Can personality dimensions influence entrepreneurial occupation preference? An exploratory study of dispositional influences on cognitive processes.
Brice, Jeff, Jr.
ABSTRACT
This study seeks to discern if there is a significant dispositional
foundation for occupational preferences. Specifically, this paper seeks
to determine if personality dimensions have any effect on an
individual's cognitive expectancies (concerning perceived intrinsic
and extrinsic occupational rewards) when considering an entrepreneurial
career. Personality dimensions composing the Five-Factor Model of
Personality are applied in this study and include Conscientious,
Agreeableness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness to Experience.
Conscientiousness determines responsibility versus inconsistency,
Agreeableness measures sociability versus detachment, Extraversion
determines assertiveness versus timidity, Neuroticism measures
self-assurance versus insecurity, and Openness to Experience involves
uniformity versus self-determination. Each of these dimensions is
related to occupational preference for an entrepreneurial career and
examined utilizing the Valence Model of the Expectancy Theory. The model
consists of two variables, Instrumentality and Valence. Instrumentality
(I) concerns the belief that the attainment of work-related goals will
lead to rewards; and, Valence (V) refers to the value of those rewards
to the individual.
Results indicate that individuals who are highly Conscientious are
significantly attracted to an entrepreneurial career due to the
intrinsic rewards of independence and a satisfying way of life. Further,
individuals who are highly Open also prefer an entrepreneurial career
due to the perceived satisfying lifestyle.
INTRODUCTION
A key question in the study of entrepreneurship is what factors
increase the likelihood that an individual will decide to pursue an
entrepreneurial career given a multitude of more traditional
alternatives. Even though entrepreneurship has been recognized as a
complex, multidimensional construct that has avoided stable definition
(Palich & Bagby, 1995), interest in entrepreneurship education has
increased dramatically around the globe. The many new hordes of
entrepreneurial-minded students seem to choose entrepreneurship as a
major area of focus due to their perceptions about an entrepreneurial
career and their estimated fitness for the craft of entrepreneurship.
This study seeks to determine if there might be a dispositional basis
(in the form of personality dimensions) to cognitions that form the
preference for an entrepreneurial career (based on expected work
rewards) and serves to influence prospective entrepreneurs.
Specifically, the purpose of this paper is to try to determine if
there is a significant relationship between any of the personality
dimensions in the Five-Factor Model of Personality and an
individual's preference for an entrepreneurial career based on
their cognitive estimation of perceived intrinsic and extrinsic rewards
of the occupation. First, dispositional research in entrepreneurship is
reviewed. Second, the Five-Factor Model of Personality is described.
Next, cognitive process literature in entrepreneurship is reviewed and
the expectancy theory and its possible relation to entrepreneurial
career preferences is presented. Then, hypotheses are developed and the
research methodology is described. Last, results are discussed and
conclusions are elaborated.
LITERATURE REVIEW
This section addresses a number of the major studies contributing
to the entrepreneur literature on personality traits and cognitive
processes. Specifically, it will detail how the Five-Factor Model of
Personality and expectancy cognitions may result in significant
relations to entrepreneurial occupational preferences. To be succinct,
it scrutinizes those topics that will be key variables in this study.
Personality Dimensions and Entrepreneurship
Personality traits have routinely been studied as possible
differentiators of entrepreneurs from other individuals. The most common
include a high need for achievement (McClelland, 1961), internal locus
of control (Brockhaus & Nord, 1979), and risk taking propensity
(Brockhaus, 1980; Sexton & Bowman, 1985). Unfortunately, the
inconsistent results of trait-oriented research did not lead us to an
authoritative conclusion of what encourages individuals to initiate
entrepreneurial behavior (Shaver & Scott, 1991; Ripsas, 1998).
However, the psychology literature has identified over 18,000 individual
personality traits that can be used to explain human expression
(Cattell, 1947). Over time this massive list has been compiled and
reduced through the use of assumptions, factor analysis, and cluster
analysis to as few as three higher-order personality dimensions (Buss
& Finn, 1987). Personality dimensions represent distinct groupings
of individual personality traits that network with each other and are
expressed by repetitive patterns of human behavior. This study applies
the Five-Factor Model of Personality (Fiske, 1949) as a basis for
examination because it is the most broadly endorsed model in personality
research (Barrick & Mount, 1991).
The Five-Factor Model of Personality
The Five-Factor Model of Personality (commonly called the Big Five)
is a descriptive representation (typology) of the five major
dispositional dimensions that encompass human personality. It has been
used extensively in industrial psychology as a basis to measure
job-related attitudes, person-organization fit, and other human resource
inquiries. The personality factors comprising the Big Five are (1)
Extraversion, which represents the inclination to be sociable,
assertive, dynamic, and directive, (2) Agreeableness, representing the
tendency to be friendly, cheerful, accommodating, and supportive, (3)
Conscientiousness, comprised of two major subfactors, achievement and
dependability, (4) Neuroticism, (also called Emotional Stability) which
is the tendency to exhibit poor emotional adjustment and experience
disparaging effects such as fear, anxiety, and rashness, and (5)
Openness to Experience, which is the propensity to be inquisitive,
creative, nonconforming and independent (Judge & Cable, 1997). Each
dimension is scaled from high to low with high scores being
representative of the most positive aspects of the dimension's
characterization while low scores signify the reverse.
The origin of personality typing can be traced back to Galen's
observations of Greek society in the Second Century A.D.. He framed his
personality type designations on what was termed the four humors which
were described as (1) Sanguine types (cheerful and upbeat), (2) Choleric types (hot-tempered and dramatic), (3) Melancholic types (fretful and
worrying), and (4) Phlegmatic types (stolid and unflappable)(Hogan,
1990). While Galen's metaphors were advanced to suggest that
personality types were stable and identifiable within ancient Greek culture, modern research on personality dispositions further generalizes
this application to include individuals in all cultures. Staw & Ross
(1985) and Staw, Bell, & Clausen (1986) performed several landmark
longitudinal experiments and found that stable individual personality
disposition is formed by the age of adolescence and, thereby, makes it
possible to predict future behavior and attitudes in spite of key
situational changes. More concisely, Staw, Bell, & Clausen (1986)
showed that dispositional measures of personality significantly and
reliably predicted occupational attitudes and employment behavior over a
span of fifty years. The pattern of dispositional decay indicated that
adolescent-stage dispositions set in motion a consistent set of
behaviors and choices that later produced important consequences for the
individual. Thus, it has been demonstrated that personality dimensions
(traits) may be stable across time and this evidence should allow us to
apply the Five-Factor Model of Personality to identify enduring
occupational preferences for groups of individuals within the populous.
Cognitive Process Approach
Due to the inconclusive results in the individual personality
trait-oriented research, scholars turned to studying the potential role
of cognitive factors in stimulating entrepreneurship (Baron, 1998). The
basic premise is that entrepreneurs differ from others based on how they
think and that these differences can be modeled empirically (Das &
Teng, 1997). In studying entrepreneurship, those taking this approach
attempt to understand how perceptions (Cooper, Woo, & Dunkleberg,
1988), cognitive and decision-making styles (Kaish & Gilad, 1991),
heuristics (Manimala, 1992), biases (Busenitz & Barney, 1997), and
intentions (Bird, 1988) of entrepreneurs affect their behavior. Indeed,
Shaver & Scott (1991) assert that any psychological approach to
entrepreneurship must include the cognitive processes that occur within
the individual entrepreneur.
The cognitive process approach allows researchers to gain insight
into such inquiries as "How do entrepreneurs recognize
opportunities in the market" and "What are the cognitive
mechanisms that motivate entrepreneurs to pursue market
opportunities?" (Baron, 1998). While both the personality-oriented
and cognitive process approaches attempt to reveal something important
about individual entrepreneurs, the cognitive process approach is
currently much more popular in academic research (Das & Teng, 1997;
Stewart, Watson, Carland, & Carland, 1998).
Contribution of the Cognitive Process View
The presence of appropriate personality dimensions that render an
individual intrinsically suited for venturing does not guarantee
entrepreneurial behavior. Kirzner (1973) stressed that entrepreneurs are
not only those that discover market opportunities, but also that they
must act upon these prospects whenever possible. Accordingly, the
purpose of the cognitive process view is to explain the mechanism of
consideration that results in such action.
