Gender-deconstructing consumption: Scale development and validation.
Larsen, Val ; Wright, Newell D.
INTRODUCTION
Among the many social and historical changes that have occurred in
this century, none is more important or has more far reaching
implications than changes in the role and economic status of women.
Unsurprisingly, these changes have had and will continue to have a
profound effect on business and public policy. It is, therefore,
imperative that businesspeople, consumer advocates, and public policy
makers attend to developments in this area. This article provides a
scale which can be used to monitor changes in consumption preferences
that flow from the evolution of gender ideology.
THE CONSTRUCTION OF GENDER
The idea that women are inferior and should be subordinate has a
history sufficiently long that it is deeply embedded in both language
and custom. Its more subtle manifestations are, therefore, difficult to
detect and even more difficult to change (Smith, 1987). To take just one
example, women's sphere of influence and range of career
opportunities have been limited by the restriction on women in combat, a
restriction framed as a beneficent protection, but a protection that no
woman could wave if she wished to obtain the opportunities that were its
price. Asymmetries encoded in language are, if anything, even more
difficult to unmask than those which take the form of custom (Larsen,
1993).
Fortunately, Jacques Derrida (1973; Culler, 1982), a prominent
post-structuralist philosopher, has provided an insight which uncovers
some of the hidden asymmetries. Language, he points out, is replete with
dichotomous and apparently equivalent pairs: man/woman, male/female,
boy/girl, husband/wife, mind/body, north/south, positive/negative,
rich/poor, competent/incompetent. Derrida's insight is that the
equivalence implied by the pairing of these terms masks an actual
asymmetry. In each case, the first term in the pair is preferred or
"valorized." This valorization is revealed in the fact that it
sounds odd to reverse the order of the terms when they are used
together, i. e. say south and north, poor and rich, female and male. It
is revealed, too, in the fact that the first term reflects the
historical locus of power or value. Historically, the north has been
more prosperous and powerful than the south (in both the US and the
world), the rich more comfortable and respected than the poor, men more
honored and obeyed than women. People in power have constructed the
language code in such a way that it hides these asymmetries behind a
falsely implied balance and, thus, makes them seem natural. Derrida and
other post-structuralists unmask the conventionality of these
distinctions, showing that they are rooted not in nature but in group
interests and the exercise of group power.
Consumer researchers have long since pointed out that consumption,
like these verbal dichotomies, is heavily gender coded (Courtney &
Lockeretz, 1971; Davis & Rigaux, 1974; Gentry & Doering, 1977).
It has more recently become apparent that the asymmetries uncovered by
Derrida's linguistic analyses are manifest in gender coded
consumption as well. For instance, Fontenelle and Zinkhan (1992) suggest
that the experience of leisure is asymmetrical for women and men. The
leisure of a woman is likely to be tied to that of her partner and
children so that even on vacation, her needs tend to be subordinated to
the needs of her family. The leisure of men, on the other hand, tends to
be relatively free from constraint. In a similar vein, Wallendorf and
Arnould (1991) found that Thanksgiving Day, a day of leisure and
relaxation for men, often involves very hard work for women, who must
cook the ritual feast, then clean up afterward. And Fischer and Arnold
(1990) have shown that family Christmas shopping tends to be framed as a
compulsory duty for women but as an optional pleasure for men.
Since language is at root a system of differences (Saussure, 1959),
linguistic and social dichotomies are probably unavoidable. However,
asymmetries in both language and custom can be "deconstructed"
as well as constructed. That is, dichotomies can be reframed in such a
way that they embody genuine balance or, even more instructively, in
such a way that the once marginal term (or gender) becomes valorized.
When the latter happens, the entire network of assumptions and
socioeconomic relationships is cast in a new light, a light that
discloses the hidden agenda and interests of those who created the
original asymmetrical dichotomy (Culler, 1982, p. 150). A striking
consumer behavior example of the more radical move is the recent actions
of the Barbie Liberation Organization. This group surreptitiously switched the voice boxes on G.I. Joe and Barbie dolls. As a result,
girls around the United States heard their Barbies proclaim, "Dead
men tell no lies"; boys heard G.I. Joe perkily ask, "Want to
go shopping?" (Associated Press, 1993). In this reversal, women are
coded as powerful and violent mistresses of their domain. They are
active and produce a ghoulish product--dead bodies. Men are coded as
socially engaged consumers who are passive, compliant, and somewhat
frivolous.
