A longitudinal study of the entrepreneurial intentions of university students.
Audet, Josee
ABSTRACT
This paper presents a longitudinal study of entrepreneurial
intentions among university students enrolled in a business
administration program. Data was collected twice: first during the last
semester in school and then 18 months later, once students had graduated
and started to work on a full-time basis. Results confirm that the
perceptions of the desirability and feasibility of launching a business
significantly explain the formation of an intention to go into business
on a long term horizon. However, the model does not hold as well when
the time frame is shorter. Indeed, both perceptions fail to explain to a
significant degree the intention to go into business. When work
satisfaction is added to the model, both this variable and perception of
feasibility become significant predictors of short term intentions,
perception of desirability remaining non significant. Our results also
tend to indicate that entrepreneurial intentions and perceptions vary
over time. As the temporal stability of intention is a condition for an
intention-based model to accurately predict behavior, the link between
entrepreneurial intentions and actual venture creation may prove
difficult to establish. Data collected in the following phase of the
study should provide more information about the temporal stability of
variables.
INTRODUCTION
If there is one question that has not been answered satisfactorily,
it is that of whether it is possible to identify the people within a
society who will eventually go on to start their own businesses. This
question is of considerable interest. Let us imagine for a moment that
it is possible to identify the individuals in a given group who have
what it takes to succeed in business, or whose profile is consistent
with the composite profile of the successful entrepreneur. Financial
institutions would find it considerably easier to decide who qualifies
for a loan and who does not, simply by administering a test to determine
whether the applicant meets the "successful entrepreneur"
criteria. Similarly, "future entrepreneurs" could be
identified as soon as they entered the educational system, and directed
towards an academy of entrepreneurship whose mission would be to provide
suitable training, in the same way that the former communist countries
used to recruit and train their most promising athletes. Clearly, these
examples are completely unrealistic, but they nevertheless illustrate
society's insatiable curiosity about new venture creation and its
principal actor, the entrepreneur. Indeed, this quest for the "Holy
Grail" has fuelled research in the field of entrepreneurship for
many years.
Several theoretical approaches have been developed to explain why
some people eventually become entrepreneurs. Among these, a relatively
new stream of research has emerged, based on entrepreneurial intentions.
More specifically, the intention to start a business is thought to be
the best predictor of actual venture creation, such intention being
formed by perceptions of the desirability and feasibility of going into
business. This research perspective looks promising as the few empirical
studies that have verified the link between perceptions and intentions
have yielded convincing results. However, we do not know yet how
entrepreneurial perceptions and intentions evolve over time. As a
significant amount of time may elapse between the moment the intention
to start a business is formed and the moment the potential entrepreneur
initiate activities leading to venture creation, temporal stability is
crucial for the model to hold. The objective of this research is to fill
this important gap, through a longitudinal study of entrepreneurial
intentions and perceptions.
The first part of the paper describes and discusses the theoretical
approach selected for this study, namely that intention is a valid
predictor of planned behavior such as starting a business. The next
section situates the theoretical framework within the research context,
i.e. a group of university students on the point of selecting their
future careers. Research objectives are then exposed; they are firstly
to test whether the students' perceptions of the desirability and
feasibility of going into business have an impact on their intention to
become entrepreneurs and secondly, to verify if those perceptions and
intentions remain stable over time. The paper also describes the
research methodology used and presents the results from the first two
phases of this longitudinal study, along with some comments. It ends
with an overview of the forthcoming phase of the research.
THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS OF THE ENTREPRENEURIAL INTENTION APPROACH
The two main theory-driven models used by researchers having
adopted the entrepreneurial intentions approach to study the venture
creation phenomenon are those of Ajzen (1991), borrowed from social
psychology, and of Shapero and Sokol (1982). This latter was developed
specifically for the field of entrepreneurship.
According to Ajzen's Theory of Planned Behavior ("TPB"), any behavior that requires a certain amount of
planning, as is unquestionably the act of venture creation, can be
predicted by the intention to adopt that behavior (1991). Thus, it would
be possible to predict whether or not an individual will eventually
launch a business by studying his or her intention to do so. In the TPB,
three variables precede the formation of intention, which itself
predicts behavior: 1) the subject's attitude toward a given
behavior, 2) subjective norms, i.e. the subject's perception of
other people's opinions of the proposed behavior, and 3) the
subject's perception of his or her control over the behavior.
