Entrepreneurial profiling: a decision policy analysis of the influence of entrepreneurial self-efficacy on entrepreneurial intent.
Brice, Jeff, Jr. ; Spencer, Barbara
ABSTRACT
An unresolved issue in the study of entrepreneurs is what factors
do individuals consider before attempting to establish new ventures?
Also, which of these factors are most influential to a decision after
deliberation is complete? Previous studies have examined similar
questions by developing inquiries of entrepreneurial self-efficacy that
are based solely on discrete business functions. However, not only are
functional assessments too venture-specific for the general nature of
most entrepreneurial self-efficacy research, they are usually
operationalized with self-report direct surveys that are highly
susceptible to social desirability response bias.
In this study, we apply a decision modeling methodology to
empirically assess the influence of human competencies in the
entrepreneurial self-efficacy assessment process. Decision modeling is a
within-subjects analytical procedure that is resistant to external
biases. A significant finding is that self-efficacy assessments
utilizing entrepreneurial competencies are able to successfully
discriminate individuals with strong entrepreneurial intentions from
others. In fact, the resultant decision profile of those with high
entrepreneurial intentions parallels that of actual entrepreneurs.
Results, limitations, and implications for future research are
presented.
INTRODUCTION
A principle inquiry in the research in entrepreneurship is what
factors enhance the probability that someone will decide to start and
manage a new business enterprise? This query is important because,
depending it's the resolution, we will develop diverse methods of
training and supporting individuals to create their own businesses.
Therefore, this vein of study should generate significant insights not
only for academics, but also to practitioners, policy makers, and
oliticians.
Early research on the decision to start a new business tended to
focus either on contextual factors such as job displacement (Shapero
& Sokol, 1982), prior work experience (Mokry, 1988) or on individual
personality factors such as the need for achievement (McClelland, 1965),
internal locus of control (Begley & Boyd, 1987), acceptance of risk
(Brockhaus & Horowitz, 1986), and the tolerance of ambiguity
(Schere, 1982). More recent models of entrepreneurial decision have
adopted a perspective in which the individual is an intentional decision
maker and actor, engaging in the rational appraisal of situational and
personal factors (Bird, 1988, Krueger, 1993). Thus, from the newer
cognitive perspective, external factors and personality factors still
influence the entrepreneurial decision, but only insofar as they are
perceived and interpreted by the potential entrepreneur.
Krueger, Reilly, and Carsrud (2000) compared two models of
entrepreneurial decision-making based on the premise that intention to
start a new venture is the major predictor of entrepreneurial behavior.
In both models (Shapero & Sokol, 1982; Ajzen, 1991), self-efficacy
emerged an important influence on intention. In essence, the belief that
one can personally execute the behaviors needed to create a new venture
is professed to enhance the intent to do so (Boyd & Vozikis, 1994;
Krueger & Brazeal, 1994). The purpose of the present paper is to
build on this cognitive approach by profiling how individuals weigh
different criteria when judging entrepreneurial efficacy.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy
While relatively new to research on entrepreneurship, self-efficacy
is widely recognized as a key construct in social learning theory
(Bandura, 1977), a perspective which assumes that behavior, cognitions,
and the environment continually influence each other in the mindset of
individuals (Bandura, 1977, 1986). Self-efficacy refers to people's
judgments regarding their ability to perform a given activity (Bandura,
1977, 1982, and 1986) and is proposed to influence individual choices,
goals, emotional reactions, effort, ability to cope, and persistence
(Gist, Stevens, & Bavetta, 1991). Hackett and Betz (1981) proposed
that Bandura's (1977) theory of self-efficacy provides a useful
conceptual framework from which to predict the occupational preferences
of individuals. Based on this foundation, Boyd and Vozikis (1994) and
Krueger and Brazeal (1994) helped lodge the notion of self-efficacy
firmly in the entrepreneurship literature by suggesting that perceptions
of entrepreneurial self-efficacy could contribute significantly to an
individual's deliberations about whether, or not, to pursue an
entrepreneurial career.
Even before the appearance of these seminal pieces, Chandler and
Jansen (1992) conducted research on business founders'
self-assessments of "proficiency in the entrepreneurial
function." A strength of this research was their development of a
scale measuring five human competencies associated with the
entrepreneurial, managerial, and technical-functional roles of business
founders (Mintzberg & Waters, 1982; Pavett & Lau, 1983; Schein,
1987). Chandler and Jansen (1992) demonstrated that founders of the most
successful firms in their sample rated themselves higher than others on
capabilities associated with all three of these roles.
More recently, Chen, Greene, and Crick (1998) operationalized
entrepreneurial self-efficacy (ESE) as self-assessed
"certainty" in dealing with 26 specific tasks identified from
prior literature and interviews with several local entrepreneurs
concerning key entrepreneurial roles. After gathering self-ratings on
these tasks from students and business owners/executives, they used
factor analysis to combine them into five categories including
marketing, innovation, management, risk-taking, and financial control.
They also created an overall "ESE" measure, by taking the mean
over all 26 items. Their findings showed that among students, overall
ESE was significantly correlated with the stated intention to start a
business. Among business executives, those who were founders rated
themselves higher on total ESE and particularly, on innovation and
risk-taking, than did non-founders.
While Chandler and Jansen's (1992) and Chen, Greene and
Crick's (1998) results are enticing, further research on
entrepreneurial self-efficacy and the intention to start a new business
is needed. For instance, what criteria do people use when deciding about
their aptitude to start a business? Are some efficacy criteria more
important than others in making this evaluation? Chandler and
Jansen's (1992) most successful entrepreneurs rated themselves
highly on all competencies, while Chen, Greene and Crick's (1998)
founders rated their abilities on innovation and risk-taking more highly
than did non-founders. But neither study tells us which criteria people
consider most, or least, important when judging their ability to start a
new venture. Such information would be particularly important if it
helped us to understand the decision-making processes of prospective
entrepreneurs.