The practical, day-to-day importance of gender deconstruction in
the consumer domain has recently been made clear by Frances Cerra
Whittelsey (1993). Citing one example after another, Whittelsey shows
that distinctions in the marketplace between male and female tend to be
costly for women. She reports, for instance, that a given style of knit
shirt almost always costs more in the women's department of a
clothing store than in the men's. And laundering the shirt at a dry
cleaner is also more expensive for women, traditionally about 27 percent
more for the same style of shirt. In New York, women generally pay more
than men for a basic shampoo, cut, and blow dry at a hair salon. And
though women buy about half of all new cars in the United States, they
are not treated equitably by dealers. Compared with white men, white
women pay, on average, about $150 more and black women about $800 more
for the same car. In Whittelsey's analysis, the valorization of the
male in the marketplace produces a cash bonus for males and a cash
penalty for females. Thus, women have a very tangible economic interest
in deconstructing the existing male/female distinction. Unfortunately,
for the time being, most women and supportive men too seldom make the
consumption choices that would promote this interest.
MEASURES OF GENDER
Since it is a social and linguistic construct, not a biological
attribute, gender cannot be equated with sex. It is most often defined
as a personality disposition and most often measured with the Bem
Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI). The BSRI can measure the extent to which a
given woman or man's personality deconstructs or constructs the
gender distinction. Thus, Frable (1989) has used the BSRI to divide
people into four groups: sex typed (feminine women, masculine men),
androgynous (both feminine and masculine), undifferentiated (neither
feminine nor masculine), and cross-sex-typed (masculine women, feminine
men). Sex typed individuals validate the traditional link between sex
and gender in their own personality and in their interactions with and
judgements of others (Anderson & Bem, 1981). Androgenous,
undifferentiated, and, most radically, cross-sex-typed individuals
deconstruct the traditional link between sex and gender (Frable, 1989).
Useful as it is, the BSRI has had limited power to predict
behaviors of interest. As Bem herself has pointed out, "the
literature is littered with failed attempts to use the [BSRI] and
Personal Attributes Questionnaire (PAQ) to predict a wide variety of
gender-related attitudes and behaviors" (Bem, 1985, p. 196).
Commenting on consumer behavior in particular, Meyers-Levy (1988, p.
522) has made a similar point: "Consumer behavior researchers have
met with only limited success in relating sex roles to product
perceptions." Frable (1989) argues that these failures may result
from attempting to use sex typing, a personality disposition, in
contexts where gender ideology, a belief system, would be a more
appropriate predictor. While measures of gender ideology already exist
(Antil et al., 1981; Kalin & Tilby, 1978; Spence, Helmreich, &
Stapp, 1973), they measure broad social and political ideology. Our
objective was to develop a measure of gender ideology which was
consumption-specific, one which could be used to estimate
consumers' tendency to construct or deconstruct gender distinctions
through their consumption choices. Such a measure must be developed if
we are to assess the effects of gender-deconstructing attitudes and
behaviors and the effectiveness of treatments designed to foster these
attitudes and behaviors in the marketplace.
Our scale was developed using procedures outlined by Churchill,
1979) and DeVellis (1991). Following the specification of the domain,
two professors and two graduate students expert in consumer behavior
and/or women's studies generated 52 items which, in their
judgement, tapped the tendency to construct or deconstruct gender
through consumption choices. These experts were instructed to suggest
consumption-related items that would produce a differential response in
subjects disposed to support traditional, asymmetrical gender roles
versus those disposed to question and resist traditional roles. The
52-item scale generated in this way was purified through four successive
stages, each of which involved data collection, factor analysis, and the
calculation of Cronbach's alpha. During the first two rounds of
this purification process, twelve additional items were generated, two
of which were eventually incorporated in the final scale.
To explore interrelationships among items in the initial set and to
suggest new items, data were gathered and analyzed in three preliminary
rounds in which 30, 27, and 65 subjects were used. In each of these
three rounds, responses were factor analyzed using principal components
analysis in SAS with the MINEIGEN > 1 and then successively more
restrictive NFACTOR parameters. Item-to-total correlations and
Cronbach's alpha were also calculated. The objective was to enhance
construct cohesiveness and validity by identifying items that loaded on
major factors, that held together as the number of factors was
restricted, that had high item-to-total correlations, and that improved
reliability (Nunnally, 1978). Items that performed poorly on these
criteria were retained in successive rounds but were moved to the end of
the scale so that they would not influence responses to items that
performed well. There were 52 items and 10 factors with an eigenvalue greater than one in the first round, 58 items and 12 factors in the
second round, and 64 items and 15 factors in the third round.
