Global entrepreneurship, income, and work norms: a seven country study.
Carraher, Shawn M. ; Carraher, Sarah C. ; Whitely, William T. 等
ABSTRACT
The subjects used for this study consist of 6,307 individuals from
7 countries examining the influence that work norms have on actual
income received across 4 occupational groups--Entrepreneurs, Engineers,
Educators, and White Collar Employees--and the general population.
Differential prediction is found across the occupational groups with
entrepreneurs found to be most similar to teachers--but still having a
unique structure of the relationship between work norms and income.
Directions for future research are suggested.
INTRODUCTION
Compensation has long been a topic of interest to employees and
employers alike. In fact, the use of compensation as a motivator has
been traced to antiquity (Peach & Wren, 1992). The concept of an
employment relationship implies that employees work in exchange for some
reward, and this reward is often monetary compensation (Brockner, 2002;
Janssen, 2001). Thus, pay satisfaction has emerged as a popular variable
for use in organizational research (for reviews, see Heneman, 1985;
Heneman & Schwab, 1979; Miceli & Lane, 1991; Rynes &
Gerhart, 2000). Pay satisfaction exhibits significant relationships with
organizationally important outcomes such as absenteeism (Weiner, 1980),
turnover intentions (Hom & Kinicki, 2001; Steel, Griffith, &
Horn, 2002), organizational citizenship behaviors (Lambert, 2000) and
job performance (Werner & Mero, 1999).
As noted by Rice, Phillips, and McFarlin (1990), one of the most
intriguing findings with respect to pay satisfaction is the modest
strength of the relationship between how much an individual is actually
paid and that individual's pay satisfaction. Although this
relationship typically has been positive and statistically significant,
it has generally explained well under 25% of the variance in pay
satisfaction. These findings have led others to examine the prediction
of pay satisfaction based upon multiple discrepancies or multiple
monetary standards of comparison for the individual employee (Law &
Wong, 1998) and demographic and psychological variables (Berkowitz,
Fraser, Treasure, & Cochran, 1987).
Scholars have noted that comparatively little research advances
models of pay and their predictors (Heneman, 1985; Miceli & Lane,
1991; Opsahl & Dunnette, 1966; Rynes & Gerhart, 2000; Williams
& Brower, 1996). This could be due to the assertions of some
researchers that it is clearly "too early to offer a precise
theoretical model of the determinants of income satisfaction"
(Berkowitz et al., 1987, p. 546), yet such model development is still
needed (Rynes & Gerhart, 2000). This is especially important in the
area of individual entrepreneurship as little work has been done
examining the compensation practices of entrepreneurial
organizations--or for entrepreneurs themselves (Buckley, Carrraher,
Ferris, & Carraher, 2001; Parnell, Carraher, & Odom,
2000)--especially across cultures (Box, Beisel, & Watts, 1995;
Parnell, Crandall, & Menefee, 1995).
How do people experience work, and how are these experiences linked
to the economic outcomes of work? The major intellectual traditions
within the study of work have been summarized by Dubin (1976). An
examination of within-culture, and cross-cultural studies of why
individual's work results in identifying six different central
variables--or work norms--which may explain why people work. The first
of these is the concept of Work Centrality (Lawler & Hall 1970; MOW
international work team 1987; Zedeck, 1992). Work centrality is the
general belief about the importance of working within one's life.
It can be thought of as the degree to which work is seen as the most
important variable for developing one's self-concept or self-image
(Super, 1953; 1957; Super et al., 1967).
The second work norm is the view of work as an Obligation to
one's employer, society, and/or family--with the obverse of this
being work as an entitlement (Erickson & Vallas, 1990; Etzioni,
1961; Kohlberg, 1963; Piaget, 1965; Zedeck, 1992). An individual with
this work norm believes that he or she works because they ought to
contribute to society (or the employer, or the family, etc.) through
work. In essence, the obligation to work norm represents an
individual's belief that he or she has a responsibility or duty to
contribute to social units.
The third work norm is intrinsic satisfaction (basically being
interested in the work itself) or expected intrinsic rewards from
working (Hall, 1986; McGregor, 1960). It has been reported that across
countries and cultures (with the sole exception of the Far East), next
to providing economic support, perceived intrinsic rewards are seen as
the most important reasons for working.