It can be reasoned that a defining factor for entrepreneurs is the
desire to pursue opportunities once they are exposed. In this context,
only those individuals who are motivated enough to pursue
entrepreneurial careers, in deference to other possible choices (e.g.,
traditional employment), can be considered entrepreneurs. The problem,
however, is that there exists no consistent explanation of the mechanism
of motivation for the exploitation of these tenuous opportunities
(Ripsas, 1998). Therefore, this study advances the idea that a possible
contributing explanation for entrepreneurial motivation is the
occupational preference for an entrepreneurial career as specified by
the valence model component of the expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964).
Expectancy Theory and Occupational Preference
According to the expectancy theory of motivation, individuals are
rational, they understand the possible consequences of their actions,
and make selections among options based on a merger of the value of the
outcomes and the probability that the outcomes will be achieved
(Gatewood, 1993). It is proposed, in this study, that the cognitive
process of forming occupational preferences outlined by the valence
model of Vroom's (1966) expectancy theory of motivation most
closely identifies that which is practiced by prospective entrepreneurs.
Entrepreneurs make rational assessments, based on the satisfaction of
their needs and potential outcomes of their efforts, which result in a
decision whether, or not, to initiate entrepreneurial behavior or to
seek safer, more traditional employment means.
Expectancy theory is divided into a multiplicative model containing
four different constructs: effort-performance expectancy,
performance-outcome expectancy, valence, and instrumentality (Campbell
& Pritchard, 1976; Lawler, 1973; Nadler & Lawler, 1979).
Effort-performance expectancy ([E.sub.1]) refers to the
individual's perception of the amount of effort required for
successful task completion. Performance-outcome expectancy ([E.sub.2])
refers to the belief that successful task completion will lead to
desired outcomes. Instrumentality (I) is the belief that the attainment
of outcomes will lead to other desired outcomes. Valence (V) refers to
the value of the outcome(s) to the individual.
Self-efficacy is often compared with [E.sub.1] and [E.sub.2] of the
expectancy theory because, at first glance, they seem to describe the
same constructs. However, this is not the case. [E.sub.1] is concerned
with the probability that reasonable effort will result in acceptable
performance and [E.sub.2] is concerned with the probability that
acceptable performance will lead to desired outcomes. Self-efficacy
differs from both of these because it is a cognitive estimation; not of
effort, performance, and outcomes, but of whether one has the required
cognitive and emotional abilities to mobilize the effort that the
expectancy theory takes for granted (Bandura, 1984). Therefore, low
self-efficacy would signify that an individual may perceive that he or
she cannot perform a task at any level while low expectancy would be
interpreted as either the individual not being able to reach an
acceptable performance-level for the effort expended ([E.sub.1]) or not
getting desired outcomes once the performance-level is reached
([E.sub.2]). Clearly, these are different concerns but both are
necessary for entrepreneurs to act on discovered opportunities.
Expectancy theory and parts of the expectancy model have a long
history of having been used to explain the occupational preferences of
individuals (Campbell, Dunnette, Lawler, & Weick, 1970; Lawler &
Suttle, 1973; Mitchell, 1974; Wanous, Keon, & Latack, 1983; Baker,
Ravichandran, & Ramarathnam, 1989; Van Eerde, & Thierry, 1996).
As one of the two major initial expectancy model divisions presented by
Vroom (1964), the valence model was described as being useful for the
prediction of an individual's attraction (valence) for specified
outcomes, which were identified as occupational preference and job
satisfaction (Mitchell, 1974). As such, it is the part of the full
expectancy model that revolves around a person's attractiveness for
possible occupational outcomes and the perceived likelihood that one can
attain these outcomes in the applicable occupation. As it relates to
this study, preference for an entrepreneurial career is defined as the
attractiveness of the possible rewards of entrepreneurship and the
magnitude of one's belief that these rewards can be obtained
(Vroom, 1964; Mitchell, 1974). Therefore, the valence model (summation VI) is a multiplicative function of the valence of possible
entrepreneurial outcomes and the instrumentality that the occupational
choice (entrepreneurship) will lead to these valuable outcomes.
Relevance of Occupational Valences to Entrepreneurship
While it has been demonstrated that the expectancy theory is a
useful tool to measure situational motivation, it has never been used,
empirically, to gauge the level of occupational preference (valence) for
a sample of prospective entrepreneurs. This is curious considering the
potential utility of the valence model for entrepreneurs. Olsen and
Bosserman (1984) introduced the concept of expectancy theory to the
field of entrepreneurship by stating that other approaches (hierarchy of
needs and need for achievement theories) were too specific to be able to
explain the motivating mechanisms for every entrepreneur. In their
application of the expectancy theory, individuals were assumed to differ
regarding needs and goals and people were expected to determine the
course of their behavior based on satisfying those needs and desires.
Since expected outcomes are considered when weighing choices about
alternative career plans, individuals will be inclined to expend effort
on those behaviors that are expected to result in the attainment of
need-satisfying outcomes. What can be assumed, in a general context, is
that an individual, who is attracted to the perceived outcomes of an
entrepreneurial career, will be motivated to initiate entrepreneurial
behavior if such effort is reasonably expected to result in their
acquiring these valuable second-order rewards.
In this conception, the expectancy theory (valence model) is
posited to be general enough to apply to all entrepreneurs. It does not
attempt to delineate all of the specific needs that influence behavior
because of the differences of each individual. It does, however,
identify universal categories of considerations (valences and
instrumentalities) that are cognitively processed to determine
individual behavior over the course of time.
Generally, there are three potential reward categories that are
posited to influence individuals to pursue entrepreneurial careers-the
rewards of profit, independence, and a satisfying way of life
(Longenecker, Moore & Petty, 2000). First, the reward of profit is
the entrepreneur's expectation of earning a yield that will
recompense them for the time and capital that they have devoted as well
as for the risks and initiative they take in running the business. This
reward is the primary basis for initiating any profit-making enterprise.
Without the hope of profit, there exists no entrepreneurial opportunity
(Kirzner, 1973). Second, the reward of independence is the expectation
of freedom from supervision, rules, and bureaucracy (Longenecker, Moore,
& Petty, 2000). This reward is symptomatic of an entrepreneur's
desire to be one's own boss and experience the autonomy of pursuing
whatever course holds personal interest. The reward for independence is
attained and sustained as a result of profitable venturing. Lastly, the
reward of a satisfying way of life is the expectation of freedom from a
routine, boring, and unchallenging job and lifestyle (Longenecker,
Moore, & Petty, 2000). This expectation is characteristic of
entrepreneurs who view their businesses as tools of pleasure instead of
work. This is a common sentiment among entrepreneurs who use their
businesses as an instrument for self-expression and self-actualization
(Scarbourough & Zimmerer, 2000) by using profits and products to
contribute to important societal causes while making a good living.
Thus, it is proposed that these three categories of rewards are the
active agents of expectancy theory (valence) cognitions within potential
and actual entrepreneurs.
Personality Dimensions (Five-Factor Model) and Their Relationship
to Extrinsic Work Rewards
While an individual's perception of the reward of profit may
be a primary basis for initiating a profit-making enterprise
(Longenecker, Moore, & Petty, 2000), the personal desire for profit
or an individual's opinion about the ability of an entrepreneur to
earn profits are clearly extrinsic considerations. Past research
concerning the viability of utilizing, inherently intrinsic, personality
dimensions to predict the attraction for extrinsic work rewards has not
been fruitful (Davis-Blake & Pfeffer, 1989). While it has been
demonstrated that Conscientiousness may positively, and Neuroticism
negatively, predict extrinsic career success (income and occupational
status) (Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999), these results
do not address the preferences that individuals hold for these
work-related rewards. Nevertheless, Judge & Cable (1997) examined an
individual's attraction for extrinsic rewards-oriented work
cultures based on the Five-Factor Model personality dimensions. It is
surmised that one's attraction for a work environment that stresses
high pay for good performance and advanced career opportunities is
indicative of a preference for these rewards (Judge & Cable, 1997).