In the final round of the scale development and purification
process, 67 subjects responded to the survey. The results for particular
items were generally consistent with those in the preliminary rounds.
Consequently, 11 items at the beginning of this scale were selected for
the final scale, items that had been moved to the beginning because they
had loaded on major factors and had good item-to-total correlations.
Factor analysis on these 11 items yielded a two factor solution that
explained 55 percent of the response variance. Cronbach's alpha was
.87, and item-to-total correlations ranged from .48 to .68. When added
to these 11, other items in the survey created new, relatively unstable
factors and/or reduced scale reliability.
The scale was validated by examining with new samples its factor
structure, its reliability, and its convergent/ discriminant and
predictive validities. The subjects were students enrolled in business
classes. Two sets of subjects were used to avoid an overlong survey and
consequent fatigue. The first had 132 subjects (48 female, 84 male), the
second 153 subjects (55 female, 98 male).
FACTOR STRUCTURE
For both validation samples as for the development sample, factor
analysis yielded a two-factor solution. For the first sample, the
solution explained 49 percent of the response variance, for the second
51 percent. All factor loadings above .30 are reported for both samples
in Table 2. The factor structure in the first validation sample is most
clearly interpretable. Factor 2 taps attitudes toward gender typing in
occupational roles. Items having to do with women pilots and automobile
mechanics and male housekeepers load most heavily on this factor and do
not load of factor 1. The item on boys playing with dolls may also cast
males in the homemaker role while that on women wearing suit coats may
cast females in a businessman role. Factor 1 seems to group responses to
products and services. Consistent with this interpretation, the doll and
suit items load on this factor while the more unambiguously occupational
items do not.
The factor structure for the second validation sample is generally
consistent with that in the first. All items that loaded on factor 2 in
the first sample also load on it in the second. The same is true for
items that loaded on factor 1, except for item 5. While most loadings
are the same, the magnitudes have changed, so the second factor
structure is less unambiguously interpretable than the first. Even so,
there appear to be two relatively stable factors that explain a
substantial proportion of the variance in attitudes toward products or
services that construct or deconstruct traditional gender codes.
RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY
Scale reliability was acceptable in all validation samples. For the
first sample (n = 132), Cronbach's alpha was .83. Item-to-total
correlations ranged from .37 to .64. Two weeks later, the scale was
again administered to 108 of these subjects (43 female, 65 male) to
assess test/retest reliability. The correlation between the first and
second scores was .85. Individual item-to-item correlations ranged from
.30 to .71 with most being in the .60 range. For the second sample (n =
153), Cronbach's alpha was .86. Item-to-total correlations ranged
from .45 to .69.
The convergent/discriminant validity of the scale was assessed
using one scale which should and two scales which should not correlate
highly with genderconstructing/deconstructing consumption. The scale
which should correlate is Kalin and Tilby's (1978) Sex-Role
Ideology Scale (SRIS). Since our scale is designed to be a
consumption-specific sex-role ideology scale, it should correlate highly
with more generalized sex-role ideology scales. This expectation was
confirmed by the SRIS's .61 correlation with the first (n = 132)
and .56 correlation with the second (n = 153) validation scale.
Our Gender-Constructing/Deconstructing Consumption (GC/DC) scale
differs from other sex-role ideology scales in its focus on consumption.
On this point of difference, it resembles materialism scales which also
tap a tendency to consume. To confirm that the GC/DC did not merely tap
differences in materialism, we measured level of materialism using
Richins and Dawson's (1992) materialism scale. Consistent with our
expectation that the GC/DC has a consumption dimension but does not tap
general materialism, the correlation between the scales was .18 (n =
132) and .23 (n = 153).
The GC/DC is designed to measure a tendency to support or challenge
traditional gender codes. To confirm that it does not merely tap a
proclivity to monitor and comply with or violate the expectations of
others (i.e. be feminist among feminists, traditional among
traditionalists, or visa versa), we measured level of attention to
social comparison information using a scale developed by Lennox and
Wolfe (1984). The .11 (n = 132) and .17 (n = 153) correlations between
the two scales suggest that the GC/DC does not measure a general
tendency to construct/ deconstruct social conventions but is, rather,
sex-ideology specific.
