
Further, they tend to experience lower self esteem (McLean & Barr,
2003). In general, preadolescent girls show lower body liking and higher
body dissatisfaction than boys (Roncolato, Huon & Braganza, 1998).
These differences could account for some of the incidence of anorexia
and bulemia.
H5: There is not a significant difference between the ideal self
images of the two groups.
Some research suggests that those with eating disorders possess
ideal self images that are based more on degree of thinness than is the
case for those without these disturbances. (Thomsen, Weber, & Brown,
2001; Neumark-Sztainer, 1996). In fact, those with the disorders often
have conceptions of body image that is nothing short of perfectionism
(Sutander-Pinnock, Woodside, Carter, Olmsted, & Kaplan, 2003).
Hence, it is possible that ideal self image may be related to anorexia
and bulemia.
H6: The gaps between the self image and the ideal self images are not
greater for those with eating disorders than those without these
disorders.
If this hypothesis is rejected, there is reason for advertisers to
reject themes, appeals, and other copy that might create gaps between
the two types of self image. When consumers perceive these voids, there
is a strong probability that they will experience anxiety and attitude
change toward body image. Studies have revealed that the gaps between
the two groups may be significant (Posavac & Posavac, 2002; Myers,
& Biocca, 1992).
The hypotheses set forth above relate to the aforementioned objectives of the study. The subsequent section describes the manner in
which the hypotheses were assessed.
THE RESEARCH STUDY
Cover letters and questionnaires were forwarded to a random sample
of 50 marketing professors who were listed in the American Marketing
Association membership roster. Each professor received thirty
questionnaires and a request to ask their students to complete these and
return them to the instructor. A total of 39 professors participated in
the study, generating a sample size of 1,073 complete and otherwise
usable questionnaires.(Some instructors did not obtain questionnaires
from all thirty students. Others requested additional questionnaires so
that an entire class could participate in the study). After they had
secured the completed questionnaires, the instructors mailed them to the
researcher. The sample was made up of 67.5% business majors and 32.5%
non-business majors. As to class ranking, 33.8% were juniors, 49.1%
seniors, 11.6% sophomores, and 5.5% freshmen.
The questionnaires requested that students specify (a) if they were
experiencing bulemia, (b) if yes, how many purges did they carry out in
an average week, (c) if they were experiencing anorexia nervosa, (d) if
yes, an estimate of how many calories they consumed on an average day.
Definitions of bulemia and anorexia nervosa were provided in the
questionnaire.
The students were informed that they should not reveal their
identities and that the questionnaires would be held in strict
confidence, in order to minimize possible bias on the requested
self-reports of the incidence of these disorders. The students were also
requested to respond to fifty bipolar adjective scale to assess their
self images and again to the same scales to assess their ideal self
images. The adjective pairs were chosen and validated in a pretest study
using the procedures developed by Brinton (1961) employing an initial
list of 220 adjective pairs selected from Osgood, Suci, and Tannenbaum
(1957). The same adjective pairs were used in a previous study of eating
disorders (Peterson, 1987).
Subsequent portions of the questionnaire asked the students various
questions relating to their exposure to elements of the marketing mix
and their attitudes and related variables. The respondents were asked
(a) how many hours per week they watched television, (b) how many
magazines they read per week, as measures of advertising exposure, (c)
how many times per week they shopped for clothing, as a measure of
shopping exposure to items related to personal appearance, (d) if they
believed that the advertisements they perceived created an image of
desire to be thin, to the extent that they attempted to control their
weight. This was a measure of the impact of advertisements on attitudes
and behavior. and (e) the degree to which they were concerned with their
personal appearance.
THE RESEARCH RESULTS
Table One presents the percentages of response to the questions
asking if the students were experiencing anorexia nervosa or bulemia. It
is apparent that 5.9% had bulemia, 3.6% had anorexia, and 2.1% had both.
The proportions for eating disorders are approximately six percent
larger than those produced by a similar study of university students in
a 1987 study (Peterson, 1987), signaling a possible increase in the
incidence of the disturbances since the earlier time period.
