首页    期刊浏览 2024年11月24日 星期日
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:An empirical examination of the relationship of personality and the propensity for self-venturing.
  • 作者:Gaulden, Corbett ; Jackson, William T. ; Gaster, Walter
  • 期刊名称:Academy of Entrepreneurship Journal
  • 印刷版ISSN:1087-9595
  • 出版年度:2002
  • 期号:January
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:The DreamCatchers Group, LLC
  • 关键词:Businesspeople;Entrepreneurs;Entrepreneurship

An empirical examination of the relationship of personality and the propensity for self-venturing.


Gaulden, Corbett ; Jackson, William T. ; Gaster, Walter 等


Like human beings, firms are constantly being born that cannot live. Others may meet...death from accident or illness. Still others die a "natural" death, as men die of old age. And the "natural" cause, in the case of firms, is precisely their inability to keep up the pace in innovating which they themselves had been instrumental in setting in the time of their vigor.

(Schumpeter, 1934, pp. 94-95)

ABSTRACT

Over the last several years, a handful of researchers have shown renewed interest in demonstrating the relationship of cognition and entrepreneurial intent. Drawing from this Herculean effort, the authors continue the search for a personality linkage. One hundred and seventeen undergraduate students from two diverse southwestern universities were administered the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI). Results provide further support of a cognitive relationship as well as implications for continued research.

INTRODUCTION

Since 1984 (Carland, Hoy, Boulton & Carland) considerable debate has centered on defining the entrepreneur. One school of thought has taken (what these authors consider) the easy way out by maintaining that anyone that engages in self-venturing is an entrepreneur. At the other end of this continuum, an aggressive, robust, and maybe (some may contend) fool-hearty group still contends that understanding the cognitive relationship of the individual and the venture is critical for better appreciating entrepreneurship.

The purpose of this paper will be to continue the monumental task of searching for a relationship between personality and the propensity to self-venture. It is worth repeating, the intent of this research is not to provide evidence of differences between small businesses and entrepreneurs, but rather, to accept the conclusions drawn by Carland, Carland and Hoy (1992), Jackson, Watts and Wright (1993) that: the entrepreneur and his/her activity is best defined on a continuum; and, that there exists a "clear and pressing need to establish a valid measure of entrepreneurship" (Carland, Carland & Ensley, 2001). We also hope to provide further support to the convergent validity of the CEI with the MBTI as a means of defining the entrepreneur.

To accomplish this, we provide a brief history of the "E" debate, an overview of the instruments recommended for future research, and an empirical examination of the relationship between the two instruments. The ultimate outcome is that we contribute a small part in ensuring that entrepreneurship research does not suffer a slow death as described by Schumpeter's opening quote.

THE LINE IN THE SAND

From the mid-fifties into the mid-eighties, considerable discussion of the entrepreneur and his/her characteristics was carried on regularly in the literature. As Table 1 suggests, characteristics assigned to the entrepreneur were quite diverse. For the most part, this early research paid little if any attention to the type of small businessperson being investigated. Carland, Hoy, Boulton and Carland (1984), however, categorically insisted that our point of research orientation had to be narrowed. A "Mom and Pop" small business owner had to be different from one that started a venture that grew into a larger corporation. Many applauded quietly while others beat their war drums to the contrary.

Probably the most vocal of the dissenters was Gartner (1988). Gartner, is essence, drew the line in the sand and strongly declared that it does not matter-self-venturing is entrepreneurship. Considering that there had already been recent calls in the management and marketing disciplines for a more scientific, quantitative approach to research, it is no surprise that many joined Gartner's camp.

For the most part, the years since have witnessed a limited approach to discussion of cognition and the entrepreneur. The Carlands have held fast in their convictions to expand research in this area (Carland, Hoy & Carland, 1988; Carland, Hoy, Boulton & Carland, 1988; Carland & Carland, 1991, 1992, 1997, 2001; Ensley, Carland & Carland, 1998; Stewart, Carland & Carland, 1996; Carland, Carland & Stewart, 1996; Carland, Carland & Carland, 1995). Fortunately, a few others have followed in the vein of the "cognitive connection" (Forsgren, 1990; Ginn & Sexton, 1990; Sexton & Ginn, 1990; Brodzinski, Scherer & Wiebe, 1990; Dugan, Feeser & Plaschka, 1990; Jackson, Watts & Wright, 1993; Reynierse, 1997; Tampere & Koiranen, 2000).