The high correlation between the GC/DC and Kalin and Tilby's
(1978) SRIS establishes convergent validity but also raises a question:
Do we need a new scale given this high correlation with the existing
one? Our answer is that, in addition to being shorter (11 items rather
than 30) and more reliable (.83 to .89 Cronbach's alpha rather than
the .57 to .84 reported for the SRIS--.74 and .72 for our samples), the
GC/DC seems to have better predictive validity in consumer behavior
contexts. We tested the predictive validity of the competing scales by
regressing the composite scores on various gender-related consumption
attitudes. Judging from the R2 and the p-value of the regressions, the
GC/DC outperforms the SRIS as a predictor of these attitudes.
The 46 items used as dependent variables included some suggested by
our experts as measures of gender deconstruction that were not included
in the final scale and others generated specifically for this test of
predictive validity. Items measured attitudes toward progressive or
nonprogressive business practices (e.g., I am more likely to purchase a
company's products if I have heard it actively seeks to promote
women employees), toward the sex-typing of occupations (I like to see
women move into occupations traditionally held by men and visa versa),
toward the sex-typing of consumption roles (A wife should be primarily
responsible for purchasing the family groceries) toward product classes
traditionally marketed to one sex or the other (I enjoy reading or
watching science fiction), toward products that mark gender (It is a
good idea to dress baby boys in blue, baby girls in pink), and toward
specific brands that have been criticized for reinforcing asymmetrical
gender distinctions (Barbie, Keystone Beer, Hooters Restaurants: My
attitude toward Hooters Restaurants is positive). All items were
measured on a 5-point scale anchored by strongly agree and strongly
disagree. Half were administered to the first, half to the second
validation sample.
While neither scale significantly predicted attitudes toward
science fiction, handguns, sport utility vehicles, stylish clothing,
traditional weddings, or Stroh's and Keystone Beer, the GC/DC did
significantly predict attitudes on 35, the SRIS on 31 of the 46
dependent variables. Focusing on the 37 variables for which one or both
scales were a significant predictor, the GC/DC had an average R2 of .16,
the SRIS of .10. The GC/DC, thus, appears to have considerable utility
in predicting a broad range of consumption attitudes.
IMPLICATIONS FOR SOCIAL ACTION
The strong relationship between the underlying attitude tapped by
the GC/DC and the broad range of consumption attitudes and behaviors
measured in the tests of predictive validity have noteworthy
implications for businesspeople and consumer policy makers. It suggests
that business practices or policy initiatives which change this
underlying attitude will have important and generally progressive
marketplace consequences, i.e., more employment opportunities, better
products, and better service for women. And changes in attitudes toward
asymmetrical gender codes are likely to be progressive because most
people have a powerful economic interest in adopting the more liberal
attitudes reflected in higher GC/DC scores: deconstructing gender
distinctions makes the economy more efficient and leads to an increase
in aggregate economic well being.
In his pioneering work on the economics of discrimination, Gary
Becker (1971) has demonstrated that discrimination based on race and sex
is a form of domestic protectionism that, like protectionism in
international markets, reduces aggregate economic output. When employers
discriminate in hiring, they must pay a wage premium to hire from an
arbitrarily restricted labor pool. In addition to hurting themselves,
they waste the skills of the groups they discriminate against. Likewise,
when consumers discriminate in their buying, they pay a price premium
for buying from an arbitrarily restricted pool of suppliers. They reduce
their own standard of living and the standard of living of the groups
they discriminate against. It is true that the group favored by those
who discriminate (whites, males) may benefit, but their economic gain
will be only a small proportion of the aggregate economy's loss. So
even the favored group may be worse off in absolute terms than they
would have been in a more robust economy that was unencumbered by the
costs of racism or sexism.
While all may suffer, Becker makes it clear that the group
discriminated against suffers the greatest loss. And the masking power
of language and ideology notwithstanding, it is difficult to hide
disadvantages from a disadvantaged class. Most victims of discrimination
experience dissonance which alerts them to hidden asymmetries (Frable,
1989; Showalter, 1977). So disadvantaged groups--blacks, women--are
likely to see through the cultural codes that legitimize and naturalize economic discrimination against them, though there are usually some
group members who embrace language and ideas that limit their
opportunities.