Of those students citing bulemia as a condition, the mean number of
purges per week was 5.1. This is slightly higher than the results of
earlier research into this eating disorder (Chiodo & Latimer, 1983;
Peterson, 1987). In turn, students in the sample with anorexia reported
a mean number of 917 calories consumed per average day. This figure is
slightly lower than that set forth in earlier studies (Duddle, 1973).
While exact caloric needs vary, depending upon factors such as weight,
age, and level of physical activity, 917 is well-below the amount
required to maintain physical health.
The questionnaire requested the sample members to indicate how many
magazines they read in an average week. The mean for the eating disorder
group was 1.7 and for the non-disorder group 1.5. The differences were
not significant at the .05 level, according to a t test. Hence, null
hypothesis 1A is supported by the findings.
The subjects were asked how many hours per week they watched
television, on the average. Those with eating disorders produced a mean
of 9.7, while those without an eating disorder generated a mean of 9.3.
The differences between the two means are not significant, according to
a t test at the .05 level. It appears that exposure to this advertising
medium is not related to the incidence of the two disorders. The results
provide support for null hypothesis 1B. The mean figures for hypotheses
1A and 1B do not support the concept that exposure to advertising may
contribute to eating disorders.
The respondents were requested to specify how frequently they
shopped for clothing during an average week. This was designed to
measure exposure to merchandising efforts The mean for the disorder
group was .6 and for the non-disorder group .4. The difference is not
significant at the .05 level, according to a t test, which supports null
hypothesis 1C
The subjects were asked "Do you believe that the ads you see
and hear create strong images of the desirability of being thin, thereby
inducing you to keep weight down?" Ninety percent of those with
eating disorders answered "yes" to this question, while only
sixty one percent of those without eating disorders responded in the
affirmative. The difference is statistically significant at the .05
level, according to a t test. These results do not support null
hypothesis 2.
It appears that those with eating disorders are not more exposed to
television and magazine advertisements than those without the disorders,
but that they respond in a more robust manner. These findings suggest
that cutting back on the volumes of advertising would not necessarily be
effective in combating bulemia and anorexia, but making the ads less
suggestive of the desirability of being thin might.
The questionnaire asked the subjects how concerned they were about
their personal appearance. Table Two sets forth the results. A Chi
Square test for homogeneity suggests that the two samples were drawn
from different populations, with a Chi Square value of 12.97, which is
significant at the .05 level. Further, the weighted mean of those with
eating disorders (weighing "Very concerned" as 3,
"concerned" as 2, and "neutral" as 1) produces a
mean of 2.7 for those with eating disorders and 2.1 for those without
eating disorders. These means are significantly different at the .05
level, according to a t test. The results lead to a rejection of null
hypothesis three. This suggests that anxiety regarding personal
appearance, raised through advertisements, could be much more persuasive
among the former than among the latter group.
Table Three presents the mean self image scores of the students
with and without eating disorders on bipolar adjectives scales, where
the first scale has a weight of one (very important) and the last a
weight of seven (not very important). Those adjective pairs which showed
significant differences between the two groups, according to a t test at
the .05 level are labeled with an asterisk. Based upon the differences
it is possible to construct a profile of how eating disorder victims in
the sample differed from those without disorders on the various self
image components. Generally, those with eating disorders perceive
themselves as more intelligent, mature, grateful, compulsive, nervous,
persevering, painstaking, easily jealous, neurotic, hurried, secretive,
dissatisfied, and successful than those without the disorders. As there
is a large number of significant differences, null hypothesis four
cannot be accepted.
Table Four presents the ideal self image means for the two groups.
Those with eating disorders pose their ideal self images as more
assertive, intelligent, independent, friendly, persevering, thoughtful,
hurried, quick, sophisticated, valuable, important, and successful than
do those without an eating disorder. On the other hand, those without
eating disorders were more likely to hold an ideal self image which
emphasized modesty, maturity, blame others, soft hearted,
adventuresomeness, not-neurotic, content, patient, and good. Since there
are a large number of significant differences, null hypothesis five
cannot be supported.