A second nagging unresolved issue regarding entrepreneurial
self-efficacy is the problem of social desirability bias in
self-assessments. Because the notion of self-efficacy inherently
involves people's judgments about their ability to perform given
activities (Bandura, 1982), the use of self-reported survey evaluations
make sense. Yet, in such circumstances, individuals may be tempted to
inflate their ratings (i.e., to impress study evaluators, among other
reasons). In fact, Chen, Greene, and Crick (1998) noted that the high
interfactor correlations among their component entrepreneurial
self-efficacy scores may well have been caused by social desirability
response bias. They stated that future researchers should think of ways
to reduce social desirability.
The study described here is an effort to advance the research on
entrepreneurial self-efficacy and entrepreneurial intentions and to
address the social desirability limitation encountered in Chen, Greene,
and Crick (1998). A decision modeling approach is applied to assess how
individuals weigh several key entrepreneurial competencies in deciding
whether someone would be capable of pursuing a promising business
venture. This method avoids the social desirability dilemma because
respondents make hypothetical decisions based on specified cues.
Decision modeling diminishes the misrepresentation of social
desirability response biases that might be uncovered by asking
respondents to make judgments about ambiguous situations in a seemingly
external world, which serves to expose their genuine sensitivities
(Fischer, 1993). Results are then compared across those who intend to
start a new venture and those who do not. The following general research
questions were addressed in this study:
1. How do individuals weigh specific entrepreneurial abilities when
judging someone's capability to start a new venture? Do some
efficacy criteria matter more than others in making such judgments?
2. Can prospective entrepreneurs be discriminated from others based
upon their application of efficacy criteria in assessing fitness for
entrepreneurial behavior?
Since our research employs a human competency description of
entrepreneurial self-efficacy, we first review the development of the
construct. We then present the decision modeling technique and its
effect on decreasing social desirability response bias. Finally, we
empirically evaluate the effect of entrepreneurial self-efficacy on the
intention to pursue an entrepreneurial career.
Operationalizing Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy
Bandura (1982) defined self-efficacy as the task-specific
consideration of perceived fitness to perform a particular activity. In
the case of entrepreneurship, entrepreneurial self-efficacy may be
comprised of deliberation of those tasks that relate to the initiation
and development of new ventures, which is considered emblematic of the
entrepreneurial act (Livesay, 1982). One way to identify these tasks is
to think about the basic functional areas of business.
For instance, a study by Scherer, Adams, Carley, and Weibe (1989)
operationalized entrepreneurial self-efficacy as expertise in
accounting, production, marketing, human resources, and general
organizational skills. A limitation of this approach is that proficiency
in all of areas may not be required for all new venture efforts. For
instance, while a prospective manufacturer of industrial equipment may
have to consider whether he or she is competent in all of the
aforementioned functional responsibilities before attempting to develop
a new venture, an independent hot-dog cart operator may only have to
consider his or her basic accounting and marketing skills before
launching a new hot-dog cart operation. As this example demonstrates,
the assessment of specific functional abilities before new venture
initiation is dependent on the scope and scale of the particular venture
being considered.
Moreover, an entrepreneurial self-efficacy construct based solely
on functional capabilities ignores the fact that co-opting from external
sources may solve some functional shortcomings, on the part of the
prospective entrepreneur. For example, an individual who lacks
accounting/bookkeeping skill can easily and inexpensively purchase that
service from an independent contractor. Knowing this, a prospective
entrepreneur without sufficient accounting expertise may still be
willing to undertake the development of a new venture. Because a
negative perception of one's fitness in some functional capacities
may not have the predicted effect on entrepreneurial behavior, it seems
likely that a functional capability description of entrepreneurial
self-efficacy may not have a decisive influence on whether or not one
decides to pursue an entrepreneurial career.
Instead of considering narrow functional tasks, a different
approach to clarifying entrepreneurial efficacy is to consider the
broader human competencies associated with new venture development since
human competency assessments are less dependent on the specification and
complexity of particular new venture entry domains. Drawing from
writings by Mintzberg and Waters (1982); Pavett and Lau (1983); and
Schein (1987), Chandler and Jansen (1992) identified five such
competencies based on the three primary roles of the entrepreneur: the
entrepreneurial, managerial, and technical-functional. The idea is that
both an industrial manufacturer and a hot-dog cart operator must assume
all of these roles while initiating their firms, regardless of the scope
or scale of their ventures.
In the entrepreneurial role, business founders examine their
environment and listen to their customers to find new opportunities, and
devise methods to exploit opportunities for the benefit of a new firm
(Mintzberg & Waters 1982). Two competencies are involved here.
First, entrepreneurs must possess the human/conceptual competency to
recognize unique opportunities, and second, they require the drive to
take the venture from conceptualization through to fulfillment
(MacMillan, Siegel, & SubbaNarisimha, 1985; Timmons, Muzyka,
Stevenson, & Bygrave, 1987; Chandler & Jansen, 1992). In the
managerial role, there are also two broad competencies: leadership and
organizational skills (Pavett & Lau 1983; Schein 1987), and the
political competence to procure the support of network members (Pavett
& Lau 1983). In the technical-functional role, business founders
must have some specialized expertise in the industry within which the
firm will operate (Pavett & Lau 1983; Chandler & Jansen, 1992).
In their research, Chandler and Jansen (1992) operationalized each
of these five competencies with multiple items. For our purposes, each
competency had to be simplified and worded as a single cue to fit into
vignettes concerning prospective entrepreneurial decisions. To this end,
we simplified their descriptions into the following five competency
statements:
1. Has strong leadership and organizational skills (LEAD/ORG
SKILLS)
2. Has good sense of what customers want & need (OPP RECOGN)
3. Is willing to make sacrifices to avoid failure (DRIVE)
4. Has specific work-related technical or functional expertise
(EXPERTISE)
5. Has political savvy needed to enlist support of key people
(POLITICAL).
The next section explains how these cues may be related to
entrepreneurial intentions.
Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy (Competencies) and Entrepreneurial
Intentions
Self-efficacy is a construct indicating that behavior, cognition,
and the environment influence each other in a dynamic fashion, thus
allowing individuals to form beliefs about their ability to perform
specific tasks (Bandura, 1977). Entrepreneurial self-efficacy (ESE) is,
therefore, viewed as having the capabilities that can modify a
person's belief in his or her likelihood of completing the tasks
required to successfully initiate and establish a new business venture
(Bandura, 1986). More specifically, entrepreneurial self-efficacy is
defined as the degree to which one believes that he or she is able to
successfully start a new business venture.
Past research can be used to link entrepreneurial self-efficacy and
entrepreneurial intentions. Hackett and Betz (1981) projected that
Bandura's (1977) theory of self-efficacy may be applied to
determine the vocational inclinations of individuals. Empirical findings
indicate that self-efficacy is highly involved in the career
decision-making process. In fact, career self-efficacy was found to be
the most important predictor of males' intentions to pursue careers
in traditionally female occupations (Giles & Rea, 1999). In relation
to entrepreneurship, individuals with high levels of entrepreneurial
self-efficacy may also have strong occupational intentions for an
entrepreneurial career. Lent, Brown, and Hackett (1994) applied
self-efficacy in a social cognitive framework (Bandura, 1986) to explain
three aspects of generalized career development: (1) the formation of
career-relevant interests, (2) selection of a career choice option
(intentions), and (3) performance and persistence in the selected
occupation. Lent, et al (1994) found that self-efficacy was
significantly related to career interests, career choice goals
(intentions), and occupational performance. However, Lent, et al (1994)
also found that self-efficacy is the sole mediator between a
person's abilities and his or her career interests. These three
findings taken together can be interpreted as meaning that self-efficacy
may be used to predict the intended career-related intentions and
behavior of individuals. It has been established that self-efficacy is
the major influence on career-related behavior in Bandura's (1986)
social cognitive theory (Lent, et al, 1994). Since social cognitive
theory proposes that individuals choose to undertake tasks in which they
are confident, comfortable, and perceive competence (Bandura, 1986),
this study hypothesizes that individuals who maintain relatively high
entrepreneurial intentions will place significant weight on their
perception of fitness for entrepreneurial competencies (highly
entrepreneurial self-efficacious). Thus,
Hypothesis 1: Individuals who maintain strong entrepreneurial
intentions will place significant weight on considerations of fitness in
the evaluation of entrepreneurial competencies.
Perceived Relative Value of Competency Components of
Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy
When contemplating which entrepreneurial competency criteria might
be weighed more heavily in the self-perception analysis, it is helpful
to review past research. As explained previously, Chandler and Jansen
(1992) identified and tested five competencies pertaining to three roles
from a sample of entrepreneurs (business founders). These entrepreneurs
placed significance on the competencies that were evaluated in a
distinctive order. Entrepreneurs placed significance on human/conceptual
competence (LEAD/ORG SKILLS), first; ability to recognize opportunity
(OPP RECOGN), second; drive to see the venture through to fruition (DRIVE), third, technical/functional competence (EXPERTISE), fourth, and
political competence (POLITICAL), last. While individuals with strong
entrepreneurial intentions are not actual entrepreneurs, it is likely
that they might value these competencies in a similar order. Intent is a
dependable predictor of human behavior in an assortment of
circumstances, and has been deemed by many to represent the most
successful forecaster of human attitudes and action (Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen
& Fishbein, 1980; Krueger, 1993; Krueger, 2000). Intentions are
assumed to capture the essence of stimulating factors that influence
behavior. They are signals of how intensely individuals are prepared to
perform and how much effort they are prepared to commit to carry out the
expected behavior. Basically, the more robust the intent, the more
probable it is to be able to foretell the anticipated behavior (Ajzen,
1991). Past research (Kim & Hunter, 1993) found that intentions
explained sixty-seven percent of the variance in behavior and path
analysis confirmed that the association between attitudes and behavior
is fully explained by the attitude--intention and intention--behavior
links (Krueger, 2000). It is, therefore, foreseeable that individuals
with strong entrepreneurial intentions will hold similar attitudes to
entrepreneurs when evaluating the relative importance of entrepreneurial
competencies. While it is impossible to forecast, with any confidence,
the exact order of relative competency significance, it is hypothesized
that the most and least valued competencies should be parallel in both
groups. Thus,
Hypothesis 2: Individuals who have strong entrepreneurial
intentions will value human/conceptual competence (LEAD/ORG SKILLS) as
most important.
Hypothesis 3: Individuals who have strong entrepreneurial
intentions will value political competence (POLITICAL) as least
important.
The next section explains how these entrepreneurial self-efficacy
competencies (cues) were used in a decision modeling procedure.
METHODOLOGY
The Decision Modeling Technique
The typical method for conducting entrepreneurial self-efficacy
studies is the self-reported direct survey. However, direct surveys are
susceptible to the effects of social desirability response bias (Fisher,
1993), which is the result of the unfortunate human propensity to
present oneself in the best possible light. Respondents are often
reluctant to respond truthfully to probing questions due to ego
defensive or impression management motivations (Fisher, 1993). This
phenomenon may result in information that is scientifically prejudiced
toward respondent's belief of what is desired by the researcher or
otherwise socially acceptable to others. Social desirability response
bias can lead to misleading research results and may be responsible for
questionable conclusions about individual attitudes, intentions, and
behaviors (Mensch & Kandel, 1988; Chen, Greene, & Crick, 1998).
A valuable remedy utilized by researchers to alleviate the
consequences of social desirability response bias is by the application
of an indirect questioning methodology. Indirect questioning is a
projective technique that requires individuals to respond to questions
that are presented from the viewpoint of another person or group
(Anderson, 1978). By employing this method, respondents are allowed to
project their unconscious biases in varying situations and disclose
personal attitudes and beliefs (Campbell, 1950). Thus, indirect
questioning allows respondents to express their true feelings under the
pretense of role-playing.