According to Shapero and Sokol (1982), the decision to change
direction significantly in life, for example by launching a business, is
precipitated by an event or a break in the established routine. The
person's choice will then depend on three elements, namely (1) his
or her perception of the desirability of the proposed behavior (a
combination of the first two variables in the preceding model), (2) his
or her propensity to act (i.e. to act in accordance with his or her
intentions), and (3) his or her perception of the behavior's
feasibility, which is similar, conceptually speaking, to the third
variable in the preceding model (see Figure 1).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
The beauty of these two theoretical models lies in their
simplicity. Both main constructs (perception of desirability and
perception of feasibility) are in fact the product of the combined
effects of several other variables studied in connection with the
venture creation phenomenon. For example, the attraction of the idea of
starting a business is probably dependent on the entrepreneurial models
an individual has in his or her immediate environment, the prestige and
respect ascribed to entrepreneurship as a career choice by the people
around the individual, the individual's need for achievement and
independence, the opportunities available in the environment, and so on.
But what is a theory worth if it cannot be verified? Generally
speaking, the empirical results have been convincing. A meta-analysis of
the findings of more than a hundred studies revealed an average
correlation of .65 for attitudes and intentions, and .46 for intentions
and behavior (Kim & Hunter, 1993). In a similar meta-analysis of the
findings of 185 studies carried out up to 1997, TPB accounted for 39% of
the variance in intention and for 27% of the variance in behavior
(Armitage & Conner, 2001). The behaviors observed for these analyses
included contraceptive use, engaging in physical exercise, giving blood,
breast-feeding a child, exercising voting rights and so on--in other
words, behaviors somewhat removed from the act of launching a business.
However, some interesting findings were also obtained for a behavior
similar to that of starting a business, namely growing a business
(Orser, Hogarth-Scott & Wright, 1998). In this latter study,
analysis of data collected from 139 small business owner-managers
suggested that the respondents' intention to grow their business
was a key factor in actual growth at the end of a four-year period.
The two models described above were also tested on the intention to
start a business, and here too the results were significant. Krueger,
Reilly and Carsrud tested and compared both models (2000): the model of
Shapero and Sokol was found to be slightly superior to that of Azjen
(adjusted [R.SUP.2] of .408 compared with .35) and in both cases the
results were statistically significant (p<.0001). Reitan (1996)
obtained results that were even more convincing with a model combining
both the above and including situational variables: this new model
explained 63% of the variance in the intention to start a business.
However, these models appear to have less explanatory power when
short-term intentions are used as the dependent variable: Reitan's
model only explained .30 of the variance in the intention to go into
business in the next two years, compared to .22 for Autio, Keeley,
Klofsten & Ulfstedt (1997) using a one year intention variable.
It is important to mention that all these studies were aimed at
explaining the formation of intention; they did not address the
relationship between intention and action. Yet, this is the most
important connection. As pointed out by Chrisman, "although the
relationship between intentions and subsequent venture creation has
considerable face validity and logic, it is not established that
intentions always, or even usually, lead to entrepreneurial
activity"(1999, p. 47). The few studies where this link was tested
produced mixed results. In the five-year period of Katz's study
(1989), only 10 out the 33 respondents who had the intention of seeking
self-employment at the beginning of the study actually did so at some
point. Two other studies showed a higher incidence of business start-ups
(Carter, Gartner et Reynolds 1996; Chrisman, 1999). However, in both
these cases respondents were in fact nascent entrepreneurs at the
beginning of the study, as they had already initiated activities
associated with starting a new firm (seek outside assistance, look for
facilities, develop a business plan, etc.). This means that they had
more than a strong intention to start a venture: they were engaged in
the process of making the transition from intention to actual behavior.
If it is not possible to verify the existence of a link between the
intention to start a business and the realization of that intention, the
models described above will remain only partially valid. We tend to
think that it may in fact be difficult to establish a statistically
significant association between intention and action for two reasons,
namely the level of control an individual has over the act of launching
a business, and the temporal stability of the intention to launch a
business.
As pointed out by Ajzen (1991), if perception of control is to be
used to predict behavior, there must be a good fit between that
perception and reality. For example, if an individual believes he or she
is perfectly capable of going into business and has mastered the process
when in fact he or she lacks the knowledge, skills or resources required
to succeed, the intention will probably not be realized. This gap
between perception and reality may lie in the individual's
evaluation of his or her own skills and competencies, or be external to
the individual, in an area over which the individual has little control
(e.g. access to a bank loan). It may also be a result of the
individual's lack of knowledge about what is required (time,
effort, resources, etc.) for the behavior to occur. In either cases,
such inaccurate perceptions of the feasibility to start a business will
weaken the predictive value of an intention-based model.
