The academic ethics of undergraduate marketing majors.
Brown, Bob S. ; Abramson, Joseph
INTRODUCTION
College faculty and administrators have had concerns about the
academic ethics, or academic misconduct, of students for several
decades. A considerable amount of research has been conducted on the
topic, most of which has been surveys of students to determine the
extent of their participation in unethical practices, how they view the
ethical level of these practices, and how unethical academic behavior is
related to student characteristics. Students from many academic
disciplines, including business administration, have been surveyed.
However, relatively little has been done with respect to the individual
majors within the business area. This paper reports the results of a
survey of marketing majors.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Participation in unethical academic behavior by business students
has been reported at alarmingly high levels. Tom and Borin (1988) found
that 49% of undergraduate students taking a marketing course had engaged
in at least 1 of 23 dishonest behaviors. Sims (1993) found 91% of
undergraduate business majors reported dishonest behavior. Brown (1995)
reported 81% of graduate business students had engaged in at least 1 of
15 unethical behaviors more than infrequently while in graduate school.
Rates of participation of business students have also been found to
be high relative to other majors. Bowers (1966) found that 66% of
business majors had engaged in dishonest behavior, the highest rate
among nine majors. Other rates ranged from 58% for engineering majors to
37% for language majors. Baird (1980), though he did not report actual
rates, found business majors more likely to cheat than liberal arts and
education majors. Meade (1992) reported a study by McCabe at 31
top-ranked schools. Business majors showed a higher rate of dishonest
behavior (87%) than engineering (74%), science (67%), or humanities
majors (63%). Roig and Ballew (1994) found that business and economics
majors showed more tolerant attitudes toward dishonest behavior than did
social science students.
The relationship between unethical academic behavior and
characteristics of students in various majors has been investigated.
Several studies found males more likely to participate in unethical
activities than females (Baird, 1980; Davis & Ludvigson, 1995;
Genereux & McLeod, 1995; Karlins, Michaels, Freilinger, &
Walker, 1989; Sierles et al., 1980). However, other studies reported no
difference (Brown, 1995; Stern & Havlecek, 1986). McCabe &
Trevino (1996) found equal rates for males and females, but the rate
among females had increased from a decade earlier while the rate for
males had stayed about the same. A study by Graham, Monday,
O'Brien, and Steffen (1994) found rates of participation higher
among females. A more consistent finding has been that cheating behavior
varies inversely with GPA (Baird, 1980; Genereux & McLeod, 1995;
Graham et al. 1994; Haines & Diekhoff, 1986; Singhal, 1982).
Two additional points about unethical academic behavior are
apparent from the literature. First, students are more likely to engage
in practices they view as less unethical (Brown, 1995; Graham, et al.,
1994; Greene & Saxe, 1992; Newstrom & Ruch, 1976; Nuss, 1984;
Stevens, 1984; Tom & Borin, 1988). Secondly, students tend to see
themselves as more ethical than their peers (Greene & Saxe, 1992;
Newstrom & Ruch, 1976; Stevens, 1984).
The desire to obtain a high grade and lack of adequate study time
dominate the reasons cited for participating in unethical behavior
(Baird, 1980; Brown, 1995; Davis & Ludvigson, 1995; Graham, et al.,
1994; Meade, 1992; Nuss, 1984).
Studies of business students have not generally indicated the
functional area of business in which the students were majoring.
However, there is limited evidence that accounting majors have
relatively high ethical standards. Nowell and Laufer (1997) reported a
1990 unpublished study by Moffat that found that among business
students, economics majors were the most likely to cheat and accounting
majors were the least likely to do so. Rates of student participation in
unethical activities were not reported. Jeffery (1993) found that at
both the beginning and senior levels accounting majors had more
developed ethical reasoning capabilities than both non-accounting
business majors and liberal arts majors, but acknowledged that the
relationship between ethical development and behavior had not been
determined.
Students enrolled in marketing classes have been the subjects of
three studies. Tom and Borin (1988) surveyed students in seven marketing
classes at a large university. Forty-nine percent said they had
participated in at least one of 23 unethical behaviors. They rated as
less severe those practices that they had engaged in at a high rate.
Allen, Fuller, and Luckett (1998) surveyed students enrolled in mass
lecture sections of an undergraduate marketing course at a large
metropolitan university. Overall, thirty six percent of respondents reported cheating behavior. Management, marketing, and general business
majors showed higher levels than other business majors, but specific
rates were not reported. Nonis and Swift (1998) collected data from a
convenience sample of students majoring in various business majors that
were enrolled in marketing classes. Eighty four percent of respondents
were found to have cheated in their college classes.