This study used the indirect questioning method of decision
modeling (Slovic & Lichtenstein, 1971) to examine individual
assessment of human competency criteria for entrepreneurial
self-efficacy. This method has also been identified under the more
general terminology "conjoint analysis" (Shepherd &
Zacharakis, 1999). While this technique has been frequently used to
examine individual differences in decision processes (Klaas &
Wheeler, 1990; Spencer & Crosby, 1997; Powell & Mainiero, 1999),
venture capitalist assessments (Shepherd & Zacharackis, 1999,
Shepherd & Zacharackis, 2002, Zacharackis & Meyer, 2000), and
consumer purchase decisions (Lang & Crown, 1993), it has never been
utilized in studies of entrepreneurial intentions or entrepreneurial
self-efficacy. Further, no entrepreneurial self-efficacy studies have
scrutinized individual assessment of entrepreneurial self-efficacy
variables; rather, they have examined the assessments made by groups of
individuals whose perceptions are averaged together across the factors
being studied.
In decision modeling methods, researchers methodically vary the cue
content on a series of conditions to produce a large number of cue
combinations. Each participant makes judgments about a sufficient
quantity of these conditions to permit individualized (within-subject)
regression analyses. The regression weights of the cues in the
conditions create a decision policy for each individual, which can be
interpreted to determine which cues hold the most importance for a
particular set of decisions. In the present study, each respondent will
be examined for his or her application of targeted human competency
criteria in the assessment of entrepreneurial self-efficacy. Then, these
cue condition assessments will be examined for their relation to
individual's entrepreneurial intent.
Sample
In entrepreneurial decision research, it is important to uncover
occupational intentions at a time when respondents are wrestling with
important career decisions (Krueger, Reilly, & Carsrud, 2000). The
sampling of only successful, current, or openly prospective
entrepreneurs introduces prejudices that influence data erratically,
especially in the case of highly ambiguous, and uncommon, phenomena
(Krueger, Reilly, & Carsrud, 2000). For this research, a general
sample of upper-class college students was utilized because while the
exact details of a new venture may have not been fully developed in the
minds of most of those with an entrepreneurial interest, global career
intentions and evaluations of efficacy should have been (Scherer, Adams,
Carley, & Weibe, 1989). Therefore, it is fitting to investigate
entrepreneurial intent and self-efficacy utilizing a sample of
upper-class college students. In keeping with this instruction, the
study participants in this research were 140 volunteer graduating
undergraduate university business students from a large southeastern
university.
The students represented a diverse variety of business disciplines
and were all enrolled in he university's capstone business
policy/strategy course as was required immediately before graduation.
The researcher visited each business policy/strategy class midway into
the semester of the study (after prior approval of each course
instructor) and made a brief appeal seeking study volunteers. Those who
agreed to participate were then given the survey in its entirety.
Instructions on the survey stressed the seriousness of academic research
and beseeched each participant to analyze each scenario and answer each
question as truthfully as possible.
Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy Decision Cues
In this research, participants were asked to make judgments about
the likelihood of hypothetical prospective entrepreneurs being able to
establish a new business venture based, solely, on vignettes which
highlighted differing combinations of the five decision-making cues
described above. Each of the five cues had two possible values, no or
yes (coded 0, 1) resulting in 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 = 32 vignettes
representing all combinations of decision cues. After consideration of
the cue combination profile presented in each vignette, the participants
recorded their opinion whether, or not, a fictional character that
possessed those particular cue attributes would be able to establish a
new business venture. Each decision was recorded as very unlikely to
very likely on a seven-point Likert continuum. Figure 1 shows a sample
decision outcome scenario.
Entrepreneurial Intent
The entrepreneurial intent of each student was measured on a
five-point Likert scale, which was adapted from an entrepreneurial
decision scale (alpha = 0.92) in Chen, Greene, and Crick (1998).
Principal components factor analysis with a varimax rotation revealed
that all five of the entrepreneurial intent items in the scale loaded on
only one factor, which demonstrates the unidimensionality of the
construct. The internal consistency reliability of the scale was
assessed with Cronbach's alpha, which was registered at 0.89. This
measure is indicative of high scale internal consistency reliability
(Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Entrepreneurial intent scores were
calculated by averaging the five items for each respondent. Low
entrepreneurial intent was characterized by those respondents
registering a mean of 1.0--2.4, with high entrepreneurial intent
corresponding to means of 3.6-5.0. Figure 2 shows the entrepreneurial
intent scale.
Demographic Variables
Each participant provided information on their gender (coded
female=0, male=1), race (coded Black=0, White=1, Asian-Pacific
Islander=2), and age.
Analysis
The analysis of the data in a decision modeling study is a
within-subjects analysis. The decision modeling approach
"models" in a mathematical computation, such as a regression
equation, the process a subject uses to bring together information to
make a judgment (Zedeck, 1977). The regression equation illustrates the
decision maker's policies for integrating and evaluating
information (Zedeck, 1977).
For each of the 132 participants who completed the survey, their
decision solution in each vignette was regressed on each of the five
cues using ordinary least squares regression within a general linear
model. This analysis is different from the usual treatment of
regression, in which one regression equation is determined to represent
an entire sample. In decision modeling, the focal point is exclusive to
one individual, and analyses are performed to discriminate the decision
policy of that one individual instead of a general equation for a sample
of subjects. This within-subjects treatment of regression serves to
diminish the measurement error attributed to individual differences
(Stahl & Harrell, 1981). Therefore, one equation was estimated for
each subject (132 regression estimates).
For the regressions, the participant's decision choices in the
32 vignettes was the dependent variable and the decision cues, the
independent variables, were coded and randomized for each vignette by
the researchers. The regressions were processed in batches of 10
utilizing the multivariate general linear model function in SPSS 11.0.
Raw beta coefficients were examined to assess the importance of each
independent variable in explaining each participant's decisions.