In the same line of thought, if the intention to launch a business
is likely to change over time and as a result of circumstances, an
intention formulated at time 1 will not predict action taken at time 2.
Indeed, the longer the time between formation of the intention and its
expected realization, the greater the chance that some new circumstance will occur to change the initial intention and invalidate the
prediction. This is recognized by Ajzen who stated that the temporal
stability of intentions is a condition for accurate behavioral
prediction (1991). Using the TPB model, Sheeran, Orbell & Trafimow
(1999) tested the hypothesis that the temporal stability of intentions
moderates the relationship between intentions and behavior. Their
results support this hypothesis: almost 20% more variance was accounted
for in the behavior of respondents with stable intentions, as opposed to
those with unstable intentions. Furthermore, the results indicate that
when a person has a positive intention to perform a behavior, the
stability of that intention is vital to successful performance. The
intention to go into business has never been proven to be stable,
indeed, some findings suggest that it may in fact be unstable (Audet,
2000; Katz, 1989). In Audet's study, students' perceptions of
the desirability and feasibility of going into business were measured
before and after they had taken a course on entrepreneurship, and some
significant differences were found. Given that perceptions form the
basis of intentions, it is reasonable to suppose that a change in
perception will also trigger a change in intention. In Katz's
study, only two of the 33 persons who indicated an intention to seek
self-employment in the first year of the study reported the same
intention one year later. The only other mention of entrepreneurial
intentions came from a single respondent in the fourth year of the
study.
RESEARCH QUESTIONS
In light of the foregoing, we felt it was essential to build on
current knowledge of the venture creation phenomenon by testing a model
based on the intention to go into business. More specifically, answers
to the following questions were sought:
* Are entrepreneurial intentions predicted by perceptions of the
desirability and feasibility of starting a business ?
* Do these perceptions remain stable over time ?
* Does the intention to start a business remain stable over time?
* What factors influence the temporal stability of the intention to
start a business?
The theoretical model selected is that developed by Shapero and
Sokol (1982), as reviewed by Krueger, Reilly & Carsrud (2000), with
one change, namely the omission of the "propensity to act"
variable.
METHODOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK
The project is scheduled to cover a three-year period, since the
research questions require longitudinal observation of the variables.
The research population is composed of university students from a
business administration degree program. The interest of this group lies
in the fact that, in terms of skills and knowledge, they appear to be
excellent candidates for venture creation. Moreover, when the project
started they were near the end of their studies and were expected to
have the time and energy available in the near future to plan a business
project. As research has shown that the entrepreneurial aspirations of
students are highly sensitive to the image of entrepreneurship as a
career path projected by the university community (Autio, Keeley,
Klofsten & Ulfstedt, 1997), it is important to understand their
entrepreneurial path and perhaps help and encourage them in the process.
The research specification provided for an initial data collection
process at the beginning of the first year, i.e. during the
respondents' last term at university ("Phase I"). The
purpose of this process was to measure the variables of the theoretical
model at time 1 ([t.sub.1]) and to test the first part of the model
(antecedents of intention). A second data collection process was carried
out in the second year, to measure the same variables at [t.sub.2], and
identify and explain any variations ("Phase II"). The third
and last data collection process will take place in the third year, for
the same purposes ("Phase III").
Phase I
The sample consisted of 107 third-year undergraduate business
students from Concordia University in Montreal, Canada. Data were
gathered using a questionnaire distributed in the classroom and over the
Internet. To avoid bias in the responses, the students were assured that
participation in the study would not affect their grades. Perception
variables were measured with a single item on a 1 to 5 scale. The
following questions were asked: "How appealing is the idea of one
day starting your own business?" (perception of desirability) and
"How confident are you in your skills and abilities to start a
business?" (perception of feasibility). Intentions were measured as
a % probability that the respondent would go into business in the
three-year period following graduation (short term) and at some point in
their life (long term).
Phase II
The research population for Phase II was the same as for Phase I.
However, the occupation of respondents changed: in the 18 months
separating Phase I and II, they graduated from university and obtained
full-time employment. The sample was much smaller than in the previous
phase; of the 107 initial respondents, only 54 were found and agreed to
participate to the study. This low retention rate is disappointing but
not all that surprising since young adults are known to be very mobile.