However, after an analysis of self-reported and peer-group surveys, it
was determined that none of the Five-Factor Model dimensions could
predict personal attraction for any extrinsic work-related reward
oriented cultures. Accordingly, it is not theorized in this study that
any of the personality dimensions will, positively or negatively,
predict the preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the
extrinsic reward of profit. Thus,
Hypothesis 1: There will be no significant relationship between any
of the Five-Factor Personality dimensions and
Preference for an Entrepreneurial Career based on the
extrinsic occupational reward of profit.
Personality Traits (Five-Factor Model) and Their Relationship to
Preference for an Entrepreneurial Career Based on Intrinsic Work Rewards
As a basis for this study, attitudes about intrinsic work-related
outcomes, which may be driven by psychological dimensions (Judge,
Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999), are proposed to influence,
significantly, entrepreneurial career preferences. The Five-Factor Model
of Personality is a descriptive representation (typology) of the five
major dispositional dimensions that encompass human personality.
Personality researchers since Allport (1937) have maintained that
individuals seek out situations that correspond with their
personalities, and empirical research supports this contention (Judge
& Cable, 1997). Consequently, the argument for any of the
Five-Factor personality dimensions to predict preference for an
entrepreneurial career is based upon the correspondence of the
characteristics of the intrinsic work-related rewards being considered
to the characteristics of the personality dimensions that may be
detected within the individual. Since personality dimensions have
previously been used to predict the attitudes and behavior of humans
(Barrick & Mount, 1991), it should be possible to forecast how they
may contribute to individual preference for an entrepreneurial career.
Conscientiousness
Conscientiousness is comprised of three main lower-order facets
(achievement orientation, dependability, and orderliness) and is
indicative of persistence, responsibility, and self-control (Judge,
Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999). Therefore, it follows that
individuals who are highly conscientious may be attracted to
entrepreneurship based on the intrinsic reward of independence. In
short, due to heightened performance abilities and tendencies for
self-control, Conscientious individuals are likely competent and
confident enough not to require, or desire, constant supervision.
In addition to being good performers, Conscientious individuals are
also cautious and risk averse (Goldberg, 1990). Hence, conscientious
individuals may be attracted to occupations that require prudent,
detail-oriented individuals. It has been shown that prospective
entrepreneurs attempt to mitigate the riskiness of new enterprises by
business planning, market analysis, and meticulously estimating
potential profits (Gatewood, Shaver, & Gartner, 1995), among other
things. Since these tasks can be categorized as challenging and
non-routine, Conscientious individuals may be attracted to an
entrepreneurial occupation based on the reward of a satisfying way of
life. As such, it is the desire and ability to perform these types of
complex analyses accurately that is expected to have a positive
influence on the decision to pursue entrepreneurship as a primary
vocational occupation. Thus,
Hypothesis 2: Conscientiousness is positively related to the
Preference for an Entrepreneurial Career based on the
intrinsic occupational reward perceptions of (a)
Independence and (b) a Satisfying Way of Life.
Agreeableness
Agreeable individuals are warm, generous, trusting, and selfless
(Costa & McCrae, 1992b) people who place a premium on sociable
interaction as a way of life (McManus & Kelly, 1999). This tendency
manifests itself as a desire to cooperate, seek consensus and
conformity, and to avoid conflict (Judge & Cable, 1997). As such,
high agreeableness is associated with passivity, dependence, and
tradition (Costa & McCrae, 1992b; Goldberg, 1992). Therefore, it
follows that agreeable individuals may not be attracted to an
entrepreneurial career based on the rewards of independence or a
satisfying way of life. Since tradition and conformity is suggestive of a lifestyle that respects conventional routines (Costa & McCrae,
1992b), there is inadequate conceptual bases from which to argue that
Agreeable individuals might appreciate the reward of a satisfying way of
life. Further, entrepreneurs are taken to be unconventional, risk takers (Begley & Boyd, 1987a; Chen, Greene, & Crick, 1998) who
assertively use their abilities to pursue market opportunities that
others either don't notice or choose to ignore (Kirzner, 1973;
Chandler & Jansen, 1992). Because agreeable individuals are highly
likely to reject the type of autonomous initiative that this market
function suggests, it is also just as highly likely that they may be
repelled by an entrepreneurial career based on the intrinsic reward of
independence. Thus,
Hypothesis 3: Agreeableness is negatively related to the Preference
for an Entrepreneurial Career based on the intrinsic
occupational reward perceptions of (a) Independence and
(b) a Satisfying Way of Life.
Extraversion
Entrepreneurs must be energetic, outgoing, and sociable
(extroversive) when they forecast venture performance to prospective
investors in ambiguous situations (Knight, 1921) or recruit and manage
support personnel to see their venture to fruition (Chandler &
Jansen, 1992) as opposed to being shy, unassertive and withdrawn
(introversive). It is this characteristic of social leadership that is
most distinctive of Extraverts (Judge & Bono, 2000). Further,
individuals who score high for Extraversion tend to be bold, forceful,
and surgent (Goldberg, 1990), which is in line with most traditional
descriptions of the outgoing demeanor of the entrepreneur (Sexton &
Bowman, 1985). It is, therefore, conceivable that Extraverts may prefer
an entrepreneurial career based on the reward of independence, which
personifies autonomous, directive propensities.
In addition to the tendency to become social leaders, Extraverts
are attracted to excitement and stimulation (Costa & McCrae, 1992b),
which may be associated with the preference for an entrepreneurial
career based on the reward of a satisfying way of life. Also, past
research has demonstrated the Extraversion is strongly correlated with
interest in enterprising occupations (Costa, McCrae, & Holland,
1984), such as entrepreneurship. Thus,
Hypothesis 4: Extraversion is positively related to the Preference
for an Entrepreneurial Career based on the intrinsic
occupational reward perceptions of (a) Independence and
(b) a Satisfying Way of Life.
Neuroticism
Individuals who score high on Neuroticism lack self-confidence and
self-esteem (Costa, McCrae, & Dye, 1991). As such, it is a
personality dimension that is wholly opposite to the orientation of
entrepreneurs, as confidence has been argued to be a core characteristic
(Knight, 1921). Neurotic individuals are prone to anxiety, making those
high on it fearful of novel situations and susceptible to feelings of
helplessness (Wiggins, 1996). The vocation of entrepreneurship requires
individuals to develop the independent ability to seek innovative
opportunities in the environment and develop them for personal gain
(Kirzner, 1973), which is distinctive in society. Part of this ability
requires prospective entrepreneurs to venture into unknown territory by
taking calculated risks, making decisions under ambiguous circumstances,
and adapting to a changing state of affairs (Chandler & Jansen,
1992; Chen, Greene, & Crick, 1998). As such, it is not likely that
highly neurotic individuals will develop an attraction for
entrepreneurial work based on the rewards of independence or a
satisfying way of life. Thus,
Hypothesis 5: Neuroticism is negatively related to the Preference for
an Entrepreneurial Career based on the intrinsic
occupational reward perceptions of (a) Independence and
(b) a Satisfying Way of Life.
Openness To Experience
Openness to Experience is expected to be a valid differentiator of
entrepreneurs from others. This dimension assesses personal
characteristics such as curiosity, broadmindedness, intelligence (Judge
& Cable, 1997) and independence of thought (Costa & McCrae,
1992b), which are reflected in an entrepreneur's venturesome spirit
(Knight, 1921). Further, Open individuals are willing to entertain novel
ideas and unconventional values (Costa & McCrae, 1992b) and may
desire situations that are challenging in order to stimulate creativity.
Because Open individuals are also nonconforming and autonomous
(Goldberg, 1990), they should be less attracted to traditional employee
roles and conventional organizational careers. Thus, it is likely that
Open individuals will be attracted to entrepreneurial vocations more
strongly than others based on the rewards of independence and a
satisfying way of life. Thus,
Hypothesis 6: Openness to Experience is positively related to the
Preference for an Entrepreneurial Career based on the
intrinsic occupational reward perceptions of (a)
Independence and (b) a Satisfying Way of Life.