The fourth work norm is the interpersonal or Social work norm
(Carraher, Buckley, Scott, Parnell & Carraher, 2002; Carraher,
Mendoza, Ishaq, Buckley, Carraher & Kerley, 1996, Erikson &
Vallas, 1990; Hall, 1986). Humans tend to be social beings and therefore
many work in order to have increased opportunities to interact with
others.
The fifth major work norm to be explored in this work, which may
seem obvious, is that work is performed to produce income necessary for
survival (Hall, 1986; Maslow, 1954; Zedeck, 1992)--which we call the
Importance of Pay work norm. A study examining the levels of importance
placed on the basic functions of work has found that across eight
culturally diverse countries over 60% of all individuals identify this
as the most important reason for working (MOW international work team,
1987).
The final major work norm is the entitlement work norm. The
entitlement work norm relates to whether one feels that they are
entitled to employment and a good job. Based upon equity theories, it
has been found (Jaques, 1961) that some classes of employees may believe
that they are entitled to a particular level of work or income due to
personal characteristics such as education, family background, and/or
previous work experiences.
RESEARCH PURPOSE
The purpose of the present study is to examine the influence that
work norms may have on actual income received globally comparing
entrepreneurs to three other professional occupational groups and within
the general population.
METHODS
Sample Characteristics
The research design is based upon work published by Carraher and
Whitely (1998) using scales from the international Meaning of Work and
the actual incomes of respondents. Specifically results from 6,307
individuals from the U.S., Japan, Germany, Israel, the Netherlands,
Belgium, and Yugoslavia are used to examine differences between the
influence of work norms on actual income for entrepreneurs, three
professional occupational groups, and the general population.
Information about each of the scales and the processes used to develop
and administer the scales are provided in The Meaning of Work (Mow
international research team, 1987).
RESULTS, DISCUSSION, & CONCLUSIONS
As can be seen in Table 1, entrepreneurs tend to earn more if they
work because they enjoy their work or because they have a sense of
obligation to contribute to their family, their community, and/or
society at large. However, we find that there is no relationship between
the importance placed upon working for pay, their desire for social
interaction, and their feelings of entitlement and an individual
entrepreneur's actual level of pay.
Turning to Tables 2, 3, 4 and 5; we find that for engineers they
earn more if they have a sense of obligation to contribute through their
work while they earn less if they work because they believe that they
are entitled to a good job, out of the desire for social interaction,
and in order to make money. Educators, on the other hand, are most
similar to entrepreneurs in that they earn more if they have a sense of
obligation to contribute through their work and/or if they have
intrinsic satisfaction in their work--however, they also make
significantly less if their major work norm is to have social
interactions through their work.
It is also possible to explain almost twice the variance in the
actual incomes of educators than for the entrepreneurs based upon the
work norms (18.9% vs 10%)--likely due to the fact that many more factors
such as performance and industry can influence the incomes of
entrepreneurs. White collar employees then seem to earn more if they
feel that they have a sense of obligation to contribute through their
work but earn less if they work because they want to earn more money or
for social reasons. In the general population all of the work norms are
related to actual income--with the exception of Work Centrality.
Entitlement, Social, and Pay all have a negative influence on actual
income received while Obligation and intrinsic satisfaction both had a
positive influence on actual income received.
Based upon the results of our current study, future research should
explore what other individual attributes might differentially influence
actual pay levels across occupations in a differential manner with
particular attention paid to what it is that differentiates
entrepreneurs from nonentrepreneurs. For instance based upon research
performed in the United States, we believe that it is likely that
individual differences in an individual's ability to resist
organizational stress as it influences their overall coping
abilities--which could influence how well employees are able to adapt to
highly stressful, but higher paying, employment (Shalit, 1977; Sullivan
& Bhagat, 1992). Additionally, given that it has been found that
individuals with different cognitive styles also have differing
preferred styles and modes of observing their environment (Myers &
McCaulley, 1985; Carraher, 1993), we believe that it is possible that
dominant cognitive type or preferred learned style also could
potentially influence an individual's dominant work motive, their
actual pay levels, and their choice of occupation. It is also likely
that individual differences in cognitive complexity, cognitive
differentiation, and/or general intelligence might be differentially
predictive of actual salaries received across cultures (Carraher &
Buckley, 1996; Lance & Scarpello, 1989). While these relationships
may be valid across occupations it might be interesting to examine these
variables between occupations.