We found no studies of the academic ethics of students who had
declared marketing as their major. This paper makes a contribution
toward filling that gap in the literature.
METHODOLOGY
We administered a questionnaire used by Brown (1994, 1995) in two
studies of graduate students, modified slightly to fit an undergraduate
population, to marketing majors at an eastern state university during
the Spring term of the 1998-99 academic year. The questionnaire
contained 16 academic practices that were selected from the literature
and that might be considered unethical. Respondents were asked to
indicate how often they had engaged in each activity while a university
student. A 6-point scale was utilized with six representing never, and a
range of one, frequently, to five, infrequently, for those who had
participated in the activity. Respondents were then asked to rate the
ethical level of each practice from one, very unethical, to five, not at
all unethical.
Eleven reasons why students might engage in unethical academic
behavior were selected from the literature. Respondents were asked to
rate on a 5-point scale from one, not at all likely, to five, very
likely, the chance that each would be a reason why students would
participate in unethical academic behavior. Demographics asked were
grade point average (GPA), hours worked on a job per week, semester hours of course work carried, gender, and year of birth.
Questionnaires were administered during class meetings. Respondents
were assured anonymity and were provided a plain envelope in which to
seal their completed questionnaires before returning them to the
instructor. Forty two completed questionnaires were returned.
RESULTS
Sample Characteristics
Characteristics of the sample are shown in Table 1. All of the
respondents were juniors (26.2%) and seniors (73.8%). Most (95.2%) were
under age 25, while the number of males and females was equal. Even
though almost 93% carried a course load of more than 12 semester hours,
57% were employed 20 hours or more per week. A little more than half had
GPAs less than 3.0.
Participation in the Practices
Results on the extent of participation in the 16 practices are
presented in Table 2 in the columns labeled "Participation."
For each practice, the percentage of respondents admitting participation
in the activity is shown, as well as the practice's rank from
highest (1) to lowest (16) level of participation. Each practice's
rating on the 5-point frequency of participation scale is also shown.
Four practices showed rates of participation in excess of 85%. They
were: working with others on an individual project (85.7%), giving
information about the content of an exam to someone who has not yet
taken it (90.5%), asking about the content of an exam from someone who
has taken it (92.9%), and having someone check over a paper before
turning it in (100%). All four of these practices involve collaboration
between or among students outside the classroom. Though collaborative
learning is often encouraged, it appears to be spilling over into
situations where it is not intended.
The three practices engaged in by the highest proportion of
students were also the three engaged in most frequently. They were the
only ones with means on the "frequently" side of the midpoint of the frequency of participation scale.
While asking for or giving information about the content of exams
at times other than when they were being taken were among the practices
engaged in by the highest proportion of students, four methods of
cheating on exams while they were being taken accounted for four of the
five least engaged in practices. They were: having information
programmed into a calculator during an exam (28.6%), passing answers
during an exam (35.7%), using exam crib notes (38.1%), and copying off
another's exam (42.9%). All four practices had frequency of
participation ratings on the "infrequently" side of the scale,
but the frequency of participation reported for the two practices that
are done alone was higher than for the two practices that involve other
students.
The practice that ranked 16th was "turning in work done by
someone else as one's own," with a substantial 26.2% of
students admitting engaging in this practice while a university student.
Levels of participation in the remaining seven practices ranged from
45.2% for "using a false excuse to delay an exam or paper" and
"taking credit for full participation in a group project without
doing a fair share of the work" to 78.6% for "padding a
bibliography."
The level of participation in the practices was related to two
student characteristics, GPA and sex. Characteristics were collapsed
into two categories, and participation was indicated as did or did not
participate. The chi-square test was used to determine significance. As
shown in Table 3, significant differences in the level of participation
were found between five practices and students' GPA and between
four practices and students' sex. In all five instances, students
with GPAs less than 3.0 showed higher levels of participation in the
practices than students with GPAs of 3.0 or higher. Differences ranged
from about 25% for padding a bibliography to 39% for taking credit for
full participation in a group project without doing a fair share of the
work. In all four instances, males showed higher levels of participation
in the practices than females. Differences ranged from about 29% for
looking at a copy of an exam before taking it to about 38% for using
exam crib notes.