METHODOLOGY
Sample Description and Data Collection
Krueger, Reilly, & Carsrud (2000) find that studies comprising
samples of upper-division college students can uncover occupational
inclinations at a time when respondents are wrestling with important
career decisions. Such samples undoubtedly include subjects with a wide
range of intentions and attitudes toward entrepreneurship. Due to the
sensitivity of intentional processes to initial conditions (Kim &
Hunter, 1993), it is important for researchers to study the onset of
entrepreneurial phenomena before they occur. More precisely, study
samples should include individuals who have not yet made a conscious
decision to initiate new ventures. The sampling of only successful,
current, or openly prospective entrepreneurs (e.g., college students
majoring in entrepreneurship) introduces biases that subjugate data
unpredictably, especially for rare phenomena (Krueger, Reilly, &
Carsrud, 2000). While the exact details of a business may have not yet
come together in the minds of most general upper-level college students,
global career intentions should have (Scherer, Adams, Carley, &
Weibe, 1989). Therefore, it is acceptable and appropriate to investigate
entrepreneurial intent utilizing a sample of upper-level college
students.
Approximately 404 students from a large southeastern university
participated in this study on a voluntary basis utilizing an online,
self-report data collection methodology. Subjects consisted of
upper-level business undergraduates and Master of Business
Administration (MBA) students in the concentrations of marketing,
management, and accounting and professional-degree students from the
College of Veterinary Medicine (CVM).
Upper-level undergraduate students in business, along with those
pursuing the MBA, were appropriate primarily because their academic
concentration implied that they had serious interest in pursuing a
business career. Also, they were likely to offer a more informed range
of interest in terms of business careers than students majoring in the
sciences, liberal arts, humanities, or education. Since the intent to
become an entrepreneur is a business career-related decision process,
these upper-level business students offered a sample that was currently
involved in such a process.
Veterinary students were appropriate for this study because the
nature of their intended profession lends itself easily to the practice
of entrepreneurship. In fact, the norm for success in the field of
veterinary medicine is the ownership of a private practice. A recent
report compiled by the three major veterinary associations in the United
States demonstrates that of the approximately 64,000 veterinarians
employed in the year 1997, 82% worked in private practice (Brown &
Silverman, 1999). Thus, the tendency for veterinarians to become
independent business owners is well established.
The procedure for garnering participation in the study was that of
offering financial incentives coupled with unobtrusive cooperation. The
researcher contacted students directly via mass targeted e-mail messages
originating from the office of their academic major department. A
website was developed so that the students could complete the survey
on-line. In all scenarios, the students were informed that if they
participated in the study, they were included in a sweepstakes drawing
for a number of cash prizes. The chance of winning a prize at any level
was approximately one in ten. Each questionnaire contained an informed
consent statement along with sufficient contact information (e-mail and
phone number) for the researcher to be able to inform students of their
prize winnings. There was no personal identifying information gathered
on the survey instrument itself. E-mail addresses were gathered on the
survey only as an option for those students wanting to participate in
the random cash drawing (100%). Each questionnaire was designed to
collect data on all of the proposed variables in the research model.
After exclusion of subjects with duplicate submissions and those
whose survey questionnaires were only partially completed, the final
sample totaled 351 individuals. This sample was equally represented
between the genders, consisting of 175 (49.8%) males and 176 (50.2%)
females. Subjects were primarily graduating undergraduate business
seniors (71.2%) and 21 to 23 years old (71.1%). In fact, there were more
CVM students (16%) than MBA students (12.8%). The majority of subjects
were Caucasian (White) (83.7%) with the next significant representation
being Black (11.4%), which is in accordance with national population
percentage demographics.
Measures
Five Factor Model of Personality.
Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, and
Openness to Experience was assessed with the 60-item measure of the NEO
Five Factor Personality Inventory-Form S (NEO-FFI-S) (Costa, McCrae,
& Dye, 1991). The NEO-FFI-S was developed to evaluate the five major
dimensions of normal personality: Conscientiousness (C), Agreeableness
(A), Extraversion (E), Neuroticism (N), and Openness (0). Participants
respond to sixty items on a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from
strongly disagree (0) to strongly agree (4). The NEO-FFI-S was developed
as a short version of the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI; Costa &
McCrae, 1995) by selecting the twelve items from the longer listing with
the highest positive or negative factor loadings on each of the five
resultant factors. Cronbach's alpha reliability estimates of .81,
.72, .77, .86, and .73 were reported for the C, A, E, N, and O scales,
respectively, for a sample of 1,539 adults (Costa & McCrae, 1992a).
Construct validity of the NEO-FFI-S is indicated by correlations with
self-report adjective factors of the Five-Factor Model (see Costa &
McCrae, 1992b). Internal consistency reliability for personality
dimensions in this study was measured as .83, .72, .77, .83, and .73 for
the C, A, E, N, and O scales, respectively, which is consistent with
prior research (Defruyt & Mervielde, 1999).
Preference for an Entrepreneurial Career.
Preference for an entrepreneurial career is defined in this study
as the attractiveness of the possible rewards of entrepreneurship and
the magnitude of one's belief that these rewards can be obtained as
an entrepreneur. As such, this multidimensional construct is represented
by the extrinsic reward of profit, and the intrinsic rewards of
independence and a satisfying way of life. These perceptions are
envisioned within the valence model of the expectancy theory (Vroom,
1964; Mitchell, 1974), which has been validated for use to discern
occupational preferences. It is a multiplicative function of the valence
of entrepreneurial outcomes and the instrumentality that the
occupational choice (entrepreneurship) will lead to second-level
outcomes.
[V.sup.n.sub.j] = summation([V.sub.k][I.sub.jk]) k = 1 to n
where
[V.sub.j] = the valence of outcome j (occupation j is a first-level
outcome);
[I.sub.jk] = the perceived instrumentality of outcome j for the
attainment of second-level outcome k;
[V.sub.k] = valence of outcome k. This outcome, which is the result
of obtaining first-level outcome j, is defined as a second-level
outcome;
n = number of outcomes.
The three scales that represent the multidimensional construct were
examined in a pilot study that was conducted prior to the main analysis.
Since the scales were developed from new measures, there exist no
historical reliability indices to report. However, the pilot study
demonstrated that the Cronbach's alpha reliability estimates of
.78, .76, and .83 were reported for the rewards of profit, independence,
and a satisfying way of life, respectively, for an unrelated sample of
349 business school students.
Valence of Outcomes.
Second-level outcome valence is defined as the strength of the
individual's affective orientation (positive or negative) toward
the outcome (Mitchell, 1974). Using scaling procedures adapted from Teas
(1981) and Bartol (1976), eleven potential rewards (second-level
outcomes) of an entrepreneurial career were measured on a 5-point Likert
scale ranging from extremely undesirable (-2) to extremely desirable
(+2). The list of potential second-level outcomes was adapted from
previous research (Teas, 1981; Bartol, 1976) and theory (Longenecker,
Moore, & Petty, 2000).
Instrumentality.
Instrumentality pertains to the degree to which the occupational
choice alternative is instrumental in leading to, or detracting from, a
second-level outcome. According to Vroom (1964) this variable can range
from fully negative to fully positive. Consequently, this variable was
measured by eleven items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from
extremely unlikely (-2) to extremely likely (+2).
Demographic and Background Information.
Information pertaining to each respondent's age, gender,
ethnicity, and class was obtained to use as control variables in the
analysis. Each of these control variables was recorded as noncontinuous,
categorical predictors.
ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
Correlation Matrix
An examination of the correlation matrix reveals that although some
variables are significantly correlated, no correlation coefficient is
greater than .371. A contribution to the construct validity of items in
this study is that some of the measures are correlated significantly,
but not highly enough to signify that any of them are measuring the same
constructs (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994).
Significant simple correlations demonstrated that Conscientiousness
and Extraversion were positively, and Neuroticism was negatively,
related to the intrinsic occupational reward perception of a Satisfying
Way of Life. Furthermore, Conscientiousness was positively, and
Neuroticism negatively, related to the extrinsic occupational reward of
profit. Last, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Openness to
Experience were positively correlated to the intrinsic occupational
reward of independence.
Hierarchical Regression Analysis
Hierarchical regression was the principal technique of analysis
used to assess the hypotheses in the investigation. All relevant
variables were standardized prior to regression analyses. Cohen &
Cohen (1983) suggest this method is most important when independent
variables possess a theoretically based casual priority, as in this
study.