Raw regression coefficients are appropriate to interpret in decision
modeling since they do not change significantly as a function of
decision cue structure, unlike semipartial correlations and standardized
regression weights (Lane, Murphy, & Marques, 1982). These
coefficients revealed the participants decision policy, with the most
important cues to each participant's group of decisions registering
a significant effect on decision outcome (p < 0.05). The sample size
for each participant in regression analyses was the number of decisions
made (n = 32). Since each participant made 32 decisions in the
experiment, 4,244 decisions were analyzed in this study.
Before analyzing the decision policy of participants, the
reliability of their decisions must be assessed (Stahl & Harrell,
1981). The multiple correlation coefficient squared (R-squared) from
each participant's regression equation is a measure of the
reliability of the decision that was made. These values represent the
consistency with which each participant made decisions. Nonsignificant multiple correlation coefficients represent that the participants were
making random decisions, as opposed to following logical decision rules,
and should be dismissed from further analyses (Butler & Cantrell,
1984).
To test the relative effect of decision cues between participants,
mean beta values for each decision cue were calculated for the entire
sample. An F-test was performed to test the equality of means between
each of the five decision cues. The test for equality of the mean values
for each quality cue was written as Ho: n1 = n2 = n3 = n4 = n5 where n1
is the mean raw beta weight derived for decision cue 1, n2 is the mean
raw beta weight derived for decision cue 2, and so on. If the test
hypothesis was rejected (p < .05), paired comparisons were performed
on each combination of decision cues utilizing paired-sample t tests.
The paired-sample t test procedure compares the means of two decision
variables within a single group. It computes the differences between
values of the two variables for each case and tests whether the average
differs from zero. From the significance indices of each combination, we
were able to distinguished which decision cues were the most, and least,
significant for each group of respondents.
RESULTS
As noted earlier, the sample initially included 140 soon to be
graduating business students. Eight of these were excluded from the
analysis because they failed to complete the research questionnaire. Of
the remaining 132 respondents, 58% were male and 42% female, 83%
Caucasian, 15% African-American, and 2% Asian-Pacific Islander. The mean
age for the sample was 22.6 yrs (median = 22) with the range extending
from 20 to 32 years of age.
Each participant's decision policy regarding the likelihood of
an individual establishing a new business venture was determined with
ordinary least squares regression within a general linear model. The
first step in analyzing these decision policies was to examine the
reliability of each individual's regression equation (Stahl &
Harrell, 1991). Of the 132 regressions, 14 were deemed nonsignificant
after reviewing their squared multiple correlations (p > .05). Since
a nonsignificant value indicates that the subject was recording random
decisions, these 14 subjects were xcluded from the study leaving a
between-subjects sample of 118.
General Research Question 1 asked how important each of the
entrepreneurial efficacy decision cues would be to a sample of
individuals assessing the likelihood of someone being able to establish
a new business venture. The decision modeling technique produced raw
beta coefficients attributable to each human competency decision cue for
each participant. The raw beta coefficients were then averaged for each
decision cue, revealing an overall decision policy for the sample. An
F-test and paired-samples t test analysis revealed a significant
difference between the mean regression weights placed on the five human
competency decision cues (F = 5.16; p = 0.0004). Specifically, having a
good sense of customer wants and needs (mean = 1.237) and
leadership/organizational skills (mean = 1.228) had the most influence
on decisions about entrepreneurial efficacy. Willingness to make
sacrifices was second (mean = 1.098); and technical/functional expertise
(mean = 1.047) and political savvy (mean = 0.999) were judged lowest in
importance, respectively.
General Research Question 2 asked whether individuals with strong
intentions to begin a business would utilize these human competency
decision cues differently from others when judging entrepreneurial
capability. From this general research question, three study hypotheses
were generated. Hypothesis 1 predicted that individuals with strong
entrepreneurial intentions would more seriously evaluate their fitness
for each entrepreneurial competency relative to others. Hypotheses 2 and
3 anticipated that individuals with strong entrepreneurial intentions
would mirror actual entrepreneur's judgment of the most and least
valued entrepreneurial competencies. In order to evaluate these three
hypotheses, sample decision policies were re-analyzed after the
assignment of participants to three groupings based on the magnitude of
their entrepreneurial intentions.
Results across these three subgroups strongly support the three
study hypotheses. As forecasted in Hypothesis 1, only those individuals
with high entrepreneurial intentions (n = 35) placed significantly
different weights on the five competencies when making judgments about
entrepreneurial efficacy (F = 5.43; p = .0004). In contrast, the mean
beta values for the five decision cues did not vary significantly among
those with low (n = 46; F = 0.468; p = 0.758) or neutral (n = 37; F =
1.71; p = 0.151) entrepreneurial intentions. Apparently, these
respondents did not bother to differentiate among the cues as much as
did those individuals with a strong desire and interest to start a
business of their own.
As shown in Figure 3, the paired t test analysis demonstrated that
individuals who have strong entrepreneurial intentions judged
leadership/organizational skills and having a good sense of customer
needs the most important indicators of entrepreneurial efficacy;
technical/functional competency and the willingness to make sacrifices
was second, and political competency was the least important. These
results support hypotheses 2 and 3.
The result of the grouping analysis demonstrates that individuals
with high entrepreneurial intentions can be discriminated from other
individuals and other groups based on their application of human
competency decision criteria. Respondents with low or moderate interest
in entrepreneurship weighed all of the capabilities about equally when
making judgments about entrepreneurial efficacy. However, those with
high entrepreneurial intentions weighed some capabilities significantly
more than others. We interpret this finding to mean that these criteria
are particularly relevant to individuals possessing serious
entrepreneurial intentions.
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to examine the criteria used by
participants, who possess different levels of entrepreneurial intent,
when assessing their ability to establish a new venture. By learning how
diverse people evaluate specific human competencies related to
entrepreneurship, we hope to learn more about the factors influencing
personal self-efficacy evaluations. Instead of straightforwardly asking
respondents about their own entrepreneurial self-efficacy, we used a
projective technique to capture their unconscious biases and personal
attitudes about the construct (Campbell, 1950).
To create our research instrument, we manipulated factors from
Chandler & Jansen's (1992) entrepreneurial competency study.