It is not a major concern as this smaller sample is nevertheless
representative of the larger sample, there being no significant
difference between these two groups when looking at the means of the
four variables measured at [t.sub.1]. Data was collected through
telephone interviews. All four variables were measured at [t.sub.2] and
in addition open-ended questions were asked to explain differences in
answers provided at [t.sub.1] and [t.sub.2]. Finally, respondents were
asked about their current work situation to investigate a possible
relationship between work satisfaction and short-term intentions to
start a business, as will be explained below.
PRESENTATION OF RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Phase I
When questioned, the respondents estimated the probability of
launching their own business in the next three years at 25 %, which is
fairly low (see Table 1 next page for descriptive statistics). Indeed,
only nine students out of 107 estimated this probability as being 75% or
more. Thus, very few students appeared to be considering an
entrepreneurial career in the short term. In the longer term, the
figures were more encouraging, with an average probability of 61%.
Nearly half the students (43%) estimated the probability of starting a
business at some time in the future at 75% or more. Thus, many students
appear to have a significant interest in the idea of becoming an
entrepreneur, but for most it is not something they feel they will do in
the immediate future. These results are in line with those obtained from
a sample of students enrolled in another Quebec university (Filion,
L'Heureux, Kadji-Youaleu & Bellavance, 2002) and from
university students in Russia and Norway (Kolvereid, 1996; Tkachev &
Kolvereid, 1999).
Perception of desirability and perception of feasibility variables
explained 49% of the variation in the long-term intention to start a
business, compared with just 32% in the case of short-term intentions.
The predictive power of both variables is fairly high (see Table 2 next
page). Not too surprisingly, these results confirm previous findings
(Autio, Keeley, Klofsten & Ulfstedt, 1997; Krueger, Reilly &
Carsrud, 2000; Reitan, 1996) to the effect that perceptions of the
desirability and feasibility of going into business explain the
formation of entrepreneurial intention to a significant degree.
Perception of desirability appears to be a stronger predictor than
perception of feasibility, for both long term and short term intentions,
this departing from Krueger's results but confirming Reitan's.
The model is more robust when the time frame is longer as opposed
to shorter. Autio, Keeley, Klofsten & Ulfstedt argue that adopting a
model using a strict short-term intention has both merits and
weaknesses. On the positive side, its power to predict venture creation
is probably higher than models using long-term intentions. However, the
occurrence of a strong intent to go into business within a short time
frame is relatively rare. Reitan explained his weaker results with a
model using short-term intentions by saying that certain contextual
variables excluded from his model (e.g. type of job) probably had a
greater impact in the short term than the variables included in the
model. For this reason and in light of Phase I results, it was decided
to collect data about the work situation of respondents in Phase II;
more precisely, the respondents' level of satisfaction with their
current jobs was measured on a 1 to 5 scale.
Phase II
A regression analysis confirmed the Phase I results for long-term
intentions (see Table 3 below) but not for short-term intentions.
Indeed, the two perception variables failed to contribute significantly
in the prediction of short-term intentions.
In an attempt to verify the explanation set forward by Reitan
(1996), the variable "work satisfaction" was added to the
regression model (see Table 4 below). This yielded disturbing results:
both perception of feasibility and work satisfaction proved
statistically significant in explaining short-term intentions, but not
perception of desirability. As might have been expected, correlation
between work satisfaction and entrepreneurial intentions was negative
([r.sup.2] of -.28), suggesting that some respondents reconsidered their
short-term intention to go into business because they enjoyed their
current jobs and were satisfied with their employment situation.
Interestingly, work satisfaction had no impact on the intention to start
a business in the distant future: when this variable was added to the
model, its p value was non significant at .35. We are thus lead to
believe that respondents' level of satisfaction with their current
jobs explains the probability that they will start a business in the
near future better than their perception of the desirability of going
into business.
There may be an alternate explanation for the fact that perceptions
of the desirability and feasibility of going into business fail to
explain the intention to start a business in the near future. Sutton
argues that one explanation for poor predictability by intention-based
models is violation of the principle of compatibility (1998). This
principle states that to maximize predictive power both the predictor
and the criterion should be measured at the same level of specificity in
terms of action, target, time and context. This principle is violated when measures of perceptions and intentions are not set in the same time
frame. For example, in this study respondents were asked about their
perception of the feasibility and desirability of starting their own
business at some point in their life to predict their intention to go
into business in the three-year period following their graduation.