Ethical Levels of the Practices
Ratings of the ethical level of the practices and their ranks from
least to most unethical are shown in the columns labeled "Ethical
Level" in Table 2. Students rated most of the practices as
unethical. Only five of the practices were rated higher than three, or
on the "not at all unethical" side of the midpoint of the
5-point scale. When the practices were ranked from highest to lowest in
terms of the proportion of students participating in them, and from
least to most in terms of being unethical, the first four practices had
identical ranks. This suggests that there is a tendency for
students' behavior to be consistent with their ethical beliefs.
However, the relationship is not perfect. While "visiting a
professor to influence a grade" was the 5th least unethical
practice, it was the 9th most engaged in practice. Perhaps the
face-to-face encounter with an authority figure was a deterrent to
participating in this practice.
The tendency for consistency between student behavior and ethics
was again apparent with the practices engaged in by the smallest
proportions of respondents. Four of the five practices engaged in by the
smallest proportion of students were among the five practices seen as
most unethical. The exception was "Allowing another to see exam
answers." This practice was rated more unethical than 12 other
practices, but was the seventh most engaged in practice.
Reasons for Unethical Behavior
The reasons cited for unethical academic behavior are shown in
Table 4. The unethical student was seen as one wanting high grades in
courses perceived as difficult, but unwilling to use available time to
study. It is worth noting that although almost 60% of respondents worked
20 or more hours per week, "does not have time to study" was
cited as only the sixth most likely reason for unethical behavior.
Respondents believed that unethical behavior was not likely to be
engaged in because everyone does it, it was a challenge or a thrill, or
because of peer pressure.
DISCUSSION
The results indicate that while the level of participation of
marketing majors in unethical academic activities is very high, their
behavior is typical of that of today's college students. It is
incumbent on faculty and administrators in all academic disciplines to
understand and deal with the problem of academic dishonesty.
Dishonest academic behavior has consequences on the campus and
beyond. Chisholm (1992) enumerated the damages dishonest behavior does
to an institution of higher learning. The reputation of the institution
is diminished in the academic community and with the general public.
Students lose faith in the institution and become alienated. The grades
of honest students may suffer to the extent grading is done "on a
curve." Dishonest behavior that is unchecked gives the impression
it is acceptable, encouraging further participation in such activities.
Correlations have been found between dishonest academic behavior
and behavior on the job. Sierles, Hendrickx, and Circle (1980) found
students who cheated in academic classes in medical school were more
likely to falsify patient records in a clinical setting. Sims (1993)
found significant correlations between the number and severity of
dishonest acts respondents engaged in as students and as employees.
Crown and Spiller (1998) cite theoretical evidence that unethical
behavior tends not to be limited to a specific situation. They found in
their review of theories of organizational ethical decision making that
most theories do not pose different models for different decision making
situations.
Recommended actions that college faculty and administrators can
take to minimize dishonest academic behavior have fallen into three
categories. The first category consists of "clarification"
activities. Their purpose is to make sure students and faculty know what
is considered cheating at the institution. In one survey, of the
respondents who said they had never cheated in college, 12% admitted to
copying homework, 6% to allowing another person to copy from their exam,
and 3% to plagiarism. About 25% of respondents said no college
instructor had ever talked to them about what is considered cheating.
The author recommends the formulation of a cheating policy at the
institutional level, distributed to students in written form and
explained by instructors in the classroom. Examples of forms of cheating
that might not be clear to students, such as plagiarism, should be
included. The policy should then be vigorously enforced (Portello,
1993).
The second category consists of "situational" activities.
Their purpose is to create an environment in which cheating is difficult
to carry out. These activities are applicable to forms of cheating that
take place in the classroom, such as cheating on exams. They include
refraining from re-using exams, using multiple versions of exams, and
proctoring exams closely (Barnett & Dalton, 1981). Note that this
approach addresses the issue of opportunity, not proclivity.
The third category consists of "values-oriented"
activities. Their purpose is to get students to see learning as a valued
activity rather than just as a means to an end such as getting into
graduate school or getting a job. Shrophsire (1997) says students would
not cheat at a sport because it ruins the game. The purpose of the game
is to develop the skill of playing it. Education should be made more
like a sport and less like a CPA exam review course, emphasizing the
memorization of material and playing it back on an exam. Though it is
uncertain how much influence instructors can have on the values of
college-age people, this is the only alternative available for
influencing the kinds of unethical practices that take place outside of
the classroom, out of view of the instructor.
Three types of additional research are suggested. The first is to
replicate the study with other populations of marketing majors to
confirm the findings presented here. The second is to replicate the
study over time to track trends in the academic ethics of marketing
majors. The third is to assess the effectiveness of strategies for
reducing unethical academic behavior as they are developed and
implemented.