The results set forth above give the impression that there may be
substantial differences between the components of the self image and
ideal self image between the two groups. This suggests that they might
respond in a different manner to advertising appeals, themes, formats,
illustrations, and featured products. The differences in the self images
and ideal self images also furnish opportunities for targeting consumers
who do not have eating disturbances and avoiding those who do, in
advertising messages. If advertisers focus their promotions on those
whose self images conform to the no-disorder configuration presented in
the table and avoid the disorder configuration for products and services
which suggest the desirability of slimness, the probability of
encouraging eating disorders may be mitigated.
The analysis included a comparison of the respondents' self
image and ideal self image scores, in order to identify gaps between
these two variables-identifying self image dimensions which were subject
to variety or uncertainty. Consumers can be expected to respond
rigorously to advertising appeals which relate to components of the self
image that differ substantially from the ideal self image (Devlin &
Zhu, 2001). Anxiety often arises when the self image and ideal self
image vary to a large degree. One whose self image is
"impatient", for instance, but whose ideal self image is
"patient" may respond strongly to a promotion portraying very
thin members of the same gender who are depicted as being patient. Thus,
responsible marketers might choose to avoid undue emphasis on the
nervousness dimension of those depicted in their promotions and instead
concentrate on other components that are less likely to arouse anxiety.
Table Five presents self image and ideal self image mean scores for
those with and those without eating disturbances. It also sets forth the
difference between each self image and ideal self image mean score for
each bipolar adjective scale. Those differences (or "D"
scores) measure the gap between actual and ideal self images.
Responsible advertisers are well advised to avoid themes and appeals for
variables with high D scores and employing those with low D scores, in
order to combat advertising-induced anxiety about slimness and weight
control.
Various pairs of adjectives generated mean D scores for the eating
disorder group that were significantly larger than the corresponding
values for the non-eating disorder category, according to t tests at the
.05 level. These were impulsive-deliberate, blame mistakes others-blame
mistakes me, inflexible-adaptable to change, hardhearted-softhearted,
easily jealous-not easily jealous, emotional- unemotional,
impatient-patient, alert-absent minded, quick-slow,
sophisticated-simple, bad-good, usual-unusual, and
important-unimportant. On the other hand, the non-eating disorder group
produced significantly larger D scores for boastful-modest,
conceited-not conceited, painstaking-slipshod, and wasteful-thrifty.
Overall, the eating disorder group had thirteen significantly larger
values, while the eating disorder group generated four. These results do
not support the sixth null hypothesis. The eating disorder group shows
considerably more significant differences between self image dimensions
and ideal self image dimensions. In accordance with the discussion
presented earlier, when advertisers stress appeals in their
advertisements that are related to adjective pairs with high D scores,
there is a strong possibility that individuals may react in a manner
that promotes eating disorders.
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this paper was to set forth the outcomes of an
inquiry into eating disorders among university students. The study
indicated that advertisements that carry positive images of slimness can
exert a strong influence on students. Those who are victims of bulemia
and anorexia possess different self image and ideal self images than
those who are not victims. The study revealed various differences
between self images and ideal self images which may be related to the
incidence of the eating disorders, providing signals to advertisers who
desire to mitigate negative influences on these eating disturbances.
This study does not, of course, prove that advertising is the sole,
or even one of the more important contributors to eating disorders.
Numerous other influents, such as primary and secondary reference
groups, celebrities, and non-advertising messages carried in the mass
media could exert a significant impact (Anonymous, 2000). Nevertheless,
there is evidence that some advertising efforts have a role and should
be scrutinized for possible negative effects.
There are some limitations to the study. The findings are largely
based upon self reports where the subjects indicated such things as
whether or not they suffer from an eating disorder and the effect of
advertising on their behavior. Self reports can be biased, although
assurances of anonymity in the data collection process were used to
minimize possible bias. The sample of those with anorexia and bulemia
was not large. However, the sample was sufficient to provide
statistically significant results. Further, the subjects were students
enrolled in marketing classes at various universities, and the results
may not be generalizable to other populations. However, college students
are among the more likely groups to become victims of the disorders, and
the marketing classes attracted students from various majors within and
out of the colleges of business administration, furnishing a relatively
heterogeneous sample composition.