Results would have probably been different, and more convincing, if
respondents had been asked about their perceptions toward starting a
business in the next three years. First, their level of work
satisfaction would have influenced their perception of the desirability
of starting a business in the short term and made this variable more
significant in explaining short-term intentions. Second, their
perception of the feasibility of starting a business in the near future
would have been a more accurate predictor of short-term intentions. To
verify this hypothesis, perceptions of desirability and feasibility will
be operationalized within the same time frame as intentions in Phase III
of the research.
Descriptive statistics for Phase II variables do not appear to
differ much from those at [t.sub.1] for the 54 respondents who
participated to both phases of data collection (see Table 1). A t-test
for equality of means and a paired samples test revealed no significant
difference between [t.sub.1] and [t.sub.2] means for all four variables.
For each of the two perception variables, approximately half the
respondents provided the same answer at [t.sub.2] as at [t.sub.1] and
the change in the probability of starting a business was greater than
20% for one third of the sample. Correlation coefficients for measures
of the same variable at [t.sub.1] and [t.sub.2] were relatively high for
perception of desirability ([r.sup.2]=.63), perception of feasibility
([r.sup.2]=.55) and long term intention ([r.sup.2]=.57) and slightly
lower for short term intention ([r.sup.2]=.35), suggesting less temporal
stability.
When looking at differences in frequencies between [t.sub.1] and
[t.sub.2], changes in perceptions were found across all score categories
(from 1 to 5). The trend appears to be that the perceptions of
respondents in the lower categories at [t.sub.1] shifted to higher
categories at [t.sub.2]. This indicates that, at [t.sub.2], more
respondents felt they had the skills and abilities to start a business
and regarded it as a desirable career option. However, it must be borne
in mind that this trend does not show when we look at the differences in
means. Such apparent changes in perceptions were associated with changes
in both short and long-term intentions of starting a business at
[t.sub.2]. Fewer respondents rated the probability of starting a
business in the short term at 50% and above, but more expressed such an
intention in the long run.
According to theory, respondents with a strong positive orientation
qualify as "potential entrepreneurs" in the sense that, if the
theory holds, they are the respondents with the highest probability of
starting a business. These would-be entrepreneurs are also the ones that
entrepreneurship scholars have been trying to identify a priori for
years. If one takes a closer look at the answers provided at [t.sub.1]
by students with a supposedly high entrepreneurial potential (%
probability of starting a business > or = 75%), it can be seen that
their short-term intentions changed drastically. Indeed, at [t.sub.2]
only one respondent out of four still intended to start a business
within the three-year period following graduation (see Table 5). The
other two respondents who stated a high short-term probability of
starting a business at [t.sub.2] had indicated probabilities of 65% and
5% (!) at [t.sub.1]. On a long term horizon, intentions are slightly
more stable. Of the 21 respondents who, at [t.sub.1], had strong
intentions of starting a business at some point in their life, only six
had changed their minds to a significant degree at [t.sub.2] (>25%
change in probability). On the other hand, among those who did not have
a strong intent at [t.sub.1] but had one at [t.sub.2], 5 out of 6
respondents expressed a change in intention greater than 25%. In fact,
the average increase in the probability of starting a business between
[t.sub.1] and [t.sub.2] for these respondents was 37.5%, which is a
change of rather large magnitude. Furthermore, two of these six
respondents had expressed a very low probability of starting a business
at [t.sub.1] (respectively 5% and 20%). This means that these
"potential entrepreneurs" at [t.sub.2] would have never been
identified as such at [t.sub.1] if an intention-based model had been
used. There is thus a risk of missing real aspiring entrepreneurs when
using such a model. Katz goes as far as to say that "studying those
who intend to enter business ownership would result in missing over 98%
of those who become business owners." (1989, p.530)
In light of the above, it is difficult to evaluate with much
certainty the temporal stability of the intention to start a business
and of the perceptions that form such intention. What is the range
within which an intention can change over time without being labeled
"unstable" ? Is a 10% or 20% change in the probability of
starting a business indicative of temporal unstability ? Would a change
from a probability of going into business of 85% to 75% affect the
likelihood that the behavior will occur as much as a change from 75 to
85% ? The answer to these questions is highly consequential as
respondents with stable strong positive intentions are considerably more
likely to perform the predicted behavior than those with positive but
unstable intentions (Sheeran, Orbell & Trafimow, 1999). There is an
urgent need to clarify the notion of temporal stability as applied to
the intention of starting a business.