The hierarchical regression procedure was used to test the research
model in three separate phases (Table 1). The first phase concerned the
relationship between the dimensions of the Five-Factor Model of
personality and preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the
extrinsic reward of profit. The second phase included the relationship
between the dimensions of the Five-Factor Model of personality and
preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the intrinsic reward
of independence. The final phase specified the relationship between the
dimensions of the Five-Factor Model of personality and preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the intrinsic reward of a satisfying way
of life. In all phases, gender, race, class, and age were included as
the first step in the procedure to control for any effects they may have
had on the proposed relationships. Principally, it was expected that
some of the Five-Factor personality dimensions would predict an
individuals affection for the intrinsic occupational rewards of
independence and a satisfying way of life and none would predict an
individuals preference for the extrinsic reward of profit.
Results of the Phase One Analysis
The result of the Phase One analysis is presented in Table 2.
Hypothesis H1 predicted that none of the Five-Factor personality
dimensions would predict preference for an entrepreneurial career based
on the extrinsic occupational reward of profit. The hypothesis was
tested in Phase One of the analysis using two steps. In step one, the
control variables of gender, race, educational classification, and age
were regressed on preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the
reward of independence. As exhibited in Table 2, the F statistic of
0.952 was not significant indicating that gender, race, educational
classification, and age do not have a significant relation to
entrepreneurial career preference based on the reward of profit.
Next, the Five-Factor Model of Personality dimensions were added to
the previous equation step 2). Again, the resultant model was not
significant (F = 1.476, p > .05) and produced no significant change
in [R.sub.2] (change in [R.sub.2] = .27, p. > .05). These results
provide support for H1 by demonstrating that personality dimensions have
no relation to an individual's preference for the extrinsic reward
of profit.
Results of the Phase Two Analysis
The result of the Phase Two analysis is presented in Table 3.
Hypotheses H2(a), H4(a), and H6(a) predicted that Conscientiousness,
Extraversion, and Openness to experience are positively and
significantly associated with the preference for an entrepreneurial
career based on the reward of independence. Specifically, the more that
an individual is reliable, assertive and focused, and open to learn new
things, the more he or she may prefer an entrepreneurial career based on
the autonomy that it offers. Conversely, hypotheses H3(a) and H5(a)
suggest that agreeable and neurotic individuals will be negatively
associated with the preference for an entrepreneurial career based on
the reward of independence. In other words, poorly emotionally adjusted
and traditional individuals will not desire entrepreneurial careers
because they are expected to be repelled at the idea of working
independent of authority. These hypotheses were tested in Phase Two of
the analysis using two steps. In step one, the control variables of
gender, race, educational classification, and age were regressed on
preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the reward of
independence. As exhibited in Table 3 the F statistic of 1.138 was not
significant indicating that gender, race, educational classification,
and age do not have a significant relation to entrepreneurial career
preference based on the reward of independence.
Next, the Five-Factor Model of personality dimensions were added to
the previous equation (step 2). The model was significant (F = 2.989, p
< .01) and produced a significant change in [R.sub.2] (change in
[R.sub.2] = .06, p. < .001). Results provide support for the proposed
positive relation between Conscientiousness and preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the reward of independence (beta = .178;
p < .01)(H2(a)). Results also supported the positive relationship
between Openness to Experience and preference for an entrepreneurial
career based on the reward of independence (beta = .125; p < .05)
(H6(a)). However, the hypothesized positive relation between
Extraversion (beta = .063; p > .05) and preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the reward of independence was not
supported (H4(a)). Neither were the proposed negative relationships
involving Agreeableness (beta = -.088; p > .05)(H3(a)) nor
Neuroticism (beta = -.020; p > .05)(H5(a)) with entrepreneurial
career preference (independence).
Overall, the results of Phase Two tests indicated that individuals
who are highly reliable performers (Conscientious) and those who
appreciate new experiences (Open) are attracted to entrepreneurship as a
plausible career choice because they, most likely, don't want to be
bound by the limitations imposed on them while working in subordinate
occupations. It is possible that Conscientious individuals don't
believe that they need supervising and Open individuals probably prefer
to be free to make their own decisions, including making potential
mistakes, as part of the learning process.
Results of the Phase Three Analysis
The results of the Phase Three analysis are presented in Table 4.
Hypotheses H2(b), H4(b), and H6(b) suggested that Conscientiousness,
Extraversion, and Openness to Experience are positively and
significantly associated with the preference for an entrepreneurial
career based on the reward of a satisfying way of life. Specifically,
the more that an individual is a reliable performer, assertive and
focused, and open to learn new things, the more he or she will prefer an
entrepreneurial career based on his or her perception of the exciting,
challenging, and non-repetitive lifestyle that it promises. Conversely,
hypotheses H3(b) and H5(b) suggests that Agreeable and Neurotic
individuals will be negatively associated with the preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the reward of a satisfying way of life.
In other words, individuals with Neurotic tendencies and those who would
rather conform to tradition instead of doing something more creative
(Agreeable) will not prefer an entrepreneurial career because they are
possibly not well-suited for the originality (Kirzner, 1973) or
innovativeness (Schumpeter, 1934) required of entrepreneurs.
The hypotheses were tested in Phase Three of the analysis utilizing
two steps. In step one, the control variables of gender, race,
educational classification, and age were regressed on preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the reward of a satisfying way of life.
As exhibited in Table 4, the F statistic of .471 was not significant
indicating that gender, race, educational classification, and age do not
have a significant relation to entrepreneurial career preference based
on the reward of a satisfying way of life.
Next, all of the Five-Factor Model of personality dimensions were
added to the previous equation (step 2). Although the model was highly
significant (F = 3.493, p < .001) and produced a significant change
in [R.sub.2] (change in [R.sub.2] =.079, p. < .001), the results only
provide support for the proposed positive relation between
Conscientiousness and preference for an entrepreneurial career based on
the reward of a satisfying way of life (beta = .198; p <
.001)(H2(b)). The results do not support the proposed positive
relationships between Extraversion (beta = .049; p > .05) (H4(b)) or
Openness to Experience (beta = .084; p > .05) (H6(b)) and preference
for an entrepreneurial career based on the reward of a satisfying way of
life. Also, the hypothesized negative relationships between
Agreeableness (beta = .014; p > .05)(H3(b)) and Neuroticism (beta =
-.090; p > .05)(H5(b)) and preference for an entrepreneurial career
based on the reward of a satisfying way of life were, similarly, not
supported.
Overall, the results of Phase Three tests indicated that
individuals who are highly Conscientious are attracted to
entrepreneurship as a probable career choice because of the perceived
exciting and challenging lifestyle. Since Conscientious individuals are
strong performers, they would likely be discouraged performing remedial
or monotonous day-to-day tasks.
DISCUSSION
In summary, the current research examined the association of
personality dimensions (as embedded in the Five-Factor Model of
personality) to preference for an entrepreneurial career based on
intrinsic and extrinsic occupational rewards. As stated in hypothesis 1,
it was expected that there would be no relation between an
individual's personality dimensions and affection for the extrinsic
reward of entrepreneurial profit. In fact, there was no justifying
literature that would convince one to forecast this relationship. So, it
is not surprising that no relationship was found.
In hypothesis 2, it was anticipated that Conscientiousness would be
positively associated with preference for an entrepreneurial career
(based on (1) the reward of independence and (2) the reward of a
satisfying way of life) and (3) entrepreneurial intentions (direct
effect). Basically, it was posited that individuals who are reliable,
achievement oriented, purposeful, and strong-willed (Conscientious),
would be more likely to prefer an entrepreneurial career, for the
autonomy (independence) and challenge (satisfying lifestyle) that
entrepreneurship offers. Both of the proposed relationships to the
entrepreneurial career preference considerations (H2a-b)) were supported
in this study.
There exist a few previous empirical inquiries that might shed some
insight on the observed relationships. Judge & Cable (1997)
demonstrated that Conscientiousness is negatively related to
team-oriented work cultures. As such, the researchers determined that
Conscientious people prefer individualism as opposed to collectivism.
Hence, it follows that the current study would find that
Conscientiousness is positively related to preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the reward of independence. Since
Conscientious individuals value high achievement, reliability, and order
(Costa & McCrae, 1992b), it appears that they would rather not have
their performance diminished by forced reliance on others. Consequently,
the idea of a vocation, like entrepreneurship, where Conscientious
individuals can excel independent of, or at least in control of, others
is appealing to them and is consistent with the findings of Judge &
Cable (1997).