IMPLICATIONS
The study described in this paper poses implications for socially
responsible advertisers. They are well-advised to avoid advertising
themes, messages, and appeals which the study revealed as potentially
dangerous for consumer welfare. Individuals with eating disorders appear
to have anxieties which probably should not be aroused from a social
responsibility perspective.
Further, there are implications for researchers. Further
examination of the relationship of advertising and other promotion
methods to eating disorders is merited, as a means of furnishing
insights which could be useful to advertisers. In addition, the results
of the study could be replicated, employing different respondents, as at
the high school and younger levels, where anorexia and bulemia are
relatively commonplace. Further, investigations could be carried out
into the receptiveness of advertisers to modify their campaigns and
individual advertisements as a means of helping in abating bulemia and
anorexia.
REFERENCES
Anonymous (2000).Ads are not to blame for all the world's
problems. Campaign, 28(3), 27.
Beals, K. A. & M. M. Manore (2002). Disorders of the female
athlete triad among collegiate athletes. International Journal of Sport
Nutrition and Exercise Metabolism, 12(3), 281-293.
Bloch, P. H. & M. L. Richins (1992). You look marvelous: the
pursuit of beauty and the marketing concept. Psychology & Marketing,
9(1), 3-19.
Botta, R. A. (2003). For our health? The relationship between
magazine reading and adolescents body image and eating disturbances. Sex
Roles, 48(10), 389-399.
Brinton, J. H. (1961). Deriving an attitude scale from semantic
differential data. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25(3), 289-295.
Carrigan, M. & A. Attalla (2001). The myth of the ethical
consumer: do ethics matter in purchase behavior? The Journal of Consumer
Marketing, 18(7), 560-577.
Chiodo, J. & P.R. Latimer (1983). Vomiting as a learned weight
control technique. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Psychiatry, 14(1), 131-136.
Christie, J., D. Fisher, J. C. Kozup, & S. Smith (2001). The
effects of bar-sponsored alcohol beverage promotions across binge and
nonbinge drinkers. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 20(2),
240-253.
Devlin, M. J. & A. J. Zhu (2001). Body image in the balance.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 286(17), 2159-2187.
DiConsiglio, J. (2000). Dying to be thin. Scholastic Choices,
16(3), 11.
Duddle, M. (1973). An increase of anorexia nervosa in a university
population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 4(1), 711-712.
Hayaki, J., M.A. Friedman, M.A. Whisman, S.S. Delinsky, & K.D.
Brownell (2003). Sociotropy and bulemic symptoms in clinical and
nonclinical samples. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34(1),
172-176.
Heinberg, L. J. & , J. K. Thompson (1995). Body image and
televised images of thinness and attractiveness: a controlled laboratory
investigation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 14(4),
325-338.
Hibbert, S., M. Piacentini, & H. Al Dajani (2003).
Understanding volunteer motivation for participation in a
community-based food cooperative. International Journal of Nonprofit and
Voluntary Sector Marketing, 8(1), 30-42.
Hoskins, M.L. (2002). Girls' identity dilemmas: spaces defined
by definitions of worth. Health Care for Women International, 23(3),
231-247.
Jacobson, B. (1985). Anorexia nervosa and bulemia: two severe
eating disorders. New York: Public Affairs Committee, 1-2, 9.
Johnson, C. & R. Larson (1982). Bulemia: An analysis of moods
and behavior. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44(1), 1034-1038.
Karande, K., C.P. Rao, & A. Singhapakdi (2002). Moral
philosophies of marketing managers a comparison of american, australian,
and malaysian cultures. European Journal of Marketing, 36(8), 768-791.
Kennedy, C. (2000). Examining television as an influence on
childrens' health behaviors. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 15(5),
272-281.
Kreth, M. L. (2000). Exploring responsibility in advertising:
health claims about dietary supplements. Business Communication
Quarterly, 63(6), 66-72.
Leon, G.R., K. Carroll, B. Chernyk, & S. Finn (1984). Binge
eating and associated habit patterns with college student and clinically
identified populations. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
11(2), 78-93.