Identifying the factors and events that are most likely to impact
on the intentions of respondents may help us understand how these
intentions change over time. Among the reasons given by respondents to
explain changes in their intentions or perceptions were:
Positive change
* Being more mature, having more work experience, this having an
effect on their self-confidence;
* Money: some realized that their earning potential was limited
when working as an employee and they want more, much more.
* Freedom: some resent the constraints of the workplace (rules,
fixed schedule, being supervised, fixed salary, etc.);
* Opportunity recognition: being active in the workplace puts
respondents in a better position to identify and recognize business
opportunities than when they were students;
* Being your own boss: some long for the satisfaction of working
for themselves, of having something left for themselves at the end of
the day.
Negative change
* Reality shock: some realized that they knew far less than they
thought and that the process of starting a business was not as easy as
they had imagined (this is a typical case of mismatch between
perceptions of feasibility and reality);
* Corporate orientation: some realized they enjoy employee status
and the corporate environment (stability, less risk, good pay, etc).
One fact worth emphasizing is that the respondents had gone through
a very major change in their lives between [t.sub.1] and [t.sub.2]: they
went from being students to having their first full-time job in their
field of expertise. As a result, their income level changed as did their
social environment, their status in society, their place of residence
(for most), their legal status (for some), and so on. Clearly, changes
of this magnitude cannot be expected to occur again in the next several
years. It will thus be extremely interesting to see if their intentions
continue to change from [t.sub.2] to t3, despite the fact that they have
entered a period of relative stability in their life paths.
LOOKING TO THE FUTURE
Data gathered in the first two phases of the project confirmed
results from previous studies: perceptions of the desirability and
feasibility of starting a business significantly explain the intention
to go into business at some point in life. However, the model does not
hold as well when the time frame is shorter. Indeed, at [t.sub.2] both
perceptions failed to explain to a significant degree the intention to
go into business in the next three years. When work satisfaction is
added to the regression model, both this variable and perception of
feasibility become significant predictors of short term intentions,
perception of desirability remaining non significant. The major
contribution of this research so far is to provide longitudinal measures
of the variables forming an intention-based model of venture creation.
Our results tend to indicate that entrepreneurial intentions and
perceptions vary over time. As the temporal stability of intentions is a
condition for an intention-based model to accurately predict behavior,
the link between entrepreneurial intentions and actual venture creation
may prove difficult to establish. As pointed out by Niittykangas and
Laukkanen, expressed readiness for an entrepreneurial career can
probably "serve as a useful indicator and a starting point for
officials making decisions about whether or not to proceed further with
a prospective self-employer. (...) Importantly, entrepreneurial
potential cannot be assumed to be confined to such persons
only"(1998, page 61). Data collected in future phases of the
project should shed more light on the temporal stability of intentions
and on the process by which intentions are transformed into action.
REFERENCES
Ajzen, I. (1991). The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.
Armitage, C. & M. Conner (2001). Efficacy of the Theory of
Planned Behavior: A Meta-analytic Review. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 40, 471-499.
Audet, J. (2000). Evaluation of Two Approaches to Entrepreneurship
Education Using an Intention-based Model of Venture Creation. Academy of
Entrepreneurship Journal, 6(1), 57-63.
Autio, E., R. Keeley, M. Klofsten & T. Ulfstedt (1997).
Entrepreneurial Intent among Students: Testing an Intent Model in Asia,
Scandinavia and in the USA.. Frontiers of Entrepreneurship Research,
Wellesley, MA: Babson College.
Carter, N., W.B. Gartner & P.D. Reynolds (1996). Exploring
start-up event sequences. Journal of Business Venturing, 11, 151-166.
Chrisman, J.J. (1999). The influence of outsider-generated
knowledge resources on venture creation. Journal of Small Business
Management, 37(4), 42-58.
Filion, L.J., L'Heureux, D., Kadji-Youaleu, C. & F.
Bellavance (2002). L'entrepreneuriat comme carriere
potentielle--Une evaluation en milieu universitaire, Chaire
d'Entrepreneurship Maclean Hunter, Cahier de recherche HEC 2002-04.
Katz, J.A. (1989) Intentions, Hurdles, and Start-ups: An Analysis
of Entrepreneurial Follow-through.. Frontiers of Entrepreneurship
Research, Wellesley, MA: Babson College.