These factors included leadership and organizational skills, knowledge
about what customers want and need, the willingness to make personal
sacrifices to avoid failure, specific technical/functional expertise,
and the political savvy to enlist support of key stakeholders. An
attempt was then made, utilizing decision-modeling methodology, to
determine which of these criteria are most relevant to the
entrepreneurial self-efficacy assessments of individuals with strong
entrepreneurial intentions. In addition, we compared the decision
policies of respondents with varying levels of entrepreneurial intent to
determine if these criteria were applied differently in their judgments
of overall entrepreneurial efficacy.
It was found that knowledge of customer wants and needs and
whether, or not, one possesses strong leadership and organizational
skills were judged as most important in the assessment of
entrepreneurial self-efficacy by the overall sample. These
considerations were followed in importance by the willingness to make
sacrifices to avoid failure, work-related technical/functional
expertise, and political savvy, respectively. Certainly, these are
critical attributes that all individuals should consider (Chandler &
Jansen, 1992) when judging whether, or not, to attempt entrepreneurial
activity. Therefore, self-efficacy research instruments that include
these criteria should be useful to discern prospective entrepreneurs
from others. In this respect, the experimental manipulation of human
competency decision cues was successful.
When comparing the decision policies of groups in the sample that
were separated by their level of entrepreneurial intent, it was
demonstrated that only the group with strong entrepreneurial intent
placed significance on any of the entrepreneurial competency decision
cues. Individuals who professed strong entrepreneurial intentions valued
strong leadership and organizational skills the most. This was followed
by knowledge of customer wants and needs, work-related
technical/functional expertise, willingness to make sacrifices to avoid
failure; and political savvy, respectively. It is obvious from these
results that the significance displayed by our overall sample emanated
from the high intent subgroup. Therefore, it can be concluded that the
decision cues in this study were able to differentiate those individuals
who had strong entrepreneurial intentions from others. These preliminary
findings should provide encouragement for future research to utilize
human competency components of entrepreneurial self-efficacy.
Accordingly, the decision policy for those with strong
entrepreneurial intentions exhibited in this study closely matches the
factor magnitude pattern of actual entrepreneurs found in Chandler and
Jansen (1992). In their study, the investigators surveyed actual
entrepreneurs and had them divulge which human competencies they felt
were most important to successfully initiate a new business venture. Our
study performed a similar analysis, except couched in terms of
self-efficacy assessment for prospective entrepreneurs. Previous
attempts to correlate prospective and actual entrepreneur's
self-efficacy component significance haven't fared as well. For
instance, Chen, Greene, and Crick (1998), developed an entrepreneurial
self-efficacy scale that incorporated five expected entrepreneurial
roles and tasks (marketing, innovation, management, risk-taking, and
financial control) that should have been relevant to both the
prospective and actual entrepreneurs in their study. However, the
prospective entrepreneurs (entrepreneurship students) registered
significantly higher efficacy scores for marketing, management, and
financial control while the actual entrepreneurs (business founders)
were highly efficacious for innovation and risk-taking. In short, the
researchers found no common factors that were important to the full
spectrum of entrepreneurs under investigation. Our examination is an
attempt to apply entrepreneurial self-efficacy components that are
relevant to all types and levels of the entrepreneur continuum.
There are several implications of entrepreneurial self-efficacy
that merits further emphasis. First, self-efficacy is a wide-ranging
evaluation of perceived fitness for the performance of a specific
activity (Bandura & Wood, 1989; Wood & Bandura, 1989; Gist &
Mitchell, 1992). In a real-world entrepreneurship context, information
derived exclusively from the individual (cognitions), the particular
venture creation and development task (behavior), and the network of
supporting individuals and organizations involved in a specific
entrepreneurial effort (environment) may possibly add to estimated
capability judgments on the part of the prospective entrepreneur.
However, in the context of global entrepreneurial self-efficacy
research, it may be more useful to examine self-efficacy for general
entrepreneurial tasks (such as nonspecific new venture initiation)
through the mechanism of universal assessment criteria (e.g. human
competencies) instead of relying on functional criteria that is too
specific to be applied to all forms of planned venture initiations.
Since we know that entrepreneurship is a planned phenomenon (Bird, 1988,
Katz & Gartner, 1988), it only makes sense that we examine
self-efficacy perceptions with universally appealing assessments unless
we regulate ourselves to research questions designed for specific types
of planned ventures (e.g., self-efficacy of initiating an industrial
equipment manufacturing operation). Second, self-efficacy may be labeled
as an evolving phenomenon since the efficacy estimation may be modified
over time as new information and know-how are obtained (Bandura &
Wood, 1989; Wood & Bandura, 1989; Gist & Mitchell, 1992). In the
context of entrepreneurship, it is possible that individual perception
of one's suitability to be an entrepreneur changes periodically as
one transforms from a nascent entrepreneur with a high-quality new
business concept to a veteran entrepreneur who has intimate familiarity
with the hardships and successes of a series of new venture start-ups,
failures, growths, and harvests. We can only test this proposed
phenomenon longitudinally, however, if we apply research questions that
revolve around recurrent considerations, such as human competencies,
instead of venture-specific discrete functions. Last, self-efficacy
research is vulnerable to the social desirability bias of the sample
(Chen, Greene, & Crick, 1998). In order to alleviate this threat to
the validity of entrepreneurial self-efficacy research, this study
applied a decision modeling indirect questioning methodology that is
resistant to social desirability biases (Fischer, 1993). Future
self-efficacy research should apply other indirect questioning
methodologies to protect the viability of research results and
conclusions.
The present research is limited by a number of issues. The first
limitation pertains to the decision modeling methodology used herein.