McLean J.A. & S.I. Barr (2003). Cognitive dietary restraint is
associated with eating behaviors, lifestyle practices, personality
characteristics, and menstrual irregularity in college women. Appetite,
40(2), 185-192.
Miles, M. P. & J. G. Covin (2000). Environmental marketing: A
source of reputational, competitive, and financial advantage. Journal of
Business Ethics, 23(3), 299-311.
Murphy, P. E. (1998). Ethics in advertising review, analysis, and
suggestions. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 17(2), 316-319.
Myers, P. N. & F. A. Biocca (1992). The elastic body image: the
effect of television advertising and programming on body image
distortions in young women," Journal of Communication , 42(3),
108-133.
Neumark-Sztainer, D. (1996). School-based programs for preventing
eating disturbances, Journal of School Health , 66(2), 29-45.
Odekerken-Schroder, G., K. De Wulf, & N. Hofstee (2002). Is
gender stereotyping in advertising more prevalent in masculine
countries? A cross-cultural analysis. International Marketing Review,
19(5), 408-419.
Osgood, C. E., G. Suci, & P. H. Tannenbaum (1957). The
measurement of meaning. Urbana, Ill: University of Illinois Press, 82,
84.
Owen, P. R.& E. Laurel-Seller (2000). Weight and shape ideals:
thin is dangerously in. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(5),
979-990.
Pechmann, C., G. Zhao, M. Goldbert, & E. Thomas Reibling
(2003). What to convey in antismoking advertisements for adolescents:
the use of protection motivation theory to identify effective message
themes. Journal of Marketing, 67(2), 1-18.
Peterson, R. T. (1987).Anorexia and bulemia in an advertising
context Journal of Business Ethics, 6(2), 495-504.
Peterson, R. T. (2002). The depiction of african american
childrens' activities in television commercials: an assessment.
Journal of Business Ethics, 36(4), 303-313.
Porter, M. E. & M. R. Kramer (2002). The competitive advantage
of corporate philanthropy," Harvard Business Review, 80(12), 56-68.
Posavac, S. S. & H. Posavac (2002). Predictors of women's
concern with body weight: The roles of perceived self media ideal
discrepancies and self esteem. Eating Disorders: The Journal of
Treatment and Prevention, 10(2), 153-160.
Rentschler, R. & G. Wood (2001). Cause related marketing: can
the arts afford not to participate? Services Marketing Quarterly, 22(1),
57-69.
Roncolato, W., G. Huon, & C. Braganza (1998). Nutritional
knowledge, food-related and body-related attitudes among preadolescent
children. Australian Journal of Nutrition and Dietetics, 55(4), 195-202.
Schur, E.A., M. Sanders & H. Steiner (2000). Body
dissatisfaction and eating in young children. International Journal of
Eating Disorders, 27(2), 74-82.
Scwitzer, A. M. Bergholz, K. Dore, & L. Salimi Terri (1998).
Eating disorders among college women-pevention, education, and treatment
response. Journal of American College Health, 46(5), 202-207.
Sen, S. & C.B. Bhattacharya (2001). Does doing good always lead
to doing better? Consumer reactions to corporate social responsibility,.
Journal of Marketing Research, 38(2), 225-243.
Steiger, H., K. R. Bruce, & M. Israel (2003). Eating disorders
in stricker. In T. George, A. Widiger, & I. B. Weiner (Eds).
Handbook of Psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Stice, E., J. Maxfield, & T. Wells (2003). Adverse effects of
social pressure to be thin on young women: an experimental investigation
of the effects of fat talk. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
34(1), 108-117.
Sutandar-Pinnock, K., D. B> Woodside, J. C. Carter, M. P.
Olmsted, & A. S. Kaplan (2003). Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa: A
6-24- month follow up study International Journal of Eating Disorders,
33(2), 225-229.
Thomsen, S.R., M.M. Weber & L.B. Brown (2001). The relationship
between health and fitness magazine reading and eating-disoriented
weight-loss methods among high school girls. American Journal of Health
Education, 32(3), 133-138.