Still others (most very recently) have actually offered specific approaches to take. It was suggested that we should explore "potential" (Palmer, 1971), "conative (motivational and volitional) constructs" (Ruothotie & Koiranen, 2000), "psychological ownership" (Pierce,Kostova & Dirks, 2001), "psychological correlates" (Stewart, 1996), "entrepreneurial teams" (Ensley, 1999), "skills" (Robinson & Herron, 2001), and "strategic continuums" (Jackson, Watts & Wright, 1993). While many declare the need-none have offered the validated means to accomplish the task.

The Carlands have done just this by suggesting that it is quite appropriate to apply multiple measurement scales to the "E" phenomenon (Carland, Carland & Ensley, 2001). They have not stopped with just the idea-they have put forth the effort to provide the initial support for answering the question of who is the dancer.

PRIOR RESEARCH

Myers-Brigg Type Indicator (MBTI) and the Entrepreneur

Before discussing the prior research accomplished with the MBTI in terms of entrepreneurial personality traits, a general overview of this instrument is warranted. One of the most succinct explanations of this instrument was provided by Myers, McCaulley, Quenk & Hammer (1998):

The purpose of the Myers-Brigg Type Indicator (MBTI) personality inventory is to make the theory of psychological types described by C. G. Jung (1921/1971) understandable and useful in peoples' lives. The essence of the theory is that much seemingly random variation in behavior is actually quite orderly and consistent, being due to basic differences in the way individuals prefer to use their perception and judgment (p. 3).

The means of accomplishing the separation needed to define types is based upon responses to forced choice questions that focus on four basic principles. These principles are: where does an individual prefer to focus their energies-internally or externally; how does an individual prefer to perceive stimuli-through their senses or through intuitive relationships; how does an individual make judgments-impersonally based upon logical consequences or rather on personal or social values; and how does an individual prefer to deal with the world-either through thinking or feeling or through sensing or intuition. These dichotomies are shown below in Table 2. The combination of responses allows for sixteen possible individual personality types.

The instrument (through numerous revisions) has been examined within over "4,000 research studies, journal articles, and dissertations ... since 1962 (Myers et al, 1998, p. 10). Considerable tests of validity and reliability of the instrument have occurred within many of these studies. However, only a limited number of studies have specifically focused on entrepreneurs. The authors have set out to add to that limited literature in the current research.

As is evidenced by Table 3 below, each preference has a dichotomous element that leans toward previously identified traits of the entrepreneur. While experts in the area of MBTI development stress that it is the collective groupings of these dichotomies that provide full meaning, the individual pairings still provide considerable insight into the individual's characteristics.

Numerous researchers have examined the differences in these individual preferences over the last decade in a wide variety of populations. Some have specifically addressed the difference between small business owners and entrepreneurs, while others have focused on the assumed characteristics of the entrepreneur.

Fasiska (1992) as well as Carland and Carland (1992) determined that entrepreneurs demonstrated a higher E value (preferring to focus their attention on the outer-world versus their inner world) than did managers. The Carland study also supported the direct difference between small business owners and entrepreneurs with the same findings.

Several studies have provided support for the conclusion that entrepreneurs (or potential entrepreneurs) demonstrated a preference for collecting information through intuition (N) versus through hard cold facts (S) (Fasiska, 1992; Carland & Carland, 1992; Carland, Carland, Hoy & Boulton, 1988; Ginn & Sexton, 1990; and, Sexton & Bowman, 1985). These results are not surprising given almost all research (whether theoretical or empirical) has recognized the entrepreneur as being innovative (Palmer, 1971), a change-agent (Drucker, 1985), as an individual that through his/her foresight "upsets and disorganizes" (Weber, 1958), or is the means for "creative destruction" (Schumpeter, 1934). Or as Stewart (1996) suggests, the commonality among [entrepreneurs] was the ability to see what was not there, to envision the creation of a new venture" (p xi).