This tendency for a disadvantaged group to deconstruct injurious distinctions was apparent in our samples. As expected, women proved to
be much more inclined than men to deconstruct the gender codes that
disadvantage them. In both of our validation samples, women's GC/DC
scores were much higher than those of men (Table 3). The attitudes
reflected in these scores provides a firm foundation for social action
in business and politics, for women's numbers (more than half of
the population) and control of hundreds of billions in spending give
them enormous potential economic and political power. The next two
sections discuss options available to businesspeople, consumer
advocates, and policy makers who wish to foster the deconstruction of
gender codes.
The discrimination against women that Whittelsey (1993) shows is
rife among American businesses creates a major opportunity for
progressive businesspeople who want to combine doing good with doing
well, for businesses can profit by deconstructing asymmetrical gender
distinctions. A dry cleaner who charges women 27 percent more than men
to launder a shirt is either earning an exceptional profit on cleaning
women's shirts or subsidizing the below-cost price for laundering
men's shirts with an above-cost price for women's. Assuming
that the dry cleaning market is price-sensitive, a progressive dry
cleaner could corner the market on women's shirts by charging an
equitable price. And the same is true in other markets where market
failure takes the form of price discrimination against women (Becker
1971).
This equitable pricing strategy should work with women consumers
who are price sensitive, regardless of gender attitudes. But in
communities where GC/DC scores are already high or where research shows
that scores rise when consciousness is raised, a business could earn
good will and additional patronage by building a promotional campaign
around the deconstruction of an asymmetrical gender distinction. Since
the network of assumptions that undergird a distinction are most fully
exposed when the implicit valorization is reversed, such a campaign
might be most effective if it reversed the status of women and men. A
dry cleaner might, for instance, abandon equitable pricing for one week
and, with some fanfare, invert their competitors' practice by
charging 27 percent more to launder a man's shirt. The same thing
could be done in any market where a gender distinction harms women.
Price discrimination is an economically inefficient and relatively
obvious instance of asymmetrical gender coding. As such, it may be
comparatively easy to eliminate through direct competition. Less
obviously asymmetric gender distinctions may be harder to identify and
eliminate but may provide even greater long-term potential for a
business to capitalize on progressive policies. It is easier for
competitors to change to equitable pricing than it is for them to match
good will won by being at the forefront of an important cultural change.
Levi Strauss is an example of a company that has built a powerful brand
name in part by being sensitive to cultural change (Economist, 1991). In
a recent jeans commercial, they continue that tradition by positioning
themselves at the forefront of efforts to deconstruct traditional gender
codes. In the commercial, the figure in a dress that symbolizes a
woman's restroom comes to life, takes off her skirt, and turns it
into a rope. Climbing down the rope, she escapes the frozen immobility
of the restroom door. As the commercial closes, she pulls on the rope
and the circle that framed her on the door falls to the floor and
splinters. She stand free and uncircumscribed, clad in Levi's
jeans, traditional clothing of frontier males. In this commercial, Levi
Strauss aligns itself and its product with women who consciously or
unconsciously feel constrained by the gender codes that surround them.
The company promotes the use of its product as an act of personal
assertion through gender-deconstructing consumption. Similar
repositioning through gender code deconstruction is possible for many
businesses and products.
While market forces and business activism can foster changes in
gender perceptions, the upper echelons of corporate America are still
massively dominated by males, who, judging from GC/DC scores in our
samples, are relatively unconcerned about deconstructing asymmetrical
gender distinctions. This result is unsurprising, for compared with
women, men tend to be less concerned about socially responsible
consumption in a variety of domains (Roberts, 1993). But the
backwardness and resistance of men may not be that important. Women
control hundreds of billions in their own income and even more in family
income since they still do most household shopping (Roper, 1990;
Schwartz & Miller, 1991). So if women consumers can be motivated to
deconstruct gender codes through their consumption choices, businesses
will be compelled to be responsive. They cannot afford to ignore the
power of the purse.