Kim, M.S. & J. E. Hunter (1993). Relationships among Attitudes,
Behavioral Intentions, and Behaviors: A Meta-analysis of Past Research.
Communication Research, 20, 331-364.
Kolvereid, L. (1996). Prediction of employment status choice
intentions, Entrepreneurship, Theory & Practice, 21(1), 47-57.
Krueger, N. F., M. D. Reilly & A.L. Carsrud (2000). Competing
Models of Entrepreneurial Intentions. Journal of Business Venturing,15,
411-432.
Niittykangas, H. & M. Laukkanen (1998). Potential
entrepreneurs--Can they be located ?. International Journal of
Entrepreneurship, 2, 47-65.
Orser, B.J., S. Hogarth-Scott & P. Wright (1998). On the Growth
of Small Enterprises: The Role of Intentions, Gender and Experience.
Frontiers of Entrepreneurship Research, Wellesley, MA: Babson College.
Reitan, B. (1996). Entrepreneurial Intentions--A Combined Models
Approach. Paper presented at the 9th Nordic Small Business Research
Conference, Lillehammer, Norway.
Shapero, A. & L. Sokol (1982).The Social Dimensions of
Entrepreneurship. In The Encyclopedia of Entrepreneurship,. C. Kent, D.
Sexton & K. Vesper (Eds.), Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Sheeran, P., S.Orbell & D.Trafimow (1999). Does the Temporal
Stability of Behavioral Intentions Moderate Intention-behavior and Past
Behavior-Future Behavior Relations?. Personnality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 25(6), 721-730.
Sutton S. (1998). Predicting and Explaining Intentions and
Behavior: How Well Are We Doing?. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
28(15), 1317-1338.
Tkachev, A. & L. Kolvereid (1999). Self-employment intentions
among Russian students. Entrepreneurship & Regional Development,
11(3), 269-280.
Josee Audet, Universite Laval
Table 1: Mean scores of perception, intention and work satisfaction
variables at [t.sub.1] and [t.sub.2]
std. std.
Variable [t.sub.1] dev. [t.sub.2] dev.
n=107 n=54
Perception of desirability 3.51 1.08 3.56 1.11
(1= low, 5= high)
Perception of feasibility 3.39 0.87 3.57 0.92
(1= low, 5= high)
Intention to start a business 25.46% 26.76 22.61% 25.40
<3 yrs (probability as a %)
Intention to start a business 60.73% 27.63 64.07% 22.51
>3 yrs (probability as a %)
Work satisfaction not available 3.57 0.92
(1= low, 5= high)
Table 2: Summary of Regression Analyses Results at [t.sub.1]
Short-term Long-term
intention at intention at
Variables [t.sub.1] [t.sub.1]
t value p t value p
Perception of desirability 3.28 .0014 6.29 <.0001
Perception of feasibility 2.89 .0047 2.21 .0292
Adjusted Adjusted
[R.sup.2] = .32 [R.sup.2] = .49
p=<.0001 p=<.0001
Table 3: Summary of Regression Analyses Results at [t.sub.2]
Short-term Long-term
intention at intention at
Variables [t.sub.2] [t.sub.2]
t value p t value p
Perception of desirability 1.68 .0985 4.04 .0002
Perception of feasibility 1.44 .1560 2.89 .0056
Adjusted Adjusted
[R.sup.2] = .13 [R.sup.2] = .47
p=.01 p=<.0001
Table 4: Summary of Regression Analyses Results at [t.sub.2] with work
satisfaction added to the model
Short-term Long-term
intention at intention at
Variables [t.sub.2] [t.sub.2]
t value p t value p
Perception of desirability 1.16 .2541 3.69 .0006
Perception of feasibility 2.39 .0212 3.32 .0018
Work satisfaction -2.52 .0155 -0.94 .3541
Adjusted Adjusted
[R.sup.2] = .23 [R.sup.2] = .48
p=.0021 p=<.0001
Table 5: Variation in probabilities of starting a business among
respondents with a strong short term intent at [t.sub.1] or [t.sub.2]
Probability Probability Change in
at [t.sub.1] at [t.sub.2] probability
Respondents with a strong 75% 25% (50%)
intent at [t.sub.1] (n=4)
75% 20% (55%)
99% 100% 1%
100% 0% 100%
Respondents with a strong 5% 95% 90%
intent at [t.sub.2] only 65% 90% 25%
(n=2)