Results of studying the third preference (T-F) have provided mixed signals. As can be seen by referring back to each preference as discussed in table 2, this is probably to be expected. It may very well be that, entrepreneurs (as is probably the case with most business managers) must, at times, base conclusions on facts (Carland & Carland, 1992; Reynierse, 1993; Roach, 1986). In addition, entrepreneurs have often been recognized as individuals with high personal values. Thus, two dilemmas surface--this is probably an area we cannot expect differences with business people in general, and that the entrepreneur may use each preference T or F with equal ease.

The final preference P--J has also received reasonable support in the literature regarding the entrepreneur. Several studies have shown the entrepreneur preferring to deal with the outer-world using P rather than J (Reynierse, 1993; Roach, 1986; Fasiska, 1992, Carland & Carland, 1992, Sexton & Ginn, 1996). It is also reasonable to expect these results--the entrepreneur is expected to exercise considerable adaptability when maneuvering a new venture into the market.

While the above summary of preference for information-related processes is encouraging from a theoretical perspective, it still enjoys only limited empirical support. For the most part, the entrepreneur has been ill defined (as has been the case of most entrepreneur/small business research), and the sample has often been questionable.

The Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI)

In 1992, Carland, Carland and Hoy presented a manuscript on empirical validation of a new entrepreneurship scale-the CEI. The authors based this instrument on considerable research in "... entrepreneurial planning, characteristic behavior and distinctions, and [borrowed] heavily from the literature of cognitive psychology." What emerged was an investigative tool that evaluated entrepreneurial drive on a continuum rather than as a dichotomous condition.

Results of this study proved promising. In their study of "conative" constructs, Ruohotie and Koiranen (2000) lauded the value of this instrument. According to these authors, "... the widely known Carland Index nicely aggregates four key elements of entrepreneurship: personality, innovativeness, risk-taking propensity, and strategic posture" (p 11). Validation of the instrument remained, however, a work in progress-that is until recently.

In their recent study (Carland, Carland & Ensley, 2001), these pioneers in the field provided considerable support in terms of reliability, discriminant and convergent validity, as well as some predictive validity. The results indicated, "... personality behavioral elements which have traditionally been associated with entrepreneurship were correlated with the entrepreneurship index at a high level and in the correct direction" (p. 67). The authors downplayed the success of the study and vowed to improve the scale (especially in the area of its predictive qualities).

The authors need to give themselves more credit. It was, indeed, a tremendous success in that through the results we can discriminate between different types of entrepreneurs on a continuum. While many have suspected this (Jackson, Watts & Wright, 1993; Carland & Carland, 1997), an instrument for confirmation is now in place. This marks a new stage in entrepreneurial research, and one that brings us a major step closer to defining the "E" word. Most great researchers (and the same might be said for true entrepreneurs) are never satisfied until total resolution occurs, but in the interim we should be applauding loudly. The research community has an obligation to continue along this path until additional modifications of the instrument are made, thus providing support for the quantum leap in research results that will surely occur.

HYPOTHESES

The current research is based on the implicit assumption that a group of advanced business students will exhibit personality characteristics in common with persons actively engaged in business. More particularly, business students will compare well with other groups of businesspersons in terms of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator.

Furthermore, prior research suggests certain relationships between the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) results and the score value on the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI). In particular, more "entrepreneurial" persons are expected to show a stronger tendency to E over I. They should also show a greater likelihood for T over F, N over S and for P over J. These statements are based on the discussion offered earlier in conjunction with Table 3. It would be expected that these correlations would be present in the business student population as well. Given those a priori expectations, the following four working hypotheses regarding traits were developed for evaluation in the current research.
H1: Respondents scoring higher on the CEI should exhibit preference
 for E over I on the MBTI.
H2: Respondents scoring higher on the CEI should exhibit preference
 for N over S on the MBTI.
H3: Respondents scoring higher on the CEI should exhibit preference
 for T over F on the MBTI.
H4: Respondents scoring higher on the CEI should exhibit preference
 for P over J on the MBTI.


A fifth hypotheses was formulated based more generally on the literature. This hypothesis relates to types rather than traits. The hypothesis states that ESTJ and ISTJ types will be more common among more entrepreneurial persons.
H5: Respondents scoring higher on the CEI should prefer ESTJ and ISTJ
 over other types.