Toro, J., M. Salamero, & E.Martinez (1994). Assessment of
sociocultural influences on the aesthetic body shape model in anorexia
nervosa. Acta Psychiatrica Scandanavica, 89(3), 147-151.
Wagner, E.R. & E.N. Hansen (2002). Methodology for evaluating
green advertising of forest products in the united states: A content
analysis. Forest Products Journal, 52(4), 17-23.
Walcott, D. D. H. D. Pratt, & D. R. Patel (2003). Adolescents
and eating disorders: gender, racial, ethic, sociocultural, and
socioeconomic issues, Journal of Adolescent Research, 18(3), 223-243.
Zucker, A., N. Z. Harrel, R. Miner, K. Stewart, & J. Abigail
(2000). Smoking in college women: The role of thinness pressures: media
exposure, and critical consciousness. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
25(3), 233-241.
Robin Peterson, New Mexico State University Minjoon Jun, New Mexico
State University
Table 1: Sample Respondents Experiencing Anorexia or Bulemia
Student Condition Number Percent of Total
Have bulemia only 63 5.9%
Have anorexia only 39 3.6
Have both bulemia and anorexia 22 2.1
Do not have either 949 88.4
Total 1,073 100.0%
Table Two: Personal Appearance Concern by the Subjects
Student Condition Very Concerned Concerned Neutral
Eating disorders 71.4% 26.2% 2.4%
Without eating disorders 43.8% 23.7% 32.5%
Table Three: Self Image Scale Means for Students With and Without
Eating Disorders
Students with Students without
an Eating an Eating
Adjective Pair disorder disorder
Boastful-modest 5.1 4.9
Assertive-submissive 4.3 4.2
Conceited-not conceited 5.0 5.2
Intelligent-stupid * 2.7 3.2
Clear thinking-confused 2.3 2.3
Independent-dependent * 2.1 2.7
Reliable-undependable 1.7 1.5
Mature-immature * 2.2 2.8
Impulsive-deliberate * 1.9 2.6
Hypochondriacal-not 5.8 5.5
hypochondriacal
Nervous * -calm 3.7 4.3
Thankless-grateful * 6.1 5.6
Hostile-friendly 5.9 5.8
Blame mistakes others-blame 4.5 4.7
mistakes me
Inflexible-adaptable to change 5.8 5.6
Hardhearted-softhearted 5.4 5.5
Unrealistic-practical 5.8 5.5
Quitting-persevering * 5.4 4.8
Painstaking*-slipshod 2.3 3.0
Thoughtful-not thoughtful 2.5 2.1
Wide interests-narrow interests 2.2 2.1
Easily jealous-not easily jealous * 2.7 3.4
Self pitying-not self pitying 4.6 4.6
Timid-adventurous 5.7 5.9
Optimistic-pessimistic 2.8 2.5
Enthusiastic-apathetic 3.1 3.0
Neurotic-not neurotic * 3.3 3.9
Fickle-loyal 5.8 5.6
Dishonest-honest 6.0 6.0
Hurried-not hurried * 2.7 3.4
Secretive-frank * 4.1 4.8
Emotional-unemotional 2.8 2.7
Dissatisfied-content * 3.0 3.6
Energetic-not energetic 3.4 3.5
Self confident-self distrustful 4.2 3.9
Suspicious-trusting 6.3 6.1
Impatient-patient 4.7 4.5
Alert-absent minded 2.5 2.5
Quick-slow 1.3 1.4
Sociable-shy 2.5 2.5
Wasteful-thrifty 5.9 6.2
Sophisticated-simple * 3.0 3.5
Graceful-awkward 1.9 1.7
Bad-good 6.8 6.9
Usual-unusual 4.1 3.8
Worthless-valuable 6.7 6.7