This study also found a significant relationship between
Conscientiousness and preference for an entrepreneurial career based on
the reward of a satisfying way of life. More specifically, the study
found that Conscientious individuals are more likely to prefer
entrepreneurial occupations because they value professional lifestyles
of excitement, challenge, and stimulation as opposed to remediation,
monotony, and boredom (Costa & McCrae, 1992b). This finding is
similar to that found in Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick (1999),
which demonstrated that Conscientiousness was able to predict intrinsic
job satisfaction, a construct similar in some characteristics to
entrepreneurial preference based on the reward of a satisfying way of
life. In their study, the observed positive effect of Conscientiousness
on intrinsic job satisfaction was significant even after the
investigators controlled for the contribution of general mental ability,
another performance predictor. The researchers concluded that knowledge
about one's personality proved to be an effective predictor about
subjective and objective career success (Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, &
Barrick, 1999). All things considered, it is apparent that Conscientious
people are attracted to livelihoods that offer them opportunities to
test their abilities and confront their limitations.
Based on hypothesis 3, it was predicted that Agreeableness would be
negatively associated with preference for an entrepreneurial career
(based on (a) the rewards of independence and (b) a satisfying way of
life). That is, it was expected that individuals who are highly trusting
and dependent (Agreeable) would be repelled by the task requirements
involving personal challenge, forcefulness, and independent activity
that entrepreneurs must execute in the course of founding and developing
new ventures (Chandler & Jansen, 1992).
The results of the current study did not support the envisioned
negative associations between Agreeableness and preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on the reward of independence (H3(a)) or
the reward of a satisfying way of life (H3(b)). A possible explanation
for this finding might be since Agreeable individuals value support,
cooperation, and conflict avoidance whenever possible (Costa &
McCrae, 1992b), they may be unconcerned with the expected rewards
related to entrepreneurial careers. Being that they might not want to be
viewed as untraditional, it is wholly plausible that highly Agreeable
individuals may be fundamentally repulsed by the idea of
entrepreneurship as a career option so they do not bother to form
opinions about the specifics of the vocation at all. This is consistent
with the findings of Judge & Cable (1997). The researchers found
that job seekers who scored high on the dimension of Agreeableness were
significantly less attracted to aggressive, outcome-oriented, and
decisive organizational environments. Since entrepreneurial careers have
been described as requiring achievement oriented (McClelland, 1961),
independent thinking (Kirzner, 1973), self-starters (Knight, 1921), it
is a reasonable suggestion that highly conforming (Agreeable)
individuals may not significantly consider the rewards of a vocation
that they find distasteful.
According to hypotheses 4 and 5, it was anticipated that there
would be significant associations for extraversion (positive
associations) and neuroticism (negative associations) with
entrepreneurial career preferences based on intrinsic entrepreneurial
work rewards. However, none of the hypothesized relationships were
supported in this study.
The fact that there were no significant findings between
Extraversion and any other study variables was unanticipated. However,
these non-findings are not unique due to the inconsistent evidence
involving Extraversion and occupational variables in previous research.
While Extraversion has been strongly correlated with interest in
enterprising occupations (Costa, McCrae, & Holland, 1984), some
researchers have noted (after regression analysis) that Extraversion
displays no significance for favoring enterprising jobs (Judge, Higgins,
Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999) like that of entrepreneurship.
Accordingly, both of these previous findings are consistent with the
results in this study. In the current examination, Extraversion was
highly correlated with entrepreneurial career preferences based on
independence and a satisfying way of life. However, regression analysis
demonstrated that the dimension is not significantly related to
preference for an entrepreneurial career based on either of the
intrinsic entrepreneurial career rewards. Thus, the results of this
study are consistent with previous findings.
Similar to Extraversion, Neuroticism also was not found to hold any
significant relation to any of the variables of interest in this study.
Neuroticism was expected to relate negatively to entrepreneurial career
preferences based on the rewards of independence (H5(a)) and a
satisfying way of life (H5(b)). It was predicted that since Neurotic
individuals are anxiety-driven, fearful of novel situations, and
vulnerable to feelings of helplessness (Wiggins, 1996), they would
neither value the independence nor the challenge of an entrepreneurial
career and, thus, refrain from forming entrepreneurial career
preferences. While Neuroticism has been termed the most pervasive domain
of all personality scales (Costa and McCrae, 1992b: page 14), it has
failed to register significance for occupational preferences in previous
studies (De Fruyt & Mervielde, 1999; Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, &
Barrick, 1999).
Since both Extraversion and Neuroticism have failed to
significantly explain vocational preferences and attraction for
differing types of occupational environments in past research, the
findings of nonsignificance in this study are not inconsistent. However,
correlational support for Extraversion's positive relation to
preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the rewards of
independence (H4(a)) and a satisfying way of life (H4(b)); and,
Neuroticism's negative relation to entrepreneurial career
preference based on the reward of a satisfying way of life, does offer
hope that these associations will be corroborated in future research.
In hypothesis 6, it was posited that Openness to Experience would
prove to be a valid predictor of preference for an entrepreneurial
career (based on the rewards of (a) independence and (b) a satisfying
way of life). In fact, it was suggested that this personality dimension
might, possibly, be the most significant predictor of entrepreneurial
career preference. Principally, it was predicted that individuals who
are willing to entertain novel ideas and unconventional values (Costa
& McCrae, 1992a), nonconforming and autonomous (Goldberg, 1990),
inquisitive, open-minded, and intelligent (Judge & Cable, 1997)
would appreciate the intrinsic rewards of an entrepreneurial career
significantly more than others. The findings of this study affirm the
hypothesis that Open individuals significantly value the entrepreneurial
career reward of independence, resulting in a preference for an
entrepreneurial career. Not supported in this study was the projected
positive relation of Openness to Experience to preference for an
entrepreneurial career based on a satisfying way of life.
The fact that Open individuals were attracted to an entrepreneurial
career because of the perceived independence that the vocation of
entrepreneurship affords is consistent with prior research. Judge,
Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, (1999) empirically determined that
Open individuals are negatively related to conventional occupations. As
such, it appears that an Open individual's abhorrence of conformity
and desire for self-reliance makes him or her an ideal match for
autonomous entrepreneurial work (Knight, 2001; Kirzner, 1973). In
addition, the finding that Openness is positively and directly related
to entrepreneurial intentions affirms several previous investigations.
Judge and Cable (1997) found that Open individuals are strongly
attracted to innovative and detail oriented organizational cultures.
Since entrepreneurial occupations revolve around innovative (Schumpeter,
1934) and detail-oriented (Gatewood, Shaver, & Gartner, 1995)
activity, the findings of the current study are consistent. Further,
Openness to Experience has been found to significantly predict whether,
or not, individuals choose to become employed in enterprising
occupations (De Fruyt & Mervielde, 1999) like entrepreneurship.
Taken together, these studies provide additional support for the results
of the current examination.
The lack of a significant relation between Openness to Experience
and the preference for an entrepreneurial career based on the reward of
a satisfying way of life is curious. The reward of a satisfying way of
life was described in this study as freedom from a non-challenging,
routine, and boring occupational lifestyle (Longenecker, Moore, &
Petty, 2000). Since Open individuals are known for independence of
thought, intellect, curiosity, and creativity (Costa & McCrae,
1992b; Judge & Cable, 1997), it was expected that the challenge and
excitement of an entrepreneurial career would be appealing. However,
this expectation was not affirmed. A possible explanation for this
finding may exist in the possible perception that Open individuals may
hold concerning their assessment of the reward of a satisfying way of
life. Since there exists many other occupations, in addition to
entrepreneurship, that consist of challenging and non-routine job
characteristics, it is possible that a career that assures a satisfying
way of life is too conventional (common) to appeal, in particular, to,
highly unconventional (Costa & McCrae, 1992b), Open individuals. In
other words, there is nothing unique, in itself, about a career that may
be considered exciting and challenging. Since Open individuals are
attracted to uncommon work situations as opposed to the ordinary (Judge
& Cable, 1997), it is feasible that people who score high for
Openness to Experience may not be swayed to prefer entrepreneurial
careers based, solely, on the reward of a satisfying way of life.