THE SAMPLE

A sample of upper-level undergraduate business students from two, small rural universities completed both the MBTI and the CEI scales. Some 117 useable sets of responses were obtained from the students. The analyses that follow are based on the data obtained from that sample of students. Before turning to the analysis, however, a brief profile of the sample is provided. The sample was young, as would be expected of undergraduates. Over 62% were between the ages of 19 and 25, and 20.5% were in the 26 to 35 group. The selection of the sample was secondary to the source of the sample elements. The researchers desired to gather data in classes composed of advanced business students. Over 85% of the students were, in fact, seniors and another 12% were juniors. As far as gender is concerned, 53.8% of the students were women and 46.2% were men. The only other major demographic concern in the research was exposure to the small business environment. Three questions were directed at this issue. Only 21% of the students surveyed reported having owned their own businesses. On the other hand, over 50% reported that their parents owned their own small businesses. In addition, over 82% of the students reported they had been (or currently were) employed by a small business at least once. The researchers saw nothing in this data that was surprising or alarming.

Historically, there have been some concerns about using student groups as populations in research. That concern does not apply in the current research because the researchers specifically wanted to know about students. However, that concern is real. For that reason, the researchers decided to "compare" the MBTI profile of the student sample to some other relevant, known group. Reynierse (1997) reports on several samples that might be of interest. One sample, in particular, discussed originally by Carland and Carland (1992) consisted partly of small business owners in a larger study of several populations. While that analysis is beyond the scope of this study, the researchers found no significant differences between the current student sample and the reported sample insofar as any traits on the MBTI are concerned. The analysis used was a standard SSRT Comparison used for such purposes.

DATA ANALYSIS

A note on CEI scoring

The analytical technique selected for the hypotheses specified above, was a simple analysis of proportions. Before beginning the analysis, the authors had to decide on the "degree of granularity" for the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI) scores. The decision to specify two levels of granularity was based on a lack of clear guidance in the literature. The researchers were not able to find any specification of a cut-off point for delineating between high and low levels of CEI. It did not seem appropriate to just choose some score. After some deliberation, the researchers decided to approach the question on the basis of the content of the data. At one level of granularity, the researchers decided to allow a boundary based on cumulative distribution. Because the interest was vested in higher CEI scores (see the hypotheses), the groups came out as follows:
LOW CEI score of 20 or less
HIGH CEI score of 21 or higher


In order to consider other approaches to deciding on the High/Low CEI issue, the researchers determined to also use a tercile approach. The decision as to what constitutes a tercile was also based on cumulative frequency. The groups fell out as specified below:
LOW CEI score of 18 or less
MEDIUM CEI score of 19 to 22
HIGH CEI score of 23 or higher


These decision rule results indicate a tight compaction of scores around the score of 20 or 21. Analysis of the data (presented below) for the four stated hypotheses was carried out on the basis of both score category schemes.

The High/Low approach to the data yielded 54 respondents with "Low" scores (20 or less) on the CEI and 63 respondents with "High" scores (21 or more) on the CEI. The other approach High/Medium/Low yielded 36 respondents with "Low" scores (18 or less), 40 with "Medium" scores (19-22) and 41 with "High" scores (22 or above) on the CEI. Having dispensed with the issue of granularity in the analysis, the following tables present the results of the analysis for each of the four hypotheses in turn.

In the following discussion of hypothesis tests, both the 2-group and 3-group CEI groupings are tested. Furthermore, the reader should note that in the data analysis tables, the MBTI anchors are presented in the order specified in the hypotheses, which is not the customary order. Specifically, in Hypothesis 2, the order in the table is reversed from the normal S-N to N in the first data column since the hypothesis predicts N as dominant in the case at hand. Also for the discussion of Hypothesis 4, the columns are reversed from the customary J-P to present P first in keeping with the prediction in the hypothesis.

Hypothesis 1

Hypothesis 1 predicted that respondents who scored higher on the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI) would be more likely to prefer personality trait E (extraversion) over trait I (introversion). Results were generated using both the 2-group and the 3-group approaches. These results are presented and discussed below.

Using the 2-group (High vs. Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 E (%) I (%)

Low CEI (< 20) 27 (50%) 27 (50%)
High CEI (>21) 45 (71%) 18 (29%)


At first glance, the data support the hypothesis that High CEI types are more likely to prefer E over I on the MBTI. These results are significant (using the Pearson chi-square test) at the .018 level.