Alternatively, it is also possible that differences in age,
organizational tenure, job tenure (Goodwin, 1991), compensation systems
(seniority vs merit systems; Li,1985), organizational hierarchical
levels (Cotton & McKenna, 1994; Goodwin, 1991) and/or work and team
structures (job rotation vs no job rotation, etc.; Cotton 1977,
Hollenbeck, LePine, and Ilgen 1996, McCarthy 1989) may be capable of
explaining individual differences in pay within cultures and/or
occupations--but they may also differentially influence actual pay
levels across cultures and occupations (Jaques, 1961). It may also be
useful for future research to examining what other work-related outcomes
might be influenced by individual work motives. For instance, do
individuals with a strong Social work norm tend to cluster within a
limited number of industries and/or occupations regardless of culture?
Further, how are individual's reasons for working developed across
cultures and occupations? Are there similar patterns of development
across cultures and occupations or are there even similar developmental
patterns based upon life experiences across cultures and occupations?
Putting pay of the other side of the prediction equations, it may then
be asked if higher pay tends to lead to greater job mobility (DiPrete
& Nonnemaker, 1997; Wilk & Sackett, 1996) and occupational
satisfaction (Carson, Carson & Phillips, 1996; Judge, Cable &
Boudreau, 1995).
It would be interesting to examine questions such as these using
multi-trait/multi-method data and/or multi-sample confirmatory factor
analysis in order to determine whether the results are sample specific
and also whether the results are generalizable across samples. With
multi-sample confirmatory factor analysis it is important the remember
the influence that the number of samples included has on the statistical
results. It is generally preferable to have 3 to 6 samples per
multi-sample confirmatory factor analysis (Carraher & Buckley,
1996). Should one desire to examine more than 8 samples then it would be
preferable to perform two or more separate analyses or to randomly
choose samples and then run a single analysis with no more than 6
samples. Due to the potential of increasing the likelihood that results
due to chance occurrences could be observed with multiple multi-sample
confirmatory factor analyses, it is the second of these suggestions that
would be more preferable.
Another interesting area for study suggested by Scarpello and
Carraher (1997) is whether entrepreneurial orientation influences the
relationship between reasons for working and actual pay
received--especially within professional level employees and
entrepreneurs. They have suggested that the relationship between reasons
for working and the actual pay received should be strongest for
self-employed individuals and weakest for those in industrialized,
unionized settings where workers may have little real influence on their
actual wages. We also believe that more work should be done examining
the sense of obligation to make a contribution through ones work as this
was found to be such a power influence on actual income received in this
study.
It is hoped that this short study can help guide future researchers
in the examination of the importance of how ones reasons for working can
influence the outcomes received from working across cultures within
occupations. With this study we have sought to help create a better
understanding in the relationship between reasons for working and actual
pay levels received--and hopefully assist help future researchers to
develop models about the influence of work norms on actual pay received
within occupations. It appears that in professional occupations that the
stronger ones sense of their obligation to make a contribution through
their job the more that they earn. Additionally, it appears that
cross-occupationally, the stronger ones desire for social interactions
from work, the less will be ones pay and that while the relationship
between individual work norms and income is most similar for the
entrepreneurs and the teachers that the entrepreneurs still have a
unique identity among the occupational groups.
REFERENCES
Berkowitz, L., Fraser, C., Treasure, F. & Cochran, S. (1987).
Pay, equity, job gratifications, and comparisons in pay satisfaction.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 544-551.
Box, T., Beisel, J. & L. Watts. (1995). Thai entrepreneurs: An
empirical investigation of individual differences, background and
scanning behaviors. Academy of Entrepreneurship Journal, 1(1), 18-25.
Brockner, J. (2002). Making sense of procedural fairness: How high
procedural fairness can reduce or heighten the influence of outcome
favorability. Academy of Management Review, 27 (1). 58-78.
Buckley, M., Carraher, S., Ferris, G. & C. Carraher. (2001).