REFERENCES
Allen, J., Fuller, D.& Luckett, M. (1998). Academic integrity:
Behaviors, rates, and attitudes of business students toward cheating.
Journal of Marketing Education, 20(1), 41-53.
Baird, J. S. (1980). Current trends in college cheating. Psychology
in the Schools, 17(4), 515-522.
Barnette, D. C. & Dalton, J. C. (1981). Why college students
cheat. Journal of College Student Personnel, 22, 545-551.
Bowers, W. J. (1966). Student dishonesty and its control in
college. (University Microfilms No. 679326)
Brown, B. (1994). Academic ethics of graduate engineering students.
Chemical Engineering Education, 28(4), 242-265.
Brown, B. (1995). The academic ethics of graduate business
students: A survey. Journal of Education for Business, 70(3), 151-156.
Chisholm, D. (1992). An epidemic of cheating? Political Science
& Politics, 25(2), 264-272.
Crown, D. F., & Spiller, M. S. (1998). Learning from the
literature on collegiate cheating: A review of empirical research.
Journal of Business Ethics, 17, 683-700.
Davis, S. F., & Ludvigson, W. H. (1995). Additional data on
academic dishonesty and a proposal for remediation. Teaching of
Psychology, 22(2), 119-121.
Genereux, R. L. & McLeod, B. A. (1995). Circumstances
surrounding cheating: A questionnaire study of college students.
Research in Higher Education, 36(6), 687-704.
Graham, M., Monday, J., O'Brien, K. & Steffen, S. (1994).
Cheating at small colleges: An examination of student and faculty
attitudes and behaviors. Journal of College Student Development, 35(4),
255-260.
Greene, A. S. & Saxe, L. (1992). Everybody (else) does it:
Academic cheating. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern
Psychological Association, April 3-5, Boston, MA. (ERIC Document No.
347931).
Haines, V. J., Diekhoff, G. M., LaBeff, E. E., & Clark, R. E.
(1986). College cheating: Immaturity, lack of commitment, and the
neutralizing attitude. Research in Higher Education, 25(4), 342-354.
Jeffery, C. (1993). Ethical development of accounting students,
non-accounting business students, and liberal arts students. Issues in
Accounting Education, 8(1), 86-96.
Karlins, M., Michaels, C., Freilinger, P. & Walker, H. (1989).
Sex differences in academic dishonesty: College cheating in a management
course. Journal of Education for Business, 65 (October), 31-33.
McCabe, D. L. & Trevino, L. K. (1996). What we know about
cheating in college: Longitudinal trends and recent developments.
Change, 28(1), 28-33.
Meade, J. (1992). Cheating: Is academic dishonesty par for the
course? Prism, 1(7), 30-32.
Newstrom, J. W. & Ruch, W. A. (1976). The ethics of business
students: Preparation for a career. AACSB Bulletin, April, 21-30.
Nonis, S. A. & Swift, C. O. (1998). Deterring cheating behavior
in the marketing classroom: An analysis of the effects of demographics,
attitudes, and in-class deterrent strategies. Journal of Marketing
Education, 20(3), 188-200.
Nowell, C. & Laufer, D. (1997). Undergraduate student cheating
in the fields of business and economics. Journal of Economic Education,
28(1), 3-13.
Nuss, E. M. (1984). Academic integrity: Comparing faculty and
student attitudes. Improving College and University Teaching, 32(3),
140-144.
Portello, P. (1993). First-year students and cheating: A study at
Keene College. Research Strategies, 11(Summer), 174-179.
Roig, M. & Ballew, C. (1994). Attitudes toward cheating of self
and others by college students and professors. Psychological Record,
44(1), 3-12.
Shropshire, W. O. (1997). Of being and getting: Academic
dishonesty. Liberal Education, 83(4), 24-32.
Sierles, F., Hendrickx, I. & Circle, S. (1980). Cheating in
medical school. Journal of Medical Education, 55(2), 124-125.
Sims, R. L. (1993). The relationship between academic dishonesty
and unethical business practices. Journal of Education for Business,
68(4), 207-211.
Singhal, A. C. (1982). Factors in student dishonesty. Psychological
Reports, 51, 775-780.
Stern, E. B. & Havlicek, L. (1986) Academic misconduct: Results
of faculty and undergraduate student surveys. Journal of Allied Health,
15(2), 129- 142.
Stevens, G. E. (1984). Ethical inclinations of tomorrow's
citizens: Actions speak louder? Journal of Business Education, 59(4),
147-152.
Tom, G. & Borin, N. (1988). Cheating in academe. Journal of
Education for Business, 63(January), 153-157.