Using the 3-group (High-Medium-Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 E (%) I (%)

Low CEI (< 18) 14 (39%) 22 (61%)
Medium CEI (=19-22) 26 (65%) 14 (35%)
High CEI (>21) 32 (78%) 9 (22%)


The hypothesis is supported at the .002 level by the Pearson test.

Obviously, the data overwhelmingly support the hypothesis that High CEI score respondents are more likely to demonstrate preference for Extraversion over Introversion on the MBTI.

Hypothesis 2

Hypothesis 2 predicted that respondents who scored higher on the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI) would be more likely to prefer personality trait N (intuition) over trait S (sensing). Using the 2-group (High vs. Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 N (%) S (%)

Low CEI (< 20) 30 (43%) 40 (57%)
High CEI (>21) 33 (70%) 14 (30%)


Again, the data support the hypothesis that High CEI types are more likely to prefer N over S on the MBTI. These results are significant (using the Pearson chi-square test) at the .004 level.

Using the 3-group (High-Medium-Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 N (%) S (%)

Low CEI (< 18) 11 (31%) 25 (69%)
Medium CEI (=19-22) 11 (28%) 29 (73%)
High CEI (>21) 25 (61%) 16 (39%)


The hypothesis is supported at the .003 level by the Pearson test.

Again, the data overwhelmingly support the hypothesis that High CEI score respondents are more likely to demonstrate preference for Intuition over Sensing on the MBTI.

Hypothesis 3

Hypothesis 3 predicted that respondents who scored higher on the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI) would be more likely to prefer personality trait T (thinking) over trait F (feeling). Results were generated using both the 2-group and the 3-group approaches. These results are presented and discussed below.

Using the 2-group (High vs. Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 T (%) F (%)

Low CEI (< 20) 26 (48%) 28 (52%)
High CEI (>21) 41 (65%) 22 (35%)


Again, the data support the hypothesis that High CEI types are more likely to prefer T over F on the MBTI. These results are significant (using the Pearson chi-square test) at the .065 level.

Using the 3-group (High-Medium-Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 T (%) F (%)

Low CEI (< 18) 14 (39%) 22 (61%)
Medium CEI (=19-22) 27 (68%) 13 (32%)
High CEI (>21) 26 (63%) 15 (37%)


The hypothesis is supported at the .026 level by the Pearson test.

The test results are not as strong as in the test of the first two hypotheses, but are convincing nonetheless. The data definitely support the hypothesis that High CEI score respondents are more likely to demonstrate Thinking over Feeling on the MBTI.

Hypothesis 4

Hypothesis 4 predicted that respondents who scored higher on the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI) would be more likely to prefer personality trait P (perceiving) over trait J (judging). Results were generated using both the 2-group and the 3-group approaches. These results are presented and discussed below.

Using the 2-group (High vs. Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 P (%) J (%)

Low CEI (< 20) 16 (30%) 38 (70%)
High CEI (>21) 25 (40%) 38 (60%)


In this case, the data do not support the hypothesis that High CEI types are more likely to prefer P over J on the MBTI.

Using the 3-group (High-Medium-Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 P (%) J (%)

Low CEI (< 18) 13 (36%) 23 (64%)
Medium CEI (=19-22) 15 (38%) 25 (62%)
High CEI (>21) 13 (32%) 28 (68%)


The 3-group granularity in the definition of level of CEI score also failed to support the hypothesis.

The test results clearly argue against the hypothesis that high CEI respondents are more likely to prefer Perceiving over Judging on the MBTI. In this case, in fact, the distribution of selection of Perceiving and Judging are remarkably constant across the two approaches to granularity.

Before proceeding with the final hypothesis, which is really of a different kind, the reader might be interested in some reflections on the bigger picture with respect to the first four hypotheses. Consider the following data on the MBTI traits without regard to CEI score.
Characteristic N

Extraversion 72
Intraversion 45
Sensing 70
Intuition 47
Thinking 67
Feeling 50
Judging 76
Perceiving 41


Note that we have highlighted the trait in each pair that is the predicted variable in the first four hypotheses. We will have more to say on this data in the implications section.