Important-unimportant 6.0 6.0
Unsuccessful-successful * 6.1 5.3
Sick-healthy 5.7 5.6
Weak-strong 4.7 4.6
* Statistically significant difference between items in the two columns
according to a t test at the .05 level of significance
Table Four: Ideal Self Image Scale Means for Students With and Without
Eating Disorders
Students with Students without
an Eating an Eating
Adjective Pair disorder disorder
Boastful-modest * 5.0 5.6
Assertive-submissive * 4.1 4.6
Conceited-not conceited 4.9 4.7
Intelligent-stupid * 2.2 2.7
Clear thinking-confused 1.7 1.7
Independent-dependent * 2.5 3.2
Reliable-undependable 2.3 2.3
Mature-immature * 2.6 2.1
Impulsive-deliberate 3.6 3.5
Hypochondriacal-not 5.6 5.5
hypochondriacal
Nervous-calm * 3.7 3.1
Thankless-grateful * 5.5 5.0
Hostile-friendly * 6.2 5.5
Blame mistakes others-blame
mistakes me * 5.6 5.1
Inflexible-adaptable to change 4.8 4.7
Hardhearted-softhearted * 2.9 3.5
Unrealistic-practical 5.5 5.6
Quitting-persevering * 5.8 5.1
Painstaking -slipshod 2.2 2.4
Thoughtful-not thoughtful * 2.1 2.7
Wide interests-narrow interests 3.6 3.6
Easily jealous-not easily jealous 3.4 3.5
Self pitying-not self pitying 4.7 4.5
Timid-adventurous * 5.9 5.2
Optimistic-pessimistic 3.5 3.6
Enthusiastic-apathetic 3.0 3.2
Neurotic-not neurotic * 3.0 3.9
Fickle-loyal 5.7 5.7
Dishonest-honest 5.5 5.6
Hurried-not hurried * 2.1 3.1
Secretive-frank 4.5 4.5
Emotional-unemotional 3.2 3.5
Dissatisfied-content* 3.5 4.1
Energetic-not energetic 4.3 4.1
Self confident-self distrustful 4.7 4.2
Suspicious-trusting 6.0 6.0
Impatient-patient * 3.8 4.5
Alert-absent minded * 1.9 2.7
Quick-slow * 1.5 2.0
Sociable-shy 2.3 2.6
Wasteful-thrifty 5.4 5.3
Sophisticated-simple * 2.4 3.4
Graceful-awkward 1.7 1.6
Bad-good * 5.4 6.2
Usual-unusual 3.3 3.5
Worthless-valuable * 6.5 6.0
Important-unimportant * 2.2 2.8
Unsuccessful-successful * 5.8 5.1
Sick-healthy 5.5 5.6
Weak-strong 4.9 5.1
* Statistically significant difference between items in the two columns
according to a t test at the .05 level of significance
Table Five: Self and Ideal Self Images and Gaps Between the Two
Adjective Pair Students with an eating disorder
Self image Ideal self D Score
Boastful-modest * 5.1 5.0 .1
Assertive-submissive 4.3 4.1 .2
Conceited- not conceited * 5.0 4.9 .1
Intelligent-stupid 2.7 2.2 .5
Clear thinking- confused 2.3 1.7 .6
Independent- dependent 2.1 2.5 .4
Reliable- undependable 1.7 2.3 .6
Mature-immature 2.2 2.6 .6
Impulsive- deliberate * 1.9 3.6 1.7
Hypochondriacal-not 5.8 5.6 .2
hypochondriacal
Nervous-calm 3.7 3.7 .0
Thankless-grateful 6.1 5.5 .6
Hostile-friendly * 5.9 6.2 .3
Blame mistakes others-blame 4.5 5.6 1.1
mistakes me *
Inflexible-adaptable to change * 5.8 4.8 1.0
Hardhearted- softhearted * 5.4 2.9 2.5
Unrealistic-practical 5.8 5.5 .3
Quitting-persevering 5.4 5.8 .3
Painstaking-slipshod * 2.3 2.2 .1