Although the self-efficacy decision cues were derived from Chandler and
Jansen's (1992) entrepreneurial competency analysis, the decision
cues are not an identical match for the factors that the researchers
developed. We summarized each dimension of their multidimensional scale
into a one-sentence description of the major emphasis of the factor. In
doing so, we transformed each multi-item factor into a single item
measure. There is the possibility that we misrepresented the factors by
being either too vague or too specific. Second, the importance of each
cue may vary depending on the experience of the rater. For example, the
political savvy decision cue was consistently the least important factor
for all of the groups studied. However, the ability to garner support
and gain consensus among a network of key supporters is vital to new
venture success (Chandler & Jansen, 1992). It is apparent from this
study that not all individuals with strong entrepreneurial intentions
know exactly what is necessary to establish a new business. This is
indicative of potential entrepreneurs who operate with little
information of possible obstacles (Krueger & Brazeal, 1994) and may
be a primary cause for the high failure rate of entrepreneurial
start-ups. The caveat here, though, is that our strong entrepreneurial
intent subgroup mirrored the perspective of actual business founders.
Last, the exclusive use of students may limit the generalizability of
our results.
In conclusion, entrepreneurship researchers have developed
entrepreneurial self-efficacy measures mainly along narrowly applicable
functional assessments utilizing self-reported direct surveys. Decision
modeling approaches can be used to rank decision criteria that underlie
universal entrepreneurial competencies. Using such an approach, this
study has made an initial attempt to validate human competency criteria
used in making judgments about entrepreneurial self-efficacy. Further
research is warranted to examine these criteria by applying indirect
questioning methodologies on other samples drawn from the broad-spectrum
of entrepreneurs.
REFERENCES
Anderson, J.C. (1978). The validity of haire's shopping list
projective technique. Journal of Marketing Research, 8: 644-649.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50: 179-211.
Ajzen, I. & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and
predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs: NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84: 91-215.
Bandura, A. (1982). The self and mechanisms of agency. In J. Suls
(Ed.), Psychological Perspectives on the Self (pp. 3-39). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. & Wood, R.E. (1989). Effect of perceived
controllability and performance standards on self- regulation of complex
decision-making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56:
805-814.
Begley, T. & Boyd, D. (1987). A comparison of entrepreneurs and
managers of small business firms. Journal of Management, 13(1): 99-109.
Bird, B.J.(1988). Implementing entrepreneurial ideas: The case for
intention. The Academy of Management Review, 13(3): 442-454.
Boyd, N. & Vozikis, G. (1994). The Influence of self-efficacy
on the development of entrepreneurial intentions and actions.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 18(4): 63-78.
Brockhaus, R. & Horowitz, P. (1986). The psychology of the
entrepreneur. In D. L. Sexton and R. W. Smilor (Eds.), The Art and
Science of Entrepreneurship. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger (25-48).
Butler, J.K. & Cantrell, R.S. (1984). A behavioral decision
theory approach to modeling dyadic trust in superiors and subordinates.
Psychological Reports, 55: 19-28.
Campbell, D.T. (1950). The indirect assessment of social attitudes.
Psychological Bulletin, 47: 15-38.
Chandler, G.N. & Jansen, E. (1992). The founder's
self-assessed competence and venture performance. Journal of Business
Venturing, 7(3): 223-237.
Chen, C.C., Greene, P. & Crick, A. (1998). Does entrepreneurial
self-efficacy distinguish entrepreneurs from managers? Journal of
Business Venturing, 13(4): 295-317.
Giles, M. & Rea, A. (1999). Career self-efficacy: An
application of the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology, 72: 393-399.
Fisher, R.J. (1993). Social desirability and the validity of
indirect questioning. Journal of Consumer Research, 20: 303-315.
Gist, M.E. (1987). Self-efficacy: Implications for organizational
behavior and human resource management. Academy of Management Review, 3:
472-485.
Gist, M.E. & Mitchell, T.R. (1992). Self-efficacy: A
theoretical analysis of its determinants and malleability. Academy of
Management Review, 17: 183-211.
Gist, M.E., Stevens, C.K. & Bavetta, A.G. (1991). Effects of
self-efficacy and post-training intervention on the acquisition and
maintenance of complex interpersonal skill. Personnel Psychology, 44:
837-861.
Hackett, G. & Betz, N.E. (1981). A self-efficacy approach to
the career development of women. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 18:
326-336.
Katz, J.A. & Gartner, W.B. (1988). Properties of emerging
organizations. Academy of Management Review, 13: 429-441.
Kim, M.S. & Hunter, J. (1993). Relationships among attitude,
behavioral intentions, and behavior. Communication Research, 20:
331-364.
Klaas, B.S. & Wheeler, H. N. (1990). Managerial decision-making
about employee discipline: A policy capturing approach. Personnel
Psychology, 43: 117-134.
Krueger, N.F. Jr. (1993). The impact of prior entrepreneurial
exposure on perceptions of new venture feasibility and desirability.
Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 18(1): 5-21.
Krueger, N.F. Jr. (2000). The cognitive infrastructure of
opportunity emergence. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 24(3):
5-23.
Krueger, N.F. Jr. & Brazeal, D.V. (1994). Entrepreneurial
potential and potential entrepreneurs. Entrepreneurship Theory and
Practice, Spring: 91-104.
Krueger, N. Jr., Reilly, M.D. & Carsrud, A.L. (2000). Competing
models of entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of Business Venturing,
15(5): 411-432.
Lane, D.M., Murphy, K.R. & Marques, T.E. (1982). Measuring the
importance of cues in policy capturing. Organizational Behavior and
Human Performance, 30: 231-240.
Lang, J.Q. & Crown, E.M., (1993). Country-of-origin effect in
apparel choices: a conjoint analysis. Journal f Consumer Studies and
Home Economics. 17 (March): 87-98.
Lent, R.W., Brown, S.D. & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward a unifying
social cognitive theory of careerand academic interest, choice, and
performance. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 45: 79-122.
Livesay, H. C. (1982). Entrepreneurial History. In Kent, Sexton,
and Vesper's Encyclopedia of Entrepreneurship (pp. 7-15). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc.
MacMillan, I.C., Siegel, R. & SubbaNarismha, P.N. (1985).
Criteria used by venture capitalists to evaluate new venture proposals.