Hypothesis 5

Hypothesis 5 predicted that respondents who scored higher on the Carland Entrepreneurial Index (CEI) would prefer personality types ESTJ and ISTJ over other personality types. One important aspect of this hypothesis is that ESTJ and ISTJ are only two of the sixteen types available in MBTI type research. This means there are fourteen other types that could be selected. This compounds selection because there is only a one in eight probability that one of the two specified types would be "uncovered" at random with any given respondent. With fairly small samples, a strong bias against the hypothesis is built in. But, you need a hypothesis to move things forward.

The researchers first looked at simple counts to test this hypothesis. For the 117 students surveyed, the following results ensued. The sixteen types are listed in order of counts from most frequent to least frequent.

Clearly, something related to the hypothesis of interest is going on. The two types predicted to dominate the data for high CEI respondents are the two most frequent for the entire sample. Together they account for 29% of all respondents. On a strictly random basis, they should account for only 12.5%. Of course, MBTI types do not occur in completely equal proportions. Some types are relatively more (and some relatively less) common in the population. It is possible that the preponderance of these two types in the sample would mask the proposition inherent in the hypothesis.
TYPE N

ESTJ 19
ISTJ 15
ENFP 11
ISFJ 10
ESFJ 10
ENTJ 9
ENFJ 9
ENTP 7
ISTP 5
INTP 5
ESFP 4
INTJ 4
ISFP 4
ESTP 3
INFP 2
INFJ 0


However, the question at hand relates to the sample itself. Are the students in the study who produced higher CEI scores more likely than students in the study who produced lower CEI scores, to prefer ESTJ or ISTJ types on the MBTI? Again, the test of choice is a simple test of proportions. As before, results were generated using both the 2-group CEI and the 3-group CEI approaches. These results are presented and discussed below.

Using the 2-group (High vs. Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 ESTJ/ISTJ (%) Not ESTJ/ISTJ (%)

Low CEI (< 20) 17 (31%) 37 (69%)
High CEI (>21) 17 (27%) 46 (73%)


In this case, the data do not support the hypothesis that High CEI types are more likely to prefer ESTJ or ISTJ over other MBTI types--at least not at a higher rate than the low CEI respondents. This is obvious in that the Low CEI group actually selected ESTJ/ISTJ at a slightly higher proportion than the High CEI group.

Using the 3-group (High-Medium-Low) approach, the following data were obtained:
 ESTJ/ISTJ (%) Not ESTJ/ISTJ (%)

Low CEI (< 18) 9 (25%) 27 (75%)
Medium CEI (=19-22) 15 (38%) 25 (62%)
High CEI (>21) 10 (24%) 31 (76%)


The finer granularity in the 3-group definition of level of CEI score also failed to support the hypothesis. Interestingly, in this case, the highest proportion was for the medium CEI group, while the low CEI and high CEI groups were almost identical in distribution.

The test results clearly argue against the hypothesis that high CEI respondents are more likely to prefer ESTJ and ISTJ types over all other MBTI types relative to lower CEI respondents.

CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS

The researchers set out to consider a simple problem. This problem relates to the correlation between the Myers-Brigg Type Indicator (MBTI) and the Carland Entrepreneurship Index (CEI). The discussion of the background literature related to the MBTI and the CEI led to the specification of five very straightforward hypotheses. Data were collected from 117 undergraduate students using a convenience sampling technique. The student respondents were recruited in senior level business classes. We assumed that the student group would exhibit some tendency toward entrepreneurship greater than the general population. The researchers concede that the sample is fairly small, but the study is exploratory in nature.

Table 3 specifies the predictions to be found in the first four of the five hypotheses. The table and accompanying hypotheses (1-4) predict specific trait preferences based on an analysis of existing literature. Specifically, we predicted that respondents who scored higher on the CEI would prefer one trait over the other on each of the four MBTI dichotomies (E over I, N over S, T over F, and P over J). Three of these four hypotheses were supported by the data collected from the sample. To wit, the data indicated a significant preference for E over I, for N over S and for T over F. The last of the trait related hypotheses was not supported by the data. It is possible that the small sample size used in the study confounded this hypothesis. For the whole sample, 65% (76) of the respondents preferred Judging over Perceiving. For the four dichotomies, this was the strongest split. It may be that the sheer weight of the Judging preference in the sample masked the expected preference.