Thoughtful- not thoughtful 2.5 2.1 .4
Wide interests- narrow interests 2.2 3.6 1.4
Easily jealous-not 2.7 3.4 .7
easily jealous *
Self pitying- not self pitying 4.6 4.7 .1
Timid-adventurous 5.7 5.9 .2
Optimistic- pessimistic 2.8 3.5 .7
Enthusiastic-apathetic 3.1 3.0 .1
Neurotic-not neurotic 3.3 3.0 .3
Fickle-loyal 5.8 5.7 .1
Dishonest-honest 6.0 5.5 .5
Hurried-not hurried 2.7 2.1 .6
Secretive-frank 4.1 4.5 .4
Emotional- unemotional * 2.8 3.5 .7
Dissatisfied-content 3.0 3.5 .5
Energetic- not energetic 3.4 4.1 .7
Self confident- self distrustful 4.2 4.7 .5
Suspicious-trusting 6.3 6.0 .3
Impatient-patient * 4.7 3.8 .9
Alert-absent minded * 2.5 1.9 .6
Quick-slow * 2.5 1.5 1.0
Sociable-shy 2.5 2.3 .2
Wasteful-thrifty * 5.9 5.4 .5
Sophisticated- simple * 3.0 2.4 .6
Graceful-awkward 1.9 1.7 .2
Bad-good * 6.8 5.4 1.4
Usual-unusual * 4.1 3.3 .8
Worthless-valuable 6.7 6.5 .2
Important- unimportant * 6.0 2.2 3.8
Unsuccessful- successful 6.1 5.8 .3
Sick-healthy 5.7 5.5 .2
Weak-strong 4.7 4.9 .2
Students without an eating disorder
Self image Ideal self D Score
Boastful-modest * 4.9 5.6 .7
Assertive-submissive 4.2 4.6 .4
Conceited- not conceited * 5.2 4.7 .5
Intelligent-stupid 3.2 2.7 .5
Clear thinking- confused 2.3 1.7 .6
Independent- dependent 2.7 3.2 .5
Reliable- undependable 1.5 2.3 .8
Mature-immature 2.8 2.1 .7
Impulsive- deliberate * 2.6 3.5 .9
Hypochondriacal-not 5.5 5.5 .0
hypochondriacal
Nervous-calm 4.3 3.1 1.2
Thankless-grateful 5.6 5.0 .6
Hostile-friendly * 5.8 5.5 .2
Blame mistakes others-blame 4.7 5.1 .4
mistakes me *
Inflexible-adaptable to change * 5.6 4.7 .6
Hardhearted- softhearted * 5.5 3.5 2.0
Unrealistic-practical 5.5 5.6 .1
Quitting-persevering 4.8 5.1 .3
Painstaking-slipshod * 3.0 2.4 .6
Thoughtful- not thoughtful 2.1 2.7 .6
Wide interests- narrow interests 2.1 3.6 1.5
Easily jealous-not 3.4 3.5 .1
easily jealous *
Self pitying- not self pitying 4.6 4.5 .1
Timid-adventurous 5.9 5.2 .7
Optimistic- pessimistic 2.5 3.6 1.1
Enthusiastic-apathetic 3.0 3.2 .2
Neurotic-not neurotic 3.9 3.9 .0
Fickle-loyal 5.6 5.7 .1
Dishonest-honest 6.0 5.6 .4
Hurried-not hurried 3.4 3.1 .3
Secretive-frank 4.8 4.5 .3
Emotional- unemotional * 3.5 3.5 .0
Dissatisfied-content 3.6 4.1 .5
Energetic- not energetic 3.5 4.1 .6
Self confident- self distrustful 3.9 4.2 .3
Suspicious-trusting 6.1 6.0 .1
Impatient-patient * 4.5 4.5 .0
Alert-absent minded * 2.5 2.7 .2
Quick-slow * 1.4 2.0 .6
Sociable-shy 2.5 2.6 .1
Wasteful-thrifty * 6.2 5.3 .9
Sophisticated- simple * 3.5 3.4 .1
Graceful-awkward 1.7 1.6 .1
Bad-good * 6.9 6.2 .7
Usual-unusual * 3.8 3.5 .3
Worthless-valuable 6.7 6.0 .7
Important- unimportant * 6.0 2.8 3.2
Unsuccessful- successful 5.3 5.1 .2
Sick-healthy 5.6 5.6 .0
Weak-strong 4.6 5.1 .5
* Statistically significant difference between D scores according to
at test at the .05 level of significance