Journal of Business Venturing, 1(1):119-128.
McClelland, D. (1965). N achievement and entrepreneurship: A
longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1:
389-392.
Mensch, B.S. & Kandel, D. B. (1988). Underreporting of
substance use in national longitudinal youth cohort. Public Opinion
Quarterly, 52, 100-124.
Mintzberg, H. & Waters, J.A. (1982). Tracking strategy in an
entrepreneurial firm. Academy of Management Journal, 25(3): 465-500.
Mokry, B.W. (1988). Entrepreneurship and public policy. New York:
Quorum Books.
Nunnally, J.C. & Bernstein, LH. (1994). Psychometric Theory,
Third Edition. New York: McGraw.
Pavett, C.M. & Lau, A.W. (1983). Managerial work: The influence
of hierarchical level and functional specialty. Academy of Management
Journal, 26(1): 170-178.
Powell, G. N. & Mainiero, L. A. (1999). Managerial decision
making regarding alternative work arrangements. Journal of Occupational
and Organizational Psychology, 72: 41-56.
Schein, E.H. (1987). Individuals and careers. In J. Lorsch's
(ed), Handbook of Organizational Behavior (pp. 151-171). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Schere, J. (1982). Tolerance of ambiguity as a discriminating variable between entrepreneurs and managers. Proceedings, (pp. 404-408).
New York: Academy of Management.
Scherer, R.F., Adams, J.S., Carley, S.S. & Wiebe, F.A. (1989).
Role model performance effects on the development of entrepreneurial
career preference. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 13(3): 53-71.
Shapero, A. & Sokol, L. (1982). The social dimensions of
entrepreneurship. In Kent, Sexton, and Vesper's Encyclopedia of
Entrepreneurship (pp. 72-90). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc.
Shepherd, D.A. & Zacharakis, A.L.(1999). Conjoint analysis: A
new methodological approach for researching the decision policies of
venture capitalists. Venture Capital, 1(3): 197-217.
Shepherd, D.A. & Zacharakis, A.L. (2002). Venture
capitalists' expertise: A call for research into decision aids and
cognitive feedback. Journal of Business Venturing, 17(1): 1-20.
Slovic, P. & Lichtenstein, S. (1971). Comparison of Bayesian
and regression approaches to the study of information processing in
judgment. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6: 649-744.
S pencer, B. A. & Crosby, L. (1997). Linking quality attributes
with customer purchasing decisions: Acomparison of two methods. Quality
Management Journal, 5(1): pp. 35-45.
Stahl, M.J. & Harrell, A.M. (1981). Modeling effort decisions
with behavioral decision theory: Toward an individual differences model
of expectancy theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 27:
303-325.
Timmons, J.A., Muzyka, D.F., Stevenson, H.H. & Bygrave, W.D.
(1987). Opportunity recognition: the core of entrepreneurship. In N.C.
Churchill, J.A. Hornaday, B.A. Kirchoff, O.J. Krasner, & K.H. Vesper
(eds) Frontiers of Entrepreneurship Research (pp. 109-123). Wellesey,
MA: Babson Center for Entrepreneurial Studies.
Wood, R. & Bandura, A. (1989). Social cognitive theory of
organizational management. Academy of Management Review, 14: 361-384.
Zacharakis, A.L. & Meyer, G.D. (2000). The potential of
actuarial decision models: Can they improve the venture capital
investment decision? Journal of Business Venturing, 15(4): 323-346.
Zedeck, S. (1977). An information processing model and approach to
the study of motivation. Organization Behavior and Human Performance,
18: 47-77.
Jeff Brice, Jr., Texas Southern University
Barbara Spencer, Mississippi State University
FIGURE 1: SAMPLE DECISION MODELING SCENARIO
DECISION MODELING EXERCISE
Directions: In each scenario, please indicate how likely it is that
someone will be able to start a promising business.
Circle (1) if Very unlikely
Circle (2) if Unlikely
Circle (3) if Somewhat unlikely
Circle (4) if Neither likely nor unlikely
Circle (5) if Somewhat likely
Circle (6) if Likely
Circle (7) if Very likely
Person A:
--has strong leadership and organizational skills... No
--has a good sense of what customers want and need... Yes
--is willing to make personal sacrifices to avoid failure... Yes
--has specific work-related technical or functional expertise.. No
--has political savvy needed to enlist support of key people.. No
1) With these factors in mind, how likely is it that A will be able to
establish a new business venture?
Very 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very
Unlikely Likely
FIGURE 2: ENTREPRENEURIAL INTENTIONS SCALE *
Directions: Please circle the appropriate number based on your response
to the questions below.
1) I am interested in setting up my own business.
Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly
disagree agree
2) I have considered setting up my own business.
Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly
disagree agree
3) I am prepared to set up my own business.
Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly
disagree agree
4) I am going to try hard to set up my own business.
Strongly 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly
disagree agree
5) How soon are you likely to set up your own business (select the
response that most closely matches your plans).
1 2 3 4 5
Never after within within within
10+ yrs 6-10 yrs 1-5 yrs 1yr
* Cronbach's alpha = 0.89
FIGURE 3: RESULTS OF DECISION MODELING ANALYSIS:
HIGH ENTREPRENEURIAL INTENTIONS SUBGROUP (DECISION POLICY MAP)
Decision Cue Profile: L/O OP TF DR PO
Means: 1.32 1.25 1.03 1.04 0.86
Decision Cue Groupings *: [L/O OP]
[OP TF DR]
[DR PO]
Most Important Least Important
Entrepreneurial Self-Efficacy Decision Cue Key:
L/O = Leadership/Organizational: Strong leadership and organizational
skills.
OP = Opportunity Recognition: A good sense of what customers want and
need.
TF = Technical/Functional: Specific work-related technical or
functional expertise.
DR = Drive: Willing to make personal sacrifices to avoid failure.
PO = Political: Political savvy needed to enlist support of key people.
* The means of decision criteria grouped within the same brackets are
not significantly different.