Further research is warranted to determine if this variable serves as a
mediator of some other relationship.
CONCLUSION
The results of the current study provide practitioners, such as
entrepreneurs, vocational educators, and public policy administrators, a
number of practical implications that may assist in the expansion of the
entrepreneurship agenda. By discerning how entrepreneurial career
preferences are formed, policy makers may be able to form programs that
take advantage of robust interest in entrepreneurial rewards to help
promote new business creation initiatives.
The current study demonstrated that the value that people designate
to these potential rewards was shown to be strongly influence by some of
their personality dimensions. Better education to enhance knowledge
about the likelihood of realistically attaining these rewards should
provide valuable perspective from which to form career-related
judgments. Essentially, the more that people understand that
entrepreneurial work requires long hours and dedicated effort (Chandler
& Jansen, 1992) instead of focusing, solely, on potential rewards
and accolades should help to decrease the notoriously high failure rates
of new ventures (Cromie, 1994) that are initiated by unsuspecting
entrepreneur novices. For example, the results suggest that people who
Conscientious and Open to Experience may attracted to the independence
and challenges that are an entrepreneurial reality. Therefore, any
entrepreneurial training that Conscientious or Open individuals receive
should include in-depth analyses about which rewards may reasonably be
attained and in what timeframes for particular types of businesses. In
this manner, prospective entrepreneurs can develop realistic business
plans based on pragmatic working lifestyles and realistic compensation
expectations.
These indications suggest that one way to identify people who are
compatible, and those who are incompatible, with the prospect of
becoming entrepreneurs is to locate individuals who are open and
conscientious. It is suspected that some may doubt the practical
feasibility of utilizing personality tests to discriminate between
potential, and improbable, entrepreneurs. This reservation is
particularly understandable when one reasons that much of the past
entrepreneurship research failed to find consistent relationships
between personality and behavior (Gartner, 1988; Shaver & Scott,
1991; Ripsas, 1998). However, the results of this study may provide some
new insights for the development of inventive approaches to vocational
counseling. Given that personality dimensions are related to
entrepreneurial career preference and potential entrepreneurial
behavior, efforts in this direction are warranted.
REFERENCES
Allport G.W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation.
New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
Baker, D.D., Ravichandran, R., & Randall, D.M. (1989).
Exploring contrasting formulations of expectancy theory. Decision
Sciences, 20(1): 1-13.
Bandura, A. (1984). Recycling misconceptions of perceived
self-efficacy. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8: 231-255.
Baron, R.A. (1998). Cognitive mechanisms in entrepreneurship: Why
and when entrepreneurs think differently than other people. Journal of
Business Venturing, 13(4): 275-295.
Barrick M. & Mount M. (1991). The big five personality
dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology,
44, 1-26.
Bartol, K.M. (1976). Expectancy theory as a predictor of female
occupational choice and attitude toward business. Academy of Management
Journal, 19: 669-675.
Begley, T. & Boyd, D. (1987). Psychological characteristics
associated with performance in entrepreneurial firms and smaller
businesses. Journal of Business Venturing, 2: 79-93.
Bird, B.J.(1988). Implementing entrepreneurial ideas: The case for
intention. The Academy Of Management Review, 13(3): 442-454.
Brockhaus, R. (1980). Risk-taking propensity of entrepreneurs.
Academy Of Management Journal, 23(3): 509
Brockhaus, R.H. & Nord, W.R. (1979). An exploration of the
factors affecting the entrepreneurial decision: Personal characteristics
versus environmental conditions. Academy of Management Proceedings,
364-368.
Brown, J. & Silverman, J. (1999). The current and future market
for veterinarians and veterinary medical services in the United States.
Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 215, 2: 161-183.
Busenitz, L.W. & Barney, J.B. (1997). Differences between
entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: Biases and heuristics
in strategic decision-making. Journal of Business Venturing, 12(1):
9-31.
Buss, A. & Finn, S.E. (1987). Classification of personality
traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(2): 432-444.
Campbell, J.P., Dunnette, M.D., Lawler, E.E., III & Weick, E.,
Jr. (1970). Managerial Behavior, Performance, & Effectiveness. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Campbell, J.P., & Pritchard. R.D. (1976). Motivation theory in
industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.).
Handbook of Industrial & Organizational Psychology: 63-130. Chicago:
Rand McNally.
Cattell, R. B. (1947). Confirmation and clarification of
personality factors. Psychometrika, 13: 197-220.
Chandler, G.N. & Jansen, E. (1992). The founder's
self-assessed competence and venture performance. Journal of Business
Venturing, 7(3): 223-237.
Chen, C.C., Greene, P., & Crick, A. (1998). Does
entrepreneurial self-efficacy distinguish entrepreneurs from managers?
Journal of Business Venturing, 13(4): 295-317.
Cohen, J. & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (Second
Edition). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Cooper, A.C., Woo, C.Y., & Dunkelberg, W.C. (1988).
Entrepreneurs' perceived chances for success. Journal of Business
Venturing, 3(2): 97-109.
Costa P., & McCrae R. (1992a). Four ways five-factors are
basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13: 653-665.
Costa P., & McCrae R. (1992b). Professional manual for the NEO
PI-R and NEO-FFI. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Costa, P., & McCrae, R. (1995). Solid ground in the wetlands of
personality: A reply to Block. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2): 216-221.
Costa P., McCrae R, & Dye D. (1991). Facet scales for
agreeableness and conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO personality
inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 12: 887-898.
Costa P., McCrae R, & Holland, J.L. (1984). Personality and
vocational interests in an adult sample. Journal of Applied Psychology,
69: 390-400.
Cromie, S. (1994). Entrepreneurship: The role of the individual in
small business development. IBAR, 15:62-76.
Das, T.K. & Teng, B. (1997). Time and entrepreneurial risk
behavior. Entrepreneurship Theory & Practice, 22(2):69-89.
Davis-Blake, A. & Pfeffer, J. (1989) Just a mirage: The search
for dispositional effects in organizational research. Academy of
Management Review, 14(3): 385-400.
De Fruyt, F. & Mervielde, I. (1999). RIASEC types & big
five traits as predictors of employment status & nature of
employment. Personnel Psychology, 52(3): 701-727.
Fiske, D. (1949). Consistency in the factorial structure of
personality ratings from different sources. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 44: 329-366.
Gartner, W.B. (1988). 'Who is an entrepreneur?' Is the
wrong question. American Journal of Small Business, 12(4): 11-33.
Gatewood, E. (1993). The expectancies in public sector venture
assistance. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 17(2) 91-96.
Gatewood, E.J., Shaver, K.G., & Gartner, W.B. (1995). A
longitudinal study of cognitive factors influencing start-up behaviors
and success at venture creation. Journal of Business Venturing, 10(5):
371-392.
Goldberg L. (1990). An alternative "description of
personality: The Big-Five-Factor structure. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 59: 1216-1229.
Goldberg L. (1992). The development of markers for the
Big-Five-Factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4: 26-42.
Hogan R.T. (1991). Personality and personality measurement. In
Dunnette M.D., Hough L.M. (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and
Organizational Psychology (pp. 873-919). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Judge, T. & Bono, S. (2000). Five-factor model of personality
& transformational leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5):
751-764.
Judge, T. & Cable, D. (1997) Applicant personality,
organizational culture, & organization attraction. Personnel
Psychology, 50, 2; 359-395.
Judge, T., Higgins, C., Thoresen, C., & Barrick, M. (1999). The
big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success
across the life span. Personnel Psychology, 52(3): 621-653.
Kaish, S. & Gilad, B. (1991). Characteristics of opportunities
search of entrepreneurs vs. executives: Sources, interests, general
alertness. Journal of Business Venturing, 6(1): 45-62.
Kim, M.S. & Hunter, J. (1993). Relationships among attitude,
behavioral intentions, and behavior. Communication Research, 20:
331-364.
Kirzner, I. (1973). Competition and entrepreneurship. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Knight, F.H. (1921). Risk, uncertainty and profit. New York:
Houghton Mifflin.
Krueger, N. Jr., Reilly, M.D., & Carsrud, A.L. (2000).
Competing models of entrepreneurial intentions. Journal of Business
Venturing, 15(5): 411-432.