The fifth hypothesis predicted that ESTJ and ISTJ types would be more common among high CEI respondents. It turned out that STJ's were simply more common. The two types accounted for 29% (34) of the respondents. Even though (or perhaps because of) the overall preference for ESTJ and ISTJ types in the sample, the hypothesis was not supported. We did not find that higher CEI score respondents were more likely to prefer ESTJ or ISTJ over the other fourteen types. Again, the size of the sample might have confounded the analysis.

The first implication to be drawn from the research is obvious. The sample behaved largely as the trait-related hypotheses predicted. The fact that three of the four trait hypotheses was supported lends to the argument that respondents who "score" near the upper end of the CEI do, in fact, tend to prefer specific traits over others on the MBTI trait pairs. Furthermore, the preferences match predictions made from prior research and theory for three of the four trait pairs.

At the same time, the data acquired from the respondents did not support the fourth trait-related hypothesis. While it is possible that the hypothesis related to this trait pair (J-P), is in some way faulty, the researchers are reluctant to say so based on the concern over sample size and results of previous research. The relationship between the CEI and the Judging/Perceiving dichotomy requires further study. We have already mentioned that a strong majority of the sample (without regard to CEI score) preferred Judging over Perceiving. That being the case, more detailed research needs to be conducted to decide whether we need to rethink the prediction of a preference for Perceiving among more entrepreneurial (as determined by CEI score) individuals. Of course, it may also be that the sample used, and not just its size, was inadequate for a fair test of the hypothesis.

Conversely, the data related to the hypothesis (H2) concerning the S-N pair are very impressive. The sample as a whole clearly preferred Sensing (60%) over Intuition (40%). What the data indicate is that while that strong Sensing preference is evident in the sample, the 40% who preferred Intuition were heavily concentrated among those who scored toward the high end of the CEI. This has to be one of the strongest conclusions in the study.

The last of the five hypotheses predicted that respondents with high CEI scores would prefer ESTJ and ISTJ types over other types. This hypothesis was not supported even though ESTJ and ISTJ were the two most preferred types over all. The lack of support for the hypothesis surprised us. Further analysis and more research need to be directed at the relationship between xSTJ types and CEI scores to uncover the nature of the relationship that might exist, particularly in light of the strong overall preference for xSTJ.

Further development of several of the findings of this research is in order and already underway. The researchers expected to find, and found, that persons with more propensity for self-venturing would prefer Extraversion to Introversion. We also expected to find, and found, that persons of the same high propensity to self-venture would prefer Thinking to Feeling. We expected to find, and found, to a surprisingly high degree that persons with high propensity to self-venture preferred Intuition to Sensing. These findings are completely consistent with general expectations. We did not find that persons with a high propensity to self-venture preferred Perceiving over Judging. In fact, the data demonstrate a very high level of Judging over Perceiving and imply that persons who do prefer Perceiving are diffuse with respect to the CEI.

In terms of research, a larger sample needs to be acquired to determine whether or not sample size confounded the analysis related to the last two hypotheses. Another implication is that a different kind of population might produce results more in keeping with expectations. Another matter for discussion among scholars is the arbitrary method(s) the researchers used to distinguish "levels" of CEI scores. The researchers used a percentile of responses approach. What cut-off levels would be most appropriate? Perhaps a theoretical cut-point based on a normal probability distribution would yield different results.

The researchers feel that the study presented in this paper lends considerable support for the proposition that personality traits, as indicated by MBTI scores, do correlate well with the propensity to self-venture. The study indicates certain areas in which more definitive research efforts are needed and can be used to help specify some of that research. Finally, the study adds another piece to the puzzle about who the entrepreneur is.

REFERENCES

Brodzinski, J. D., Scherer, R. F. & Wiebe, F. A. (1990). Boundary spanning activity: A function of the small business owner's decision style. Journal of Business & Entrepreneurship, 2(2), 1-12.

Carland, J. W., Hoy, F., Boulton, W. R. & Carland, J. C. (1984). Differentiating entrepreneurs from small business owners: A conceptualization. Academy of Management Review, 9(2), 354-359.

Carland, J. W., Carland, J. C, Hoy, F. & Boulton, W. R. (1988). Distinctions between entrepreneurial and small business ventures. International Journal of Management, 5, 98-103.