Through education and lobbying, consumer organizations can play an
important role in motivating women to deploy their economic and
political power against harmful gender distinctions. For instance, Ralph
Nader's Center for Responsive Law has published Frances Cerra
Whittelsey's (1993) Why Women Pay More: How to Avoid Marketplace
Perils, an example of effective gender code deconstruction by a consumer
advocate. In this book, Whittelsey argues in the text (and Nader in the
introduction) that women need to become more reflective and demanding
consumers. Whittelsey educates women on business practices and products
that do not serve their interests and explains how they can avoid and
punish exploitation. The book has the merit of providing concrete
examples of invidious gender distinctions that harm women and of
effective consumer responses that deconstruct those distinctions. But it
also has one potential weakness. In emphasizing the responsibility to be
an informed consumer, Whittelsey may add one more burden to the already
overburdened women who now spend an inequitable amount of time on child
care and household shopping.
While educating consumers to be more conscientious is important, it
may be still more important to reduce the search costs of the many women
who, judging from GC/DC scores in our samples, are already disposed to
be gender-deconstructing consumers. Efforts to foster green marketing
and green consumption show the variety of ways in which this might be
done. Taking a positive tack as some environmentalist organizations have
done (Ottman, 1994), feminist and consumer organizations could present
awards to businesses that have excelled in hiring and promoting women
and/or in developing and marketing products that deconstruct traditional
gender distinctions. Recipients could be encouraged to use their awards
in product promotions. A more ambitious but more effective strategy
would be to develop an equivalent of the green seal that has been used
to mark environmentally friendly products (List, 1993). This would
require the establishment of an institution to administer the seal and
of guidelines on how to earn it. It would have the advantage of
identifying progressive companies and products in the marketplace and,
thus, allow women to recognize and reward gender equity while incurring
very low search costs. Taking the more negative tack that Greenpeace has
adopted in the environmental movement, groups like the Barbie Liberation
Organization can continue to subject companies that victimize women to
negative publicity. Since large businesses generally dislike
controversy, tactics of this sort could motivate some positive changes.
Finally, consumer advocates and individual consumers should not
neglect political action. As Gregory (1987) has pointed out, the
appropriate model here is the political and legal fight against racial
discrimination. Since women constitute political majorities in most
developed countries, they have been and will continue to be big winners
when governments intervene to limit discrimination through laws such as
The Equal Credit Opportunity Act of 1974 and to remedy past
discrimination through regulations requiring affirmative action (Gleckman, Smart, & Dwyer, 1991). Even if laws do not explicitly
forbid practices such as the kinds of gender-based price discrimination
that Whittelsey (1993) has identified, government action can raise the
perceived price of a legal challenge and, thus, make businesses more
responsive to consumer pressures.
LIMITATIONS AND DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
The principal limitation of this study is its use of a
demographically restricted sample in the development and validation of
the GC/DC scale--college business students. While young professionals
are very important to most businesses and to policy makers, it is
possible and even likely that these consumers may differ from other
consumers, especially the elderly. So the most obvious extension of this
study would be to validate the GC/DC scale with other demographic
groups. Once validated with a relevant group, activists in business,
government, and consumer organizations could use the instrument to
evaluate efforts to encourage gender-deconstructing behavior. The level
of GC/DC scores among various populations and the degree to which scores
can be raised by initiatives such as those discussed above may indicate
the degree to which political and consumer pressures can be mobilized to
deconstruct gender distinctions that harm the interests of women.
REFERENCES
Anderson, S. & Bem, S. (1981). Sex typing and androgyny in
dyadic interaction: Individual differences in responsiveness to physical
attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 74-86.
Antil, J. K., Cunningham, J. D., Russell, G. & Thompson, N. L.
(1981). An Australian sex-role scale. Australian Journal of Psychology.
33, 169-183.
Associated Press. (1993). 'Terrorist' act has G.I. Joe
shopping. Roanoke Times & World News. 30 December.
Becker, G. S. (1971). The economics of discrimination. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Bem, S. (1985). Androgyny and gender schema theory: A conceptual
and empirical integration. Nebraska symposium on motivation: Psychology
and gender. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 179-226.
Churchill, G. (1979). A paradigm for developing better measures of
marketing constructs. Journal of Marketing Research. 16, 64-73.
Courtney, A. E. & Lockeretz, S. W. (1971). A woman's
place: An analysis of the roles portrayed by women in magazine
advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research. 8, 92-95.
Culler, J. (1982). On deconstruction. Ithaca, New York: Cornell
University Press.
Derrida, J. (1973). Speech and phenomena: And other essays on
Husserl's theory of signs. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.