Human resource concerns in international multinational high technology
firms. Journal of Applied Management & Entrepreneurship, 6, 97-104.
Carraher, S.M. (1993). Another look at the dimensionality of a
learning style inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53,
411-415.
Carraher, S.M. & M. Buckley. (1996). Cognitive complexity and
the perceived dimensionality of pay satisfaction. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 81(1), 102-109.
Carraher, S., Buckley, M., Scott, C., Parnell, J. & C.
Carraher. (2002). Customer service selection in a global entrepreneurial
information services organization. Journal of Applied Management &
Entrepreneurship, 7(1), 45-55.
Carraher, S., Mendoza, J., Ishaq, M., Buckley, M., Carraher, C.
& J. Kerley. (1996). Validation of an instrument to measure
service-orientation. Midwest Academy of Management Proceedings,
Southbend, IN..
Carraher, S., Mendoza, J. & A. McBride. (1995). Identifying
service-oriented individuals with biodata. SouthWest Academy of
Management Proceedings, Houston, 114-117.
Carson, K.D., Carson, P. & J. Phillips. (1996). A career
entrenchment model: Theoretical development and empirical outcomes.
Journal of Career Development, 22, 273-286.
Cotton, C. (1977). Marginality--a neglected dimension in the design
of work. Academy of Management Review, 2, 133-138.
Cotton, C. & J. McKenna. (1994). Gender and occupational level
differences in union voting behavior. Human Relations, 47, 233-242.
DiPrete, T. & K. Nonnemaker. (1997). Structural change, labor
market turbulence, and labor market outcomes. American Sociological
Review, 62, 386-404.
Dubin, R. (1976). Work in modern society. In R. Dubin (Ed.).
Handbook of work, organization and society. Chicago: Rand McNally, 5-35.
Erickson, K. & S. Vallas. (1990). The nature of work. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Etzioni, A. (1961). A comparative analysis of complex
organizations. New York: The Free Press of Glencoe.
Goodwin, V. (1991). Antecedents, consequences, and covariates of
organizational cognitive complexity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
University of Texas-Arlington.
Hall, R. H. (1986). Dimensions of work. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Heneman, H.G. (1985). Pay satisfaction. Research in Personnel and
Human Resources Management, 3, 115-139.
Heneman, H.G. & Schwab, D.P. (1979). Work and rewards theory.
In D. Yoder & H.G. Heneman (Eds.), ASPA Handbook of Personnel and
Industrial Relations. Washington, D.C.: Bureau of National Affairs,
6.1-6.22
Hollenbeck, J., LePine, J. & D. Ilgen. (1996). Adapting to
roles in decision-making teams. In K. Murphy (Ed.) Individual
differences and behavior in organizations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
300-333.
Hom, P. & A. Kinicki. (2001). Toward a greater understanding of
how dissatisfaction drives employee turnover. Academy of Management
Journal, 44(4), 975-989.
Janssen, O. (2001). Fairness perceptions as a moderator in the
curvilinear relationships between job demands, and job performance and
job satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 44(5), 1039-1053.
Jaques, E. (1961). Equitable payment. New York: Wiley.
Judge, T., Cable, D. & J. Boudreau. (1995). An empirical
investigation of the predictors of executive career success. Personnel
Psychology, 48, 485-519.
Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children's
orientations toward moral order: I. Sequence in the development of human
thought. Vita Humana, 6, 11-33.
Lambert, S. (2000). Added benefits: The link between work-life
benefits and organizational citizenship behavior. Academy of Management
Journal, 43(5), 801-817.
Lance, C.E. & V. Scarpello. (1989). Measurement and
dimensionality of compensation satisfaction: A reexamination. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Industrial and
Organizational Psychology, Boston.
Law, K. & C. Wong. (1998). Toward a taxonomy of
multidimensional constructions. Academy of Management Review, 23(4),
741-757.
Lawler, E. & D. Hall. (1970). Relationships of job
characteristics to job involvement, satisfaction, and intrinsic
motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 54, 305-312.
Li, Y. (1985). The effect of pay incentive plan on pay
satisfaction. Unpublished masters thesis. University of Kansas.
Locke, E., Feren, D.B., McCaleb, V.M., Shaw, K.N. & A. Denny.