Bob S. Brown, Marshall University Graduate College
Joseph Abramson, Marshall University
Table 1. Respondent Characteristic
Characteristic Percent
Class rank:
Junior 26.2
Senior 73.8
GPA:
Less than 3.0 52.4
3.0 and above 47.6
Hours of employment per week:
Less than 20 42.9
20 hours or more 57.1
Semester hour course load:
1 to 12 7.3
More than 12 92.7
Gender:
Female 50.0
Male 50.0
Age:
Under 25 95.2
25 and older 4.8
Table 2. Participation In and Ethical Level of Practices
Participation
Practice Rank Pct. (1) Mean (2)
Having someone check over a paper 1 100.0 2.38
before turning it in
Asking about the content of exam 2 92.9 2.67
from someone who has taken it
Giving information about the content 3 90.5 2.84
of an exam to someone who has not
yet taken it
Working with others on an individual 4 85.7 3.64
project
Padding a bibliography 5 78.6 3.73
Before taking an exam, looking at a 6 66.7 3.14
copy that was not supposed to be
available to students
Allowing another to see exam answers 7 64.3 4.26
Plagiarism 8 59.5 4.28
Visiting a professor to influence 9 54.8 3.74
grade
Taking credit for full participation 10/11 45.2 4.16
in a group project without doing a
fair share of the work
Using a false excuse to delay an 10/11 45.2 4.53
exam or paper
Copying off another's exam 12 42.9 4.56
Using exam crib notes 13 38.1 3.87
Passing answers during an exam 14 35.7 4.47
Having information programmed into a 15 28.6 3.33
calculator during an exam
Turning in work done by someone else 16 26.2 4.64
as one's own
Overall percent admitting 100.0
participation
Ethical Level
Practice Rank (3) Mean (4)
Having someone check over a paper 1 4.45
before turning it in
Asking about the content of exam 2 3.43
from someone who has taken it
Giving information about the content 3 3.40
of an exam to someone who has not
yet taken it
Working with others on an individual 4 3.24
project
Padding a bibliography 6 2.88
Before taking an exam, looking at a 7 2.79
copy that was not supposed to be
available to students
Allowing another to see exam answers 13 2.17
Plagiarism 10 2.43
Visiting a professor to influence 5 3.05
grade
Taking credit for full participation 8 2.52
in a group project without doing a
fair share of the work
Using a false excuse to delay an 9 2.45
exam or paper
Copying off another's exam 16 1.86
Using exam crib notes 12 2.26
Passing answers during an exam 14 1.95
Having information programmed into a 11 2.33
calculator during an exam
Turning in work done by someone else 15 1.93
as one's own
Overall percent admitting
participation
(1) Percent admitting participation
(2) Scale: 1 = frequently, 5 = infrequently
(3) Ranked from least to most unethical
(4) Scale: 1 = very unethical, 5 = not at all unethical
Table 3. Significant Differences in Participation by Student
Characteristics
GPA
Practice <3.0 3.0 & up Chi sq. ** P
Padding a bibliography 90.9 * 65.0 4.18 .041
Before taking an exam, 81.8 50.0 4.77 .029
looking at a copy that was
not supposed to be available
to students
Allowing another to see exam
answers
Plagiarism 77.3 40.0 6.04 .014
Visiting a professor to 72.7 35.0 6.02 .014
influence grade
Taking credit for full 63.6 25.0 6.31 .012
participation in a group
project without doing a fair
share of the work
Using exam crib notes
Sex
Practice F M Chi sq. ** P
Padding a bibliography
Before taking an exam, 52.4 81.0 3.86 .050
looking at a copy that was
not supposed to be available
to students
Allowing another to see exam 47.6 81.0 5.08 .024
answers
Plagiarism 42.9 76.2 4.84 .028
Visiting a professor to
influence grade
Taking credit for full
participation in a group
project without doing a fair
share of the work
Using exam crib notes 19.0 57.1 6.46 .011
* Percent participating
** df = 1 for all tests
Table 4. Reasons for Unethical Behavior
Reason Mean (1)
To get a high grade 4.43
Has the time but does not study 4.07
Difficulty of material 3.86
Feels no one is hurt by behavior 3.79
Low risk of getting caught 3.52
Does not have time to study 3.48
Feels work is irrelevant 3.10
Instructor is poor or indifferent 3.18
Everyone does it 2.76
Peer pressure to do it 2.45
Was a challenge or thrill 2.29
(1) Scale: 1 = not at all likely, 5 = very likely.