Davis, H. & Rigaux, B. (1974). Perception of marital roles in
decision processes. Journal of Consumer Research. 1, 51-61.
DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development: Theory and applications.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Economist. (1991). A comfortable fit. The Economist. 319 (June
22nd), 67-68.
Fisher, E. & Arnold, S. J. (1990). More than a labor of love:
Gender roles and Christmas gift shopping. Journal of Consumer Research.
17, 333-345.
Fontenelle, S. M. & Zinkhan, G. M. (1992). Gender Differences
in the Perception of Leisure: A Conceptual Model. Advances in Consumer
Research. 20, 534-540.
Frable, D. E. S. (1989). Sex typing and gender ideology: Two facets
of the individual's gender psychology that go together. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology. 56, 95-108.
Gentry, J. & Doering, M. (1977). Masculinity-femininity related
to consumption choice. Advances in Consumer Research. 5, 423-437.
Gleckman, H., Smart, T. & Dwyer, P. (1991). Race in the
workplace. BusinessWeek. July 8, 50-63.
Gregory, J. (1987). Sex, race and law. London: Sage Publications.
Kalin, R. & Tilby, P. J. (1978). Development and validation of
a sex-role ideology scale. Psychological Reports. 42, 731-738.
Larsen, Val. (1993). A sociolinguistic approach to gender and
personal selling. Advances in Consumer Research. 20, 48-51.
Lennox, R. D. & Wolfe, R. N. (1984). Revision of the
self-monitoring scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 46,
1349-1364.
List, S. K. (1993). The green seal of eco-approval. American
Demographics. 15 (January), 910.
Meyers-Levy, Joan (1988). The influence of sex roles on judgment.
Journal of Consumer Research. 14, 522-530.
Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ottman, J. (1994). Nominations sought for 'green' awards.
Marketing News. 28 (19), 19.
Richins, M. L. & Dawson, S. (1992). Materialism as a consumer
value: Measure development and validation. Journal of Consumer Research.
19, 303-316.
Roberts, J. (1993). Sex differences in socially responsible
consumers' behavior. Psychological Reports. 73, 139-148.
Roper Poll (1990). The environment: Public attitudes and individual
behavior. New York: The Roper Organization.
de Saussure, F. (1959). Course in general linguistics. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Schwartz, J & Miller, T. (1991). The earth's best friends.
American Demographics. 13 (February), 26-35.
Showalter, E. (1977). A literature of their own. Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Smith, D. (1987). The everyday world as problematic. Boston:
Northeastern University Press.
Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. & Stapp, J. (1973). A short
version of the Attitude Toward Women Scale (AWS). Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society. 23, 210-220.
Wallendorf, M. & Arnould, E. J. (1991). 'We gather
together': Consumption rituals of Thanksgiving Day. Journal of
Consumer Research. 18, 13-31.
Whittelsey, F. C. (1993). Why women pay more: How to avoid
marketplace perils.
Val Larsen, Truman State University Newell D. Wright, James Madison
University
Table 1
1. If a couple is buying a car, the man should take the lead.
2. I would not want my boy to play with dolls.
3. I would be reluctant to fly on a plane that had a woman pilot.
4. A motorcycle is a male, not a female product.
5. The use of power tools like chain saws and electric drills is
more appropriate for men than for women.
6. I believe women's hair should generally be cut and styled
differently from men's hair.
7. * Were one available, I might use the services of a female
automobile mechanic.
8. * A magazine like Good Housekeeping is proper reading for a man.
9. Women should not wear suit coats cut like those customarily
worn by men.
10. On the whole, it is better that sewing be left to women.
11. Plastic surgery is more appropriate for women than for men.
* Starred items are reverse coded.
Table 2
Sample 1 (n = 132) Sample 2 (n = 153)
Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Item Factor 1 Factor 2
5 .76 1 .79
10 .73 4 .69 .36
4 .68 .33 10 .64 .41
11 .67 3 .60 .50
6 .62 6 .59
1 .56 7 .52 .32
3 .78 11 .51
7 .77 2 .49 .38
8 .64 5 .83
2 .33 .42 8 .67
9 .34 .39 9 .65
Principal components analysis with a varimax rotation
Table 3
Sample Sex Mean SD p-value of t-test
n = 132 female 30.79 7.89 0.0001
male 22.84 6.41
n = 153 female 28.96 7.92 0.0001
male 22.06 7.47