(1980). The relative effectiveness of four measures of motivating
employee performance. In K.D. Duncan, M.M. Gruneberg, & D. Wallis
(Eds.). Changes in working life. London: Wiley Ltd., 363-383.
Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper
& Row.
McBride, A., Mendoza, J. & S. Carraher. (1993). The development
of a biodata instrument to measure service orientation. Midwest Academy
of Management Proceedings, Indianapolis, 8-13.
McCarthy, M. (1989). Burnout: What price care giving? Loss, Grief,
and Care, 3, 67-71.
McGregor, D. (1960). The human side of enterprise. New York:
McGraw-Hill Book Co.
Miceli, M. & Lane, M.C. (1991). Antecedents of pay
satisfaction: A review and extension. In G.R. Ferris & K.M. Rowland
(Eds.), Research in personnel and human resources management, Vol. 9
(pp. 235-309). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
MOW international research team. (1987). The meaning of working.
London: Academic Press.
Myers, I. & M. McCaulley. (1985). A guide to the development
and use of the Myers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Opsahl, R.L. & Dunnette, M.D. (1966). The role of financial
compensation in industrial motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 66,
94-118.
Parnell, J., Carraher, S. & R. Odom. (2000). Strategy and
performance in the entrepreneurial computer software industry. Journal
of Business & Entrepreneurship, 12(3) 49-66.
Parnell, J., Crandall, W. & M. Menefee. (1995). Examining the
impact of culture on entrepreneurial propensity: An empirical study of
prospective American and Egyptian entrepreneurs. Academy of
Entrepreneurship Journal, 1(1), 39-52.
Peach, E. & Wren, D. (1992). Pay for performance from antiquity
to the 1950's. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 12,
5-26.
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral development of the child. Glencoe, Il:
The Free Press.
Rice, R.W., Phillips, S.M. & McFarlin, D.B. (1990). Multiple
discrepancies and pay satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75,
386-393.
Rynes, S. & Gerhart, B. (2000). Compensation in organizations:
Current research and practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Scarpello, V. & S. Carraher. (1997). Pay satisfaction and pay
fairness are they the same construct? Southwest Academy of Management
Proceedings, New Orleans, 69-73.
Shalit, B. (1977). Structural ambiguity and limits to coping.
Journal of Human Stress, 3(4), 32-45.
Steel, R., Griffeth, R. & P. Horn. (2002). Practical retention
policy for the practical manager. Academy of Management Executive,
16(2), 149-166.
Sullivan, S. & R. Bhagat. (1992). Organizational stress, job
satisfaction and job performance: Where do we go from here? Journal of
Management, 18, 353-374.
Super, D. (1953). A theory of vocational development. American
Psychologist, 8, 185-190.
Super, D. (1957). The psychology of careers. New York: Harper and
Brothers.
Super, D., Crites, J., Hummel, R., Moser, H., Overstreet, P. &
C. Warnath. (1967). Vocational development. New York: Bureau of
Publications.
Weiner, N. (1980). Determinants and behavioral consequences of pay
satisfaction: A comparison of two models. Personnel Psychology, 33,
741-757.
Wilk, S. & P. Sackett. (1996). Longitudinal analysis of
ability-job complexity fit and job change. Personnel Psychology, 49,
937-967.
Williams, M.L. & H. Brower. (1996). A comparative model and
measure of pay satisfaction. Paper presented at the annual meeting of
the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA.