Lawler, E.E., III. (1973). Motivation in work organizations.
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Lawler, E.E., III., & Suttle, J.L. (1973). Expectancy theory
and job behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 9:
482-503.
Longenecker, J., Moore, C., & Petty, J (2000). Small business
management: An entrepreneurial emphasis (Eleventh Edition.). Cincinnati,
OH: South-Western College Publishing.
Manimala, M.J. (1992). Entrepreneurial heuristics: A comparison
between high PI (pioneering-innovative) and low PI ventures. Journal of
Business Venturing, 7(6): 477-305.
McClelland, D. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ: Van
Nostrand.
McManus, M.A., & Kelly, M.L. (1999). Personality measures and
biodata: Evidence regarding their incremental predictive value in the
life insurance industry. Personnel Psychology. 52(1): 137-148.
Mitchell, T.R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction,
occupational preference and effort: A theoretical, methodological, and
empirical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81: 1053-1077.
Nadler, D.A. & Lawler, E.E. III. (1983). Motivation: A
diagnostic approach. In J. R. Hackman, E.E. Lawler III, & L.W.
Porter (eds.) Perspectives on Behavioral Organizations, pp. 67-78. New
York: McGraw Hill Book Company.
Nunnally, J.C. & Bernstein, LH. (1994). Psychometric theory,
(Third Edition). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Olson, P.D. & Bosserman, D.A. (1984). Attributes of the
entrepreneurial type. Business Horizons, 27: 53-56.
Palich, L.E. & Bagby, D.R. (1995). Using cognitive theory to
explain entrepreneurial risk-taking: Challenging conventional wisdom.
Journal of Business Venturing, 10, 425-439.
Ripsas, S. (1998). Towards an interdisciplinary theory of
entrepreneurship. Small Business Economics, 10(2): 103-116.
Scarbourough, N., & Zimmerer, T. (2000). Effective small
business management (Sixth Edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice-Hall Inc.
Scherer, R.F., Adams, J.S., Carley, S.S., & Wiebe, F.A. (1989).
Role model performance effects on the development of entrepreneurial
career preference. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 13(3): 53-71.
Schumpeter, J.A. (1934). The theory of economic development.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Sexton, D.L. & Bowman, N. (1985). The entrepreneur: A capable
executive and more. Journal of Business Venturing, 1(1): 129-141.
Shaver, K.G. & Scott, L. (1991). Person, process, choice: The
psychology of new venture creation. Entrepreneurship Theory and
Practice, 16(2): 23-46.
Staw, B., Bell, N. & Clausen, J. (1986). The dispositional
approach to job attitudes: A lifetime longitudinal test. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 31: 56-77.
Staw, B., & Ross, J. (1985). Stability in the midst of change:
A dispositional approach to job attitudes. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 70: 469-480.
Stewart, W.H. Jr., Watson, W.E., Carland, J.C., & Carland, J.W.
(1998). A proclivity for entrepreneurship: A comparison of
entrepreneurs, small business owners, and corporate managers. Journal of
Business Venturing, 14(2): 189-215.
Teas, R.K. (1981). An empirical test of models of sales
persons' job expectancy and instrumentality perceptions. Journal of
Marketing Research, 18: 209-226.
Van Eerde, W., & Thierry, H. (1996). Vroom's expectancy
models and work-related criteria: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 81(5): 575-587.
Vroom, V.H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Vroom, V.H. (1966). Organizational choice: A study for pre- and
post-decision processes. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
1: 212-225.
Wanous, J.P., Keon, T.L., & Latack, J.C. (1983). Expectancy
theory and occupational/organizational choices: A review and test.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 32: 66-86.
Wiggins JS (Ed). (1996). The five-factor model of personality:
Theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.
Jeff Brice, Jr., Texas Southern University
Table 1: Outline of Phases 1-3 of the Hierarchical Regression Analysis
Regression Dependent Variable Independent Variables
Entered
All Phases
Step 1 Preference for an Gender, Race, Class, Age
Entrepreneurial Career
Phase 1
Step 2 Preference for an CONSCI, AGREE, EXTRA
Entrepreneurial Career NEURO, OPEN
(Profit)
Phase 2
Step 2 Preference for an CONSCI, AGREE, EXTRA
Entrepreneurial Career NEURO, OPEN
(Independence)
Phase 3
Step 2 Preference for an CONSCI, AGREE, EXTRA
Entrepreneurial Career NEURO, OPEN
(Satisfying Way of Life)
CONSCI (Conscientiousness)
AGREE (Agreeableness)
EXTRA (Extraversion);
NEURO (Neuroticism)
OPEN (Openness to Experience)
Table 2: Phase One Analysis
Regression Dependent Independent Beta
Variable Variables (Standardized)
Step 1 Preference for an Gender (.055)
Entrepreneurial Race (.026)
Career (Profit) Class (.014)
Age (.079)
Step 2 Preference for an
Entrepreneurial CONSCI (.067)
Career (Profit) AGREE (.017)
EXTRA (.036)
NEURO (.091)
OPEN (.072)
Regression Dependent Independent F [R.sup.2]
Variable Variables
Step 1 Preference for an .952 .011
Entrepreneurial Gender
Career (Profit) Race
Class
Age
Step 2 Preference for an 1.476 .037
Entrepreneurial CONSCI
Career (Profit) AGREE
EXTRA
NEURO
OPEN
Regression Dependent Independent [R.sup.2] Partial F
Variable Variables (Change)
Step 1 Preference for an .011 0.952
Entrepreneurial Gender
Career (Profit) Race
Class
Age
Step 2 Preference for an .027 1.885
Entrepreneurial CONSCI
Career (Profit) AGREE
EXTRA
NEURO
OPEN
N = 351
* p < .05
** p < .01
*** p < .001
( ) Negative relationships
CONSCI - Conscientiousness
AGREE - Agreeableness
EXTRA - Extraversion
NEURO - Neuroticism
OPEN - Openness to Experience
Table 3: Phase Two Analysis
Dependent Independent Beta
Regression Variable Variables (Standardized)
Step 1 Preference for an Gender (.096)
Entrepren-eurial Race (.016)
Career Class (.027)
(Independence) Age .039
Step 2 Preference for an
Entrepreneurial
Career CONSCI .178 **
(Independence) AGREE (.088)
EXTRA .063
NEURO (.020)
OPEN .125 *
Dependent
Regression Variable F [R.sup.2]
Step 1 Preference for an 1.138 .013
Entrepren-eurial
Career
(Independence)
Step 2 Preference for an 2.989 ** .073
Entrepreneurial
Career
(Independence)
Dependent [R.sup.2]
Regression Variable (Change) Partial F
Step 1 Preference for an .013 1.138
Entrepren-eurial
Career
(Independence)
Step 2 Preference for an .060 .426 ***
Entrepreneurial
Career
(Independence)
N = 351
* p < .05
** p < .01
*** p < .001
() Negative relationships
CONSCI - Conscientiousness
AGREE - Agreeableness
EXTRA - Extraversion
NEURO - Neuroticism
OPEN - Openness to Experience
Table 4: Phase Three Analysis
Dependent Independent Beta
Regression Variable Variables (Standardized)
Step 1 Preference for an Gender .026
Entrepreneurial Race .008
Career Class .056
(Satisfying Way Age (.041)
of Life)
Step 2 Entrepreneurial
Career CONSCI .198 ***
(Satisfying Way AGREE .014
of Life) EXTRA .049
NEURO (.090)
OPEN .084
Dependent
Regression Variable F [R.sup.2]
Step 1 Preference for an .471 .005
Entrepreneurial
Career
(Satisfying Way
of Life)
Step 2 Entrepreneurial 3.493 *** .084
Career
(Satisfying Way
of Life)
Dependent [R.sup.2]
Regression Variable (Change) Partial F
Step 1 Preference for an .005 .471
Entrepreneurial
Career
(Satisfying Way
of Life)
Step 2 Entrepreneurial .079 5.885 ***
Career
(Satisfying Way
of Life)
N = 351
* p < .05
** p < .01
*** p < .001
() Negative relationships
CONSCI - Conscientiousness
AGREE - Agreeableness
EXTRA - Extraversion
NEURO - Neuroticism
OPEN - Openness to Experience