Zedeck, S. (1992). Work, families, and organizations. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass
Shawn M. Carraher, Texas A & M University--Commerce
Sarah C. Carraher, Texas A & M University--Commerce
William T. Whitely, University of Oklahoma
Table 1: Entrepreneurs
Multiple R .3164
R Square .1001
Adjusted R Square .0886
Standard Error 1089.29577
Analysis of Variance DF Sum of Squares Mean Square
Regression 6 62148878.60050 10358146.43342
Residual 471 558872243.56059 1186565.27295
F 8.72952 Signif F .00001
Variable B SE B Beta
Entitlement 81.470506 103.994449 .035105
Work Central 2.799370 6.014815 .021635
Social 9.111768 7.057538 .059310
Intrinsic Sat. 28.581235 9.725662 .137337
Obligation 608.612670 95.292801 .296230
Pay 10.939088 6.954566 .074638
(Constant) 3300.874872 957.466948
Variable T Sig T
Entitlement .783 .4338
Work Central .465 .6419
Social 1.291 .1973
Intrinsic Sat. 2.939 .0035
Obligation 6.387 .0001
Pay 1.573 .1164
(Constant) 3.448 .0006
Table 2: Engineers
Multiple R .33123
R Square .10972
Adjusted R Square .09808
Standard Error 1469.53241
Analysis of Variance DF Sum of Squares Mean Square
Regression 6 122154332.68065 20359055.44678
Residual 459 991222202.40089 2159525.49543
F 9.42756 Signif F .00001
Variable B SE B Beta
Entitlement 350.036206 147.964315 .10547
Work Central 3.657807 10.329310 .01644
Social 48.309775 12.477399 .18045
Intr. Sat 20.470233 15.470690 .06133
Obligation 696.209536 161.980409 .19775
PAY 39.120082 10.916012 .17284
(Constant) 3764.239268 1614.704223
Variable T Sig T
Entitlement 2.366 .0184
Work Central .354 .7234
Social 3.872 .0001
Intr. Sat 1.323 .1864
Obligation 4.298 .0001
PAY 3.584 .0004
(Constant) 2.331 .0202
Table 3: Teachers
Multiple R .43429
R Square .18861
Adjusted R Square .17976
Standard Error 686.75299
Analysis of Variance DF Sum of Squares Mean Square
Regression 6 60296974.79721 10049495.7995
Residual 550 259396317.66598 471629.6685
F 21.30802 Signif F .0000001
Variable B SE B Beta
Entitlement 20.156316 73.654468 .010687
Work Central. 1.867839 4.482575 .016620
Pay 1.253011 4.486225 .011493
Obligation 490.716044 58.827645 .333750
Intrinsic Sat. 16.695508 6.431315 .102937
Social 23.461882 4.760499 .202633
(Constant) 303.364626 638.283586
Variable T Sig T
Entitlement .274 .7844
Work Central. .417 .6771
Pay .279 .7801
Obligation 8.342 .0001
Intrinsic Sat. 2.596 .0097
Social 4.928 .0001
(Constant) .475 .6348
Table 4: White Collar Employees
Multiple R .35235
R Square .12415
Adjusted R Square .11535
Standard Error 799.18807
Analysis of Variance DF Sum of Squares Mean Square
Regression 6 54050631.49116 9008438.58186
Residual 597 381304841.69262 638701.57737
F 14.10430 Signif F .00001
Variable B SE B Beta
Entitlement 54.172880 81.712677 .025962
Pay 22.718926 4.562652 .199990
Work Central 0.443415 4.293434 .004123
Intrinsic Sat. 7.061407 6.429295 .042636
Obligation 391.851106 70.589754 .224546
Social 21.221300 5.101138 .168973
(Constant) 1623.437679 646.180621
Variable T Sig T
Entitlement .663 .5076
Pay 4.979 .0001
Work Central .103 .9178
Intrinsic Sat. 1.098 .2725
Obligation 5.551 .0001
Social 4.160 .0001
(Constant) 2.512 .0123
Table 5: General Population
Multiple R .30753
R Square .09458
Adjusted R Square .09334
Standard Error 962.76953
Analysis of Variance DF Sum of Squares Mean Square
Regression 6 425538874.41492 70923145.73582
Residual 4395 4073836147.70867 926925.17582
F 76.51442 Signif F .0000001
Variable B SE B Beta
Entitlement 144.431372 33.317761 .062789
Work Central 2.461696 1.895356 .019375
Pay 10.753453 1.999066 .084491
Obligation 439.807354 29.717750 .220308
Int. Sat. 16.325154 2.796920 .088444
Social 21.326386 2.275713 .143647
(Constant) 528.551043 293.674612
Variable T Sig T
Entitlement 4.335 .0001
Work Central 1.299 .1941
Pay 5.379 .0001
Obligation 14.799 .0001
Int. Sat. 5.837 .0001
Social 9.371 .0001
(Constant) 1.800 .0720