Carland, J. W. & Carland, J. C. (1991). An empirical investigation into the distinctions between male and female entrepreneurs and managers. International Journal of Small Business, 9(3), 62-72.

Carland, J. C. & Carland, J. W. (1992). Managers, small business owners, entrepreneurs: The cognitive dimensions. Journal of Business & Entrepreneurship, 4 (2), 55-66.

Carland, J. W., Carland, J. C. Stewart, W. H. (1996). Seeing what's not there: The enigma of entrepreneurship. Journal of Small Business Strategy, 7(1), 1-20.

Carland, J. W., Jr. & Carland, J. C. (1997). Entrepreneurship: An American dream. Journal of Business & Entrepreneurship, 9 (1), 33-46.

Carland, J. W., Carland, J. C. & Ensley, M. D. (2001). Hunting the heffalump: The theoretical basis and dimensionality of the Carland Entrepreneurship Index. Academy of Entrepreneurship Journal, 7(2), 51-84.

Drucker, P. F. (1985). Innovation and Entrepreneurship. New York: Harper Row.

Dugan, K. W., Feeser, H. R. & Plaschka, G. R. (1990). A comparison of personality characteristics among women entrepreneurs, corporate businesswomen and the general female population, In T. W. Garsombke & D. J. Garsombke (Eds.), Conference Proceedings, Orlando, FL: United States Association for Small Business & Entrepreneurship, 88-94.

Fasiska, E. J. (1992). Managerial type determination and its role in the development of a Entrepreneurial Quotient (EQ) instrument. International Journal of Value-Based Management, 5, 17-37.

Gartner, W. B. (1988). "Who is the entrepreneur?" is the wrong question. American Journal of Small Business, 12(4), 11-32.

Ginn, C. & Sexton, D. L. (1990). A comparison of the personality type dimensions of the 1987 inc. 500 company founder/ceos with those of slower-growth firms. Journal of Business Venturing, 5(5), 313-326.

Herron, L. (1992). Cultivating corporate entrepreneurs. Human Resource Planning, 15, 3-14.

Jackson, W. T., Watts, L. R. & Wright, P. (1993). Small businesses: An examination of strategic groups. Journal of Business & Entrepreneurship, 5(1), 85-96.

Jackson, W. T., Gaulden, C. & Gaster, W. (2001). Who it is and what it does: Finding the "Heffa-Preneur". Academy of Entrepreneurship Journal, 7(2), 17-29.

Jackson, W. T. & Gaulden, C. (2001). Life cycle theory: New implications for defining entrepreneurial types in developing countries. International Journal of Entrepreneurship, 5, 81-92.

Jung, C. (1923). Psychological Types. London: Pantheon books.

McClelland, D. C. (1961). The Achieving Society. Princeton: Van Nostrand.

Myers, I. B. & Briggs, K. C. (1962). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Princeton: Educational Testing Services.

Myers, I. B. & Myers, P. B. (1980). Gifts Differing. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Myers, I. B., McCaulley, M. H., Quenk, N. L. & Hammer, A. L. (1998). MBTI Manual: A Guide to the Development and Use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (3rd Edition). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.

Palmer, M. (1971). The application of psychological testing to entrepreneurial potential. California Management Review, 8(3), 32-38.

Pierce, J. L., Kostova, T. & Kirks, K. T. (2001). Toward a theory of psychological ownership in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 26(2), 298-310.

Reynierse, J. H. (1993). The distribution and flow of managerial types through organizational levels in business and industry. Journal of Psychological Type, 25, 11-23.

Reynierse, J. H. (1997). An MBTI model of entrepreneurism and bureaucracy: The psychological types of business entrepreneurs compared to business managers and executives. Journal of Psychological Type, 40, 3-19.

Robinson, R. & Herron, L. (2001). The impact of strategy and industry structure on the link between the entrepreneur and venture performance. Academy of Entrepreneurship Journal, 7(2), 31-50.

Ruohotie, P. & Koiranen, (2000). In the pursuit of conative constructs into entrepreneurship education. Journal of Entrepreneurship Education, 3, 9-21.

Schumpeter, J. A. (1934). The Theory of Economic Development, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Stewart, W. H., Jr. (1996). Psychological Correlates of Entrepreneurship, New York: Garland Publishing, Inc.

Weber, M. (1958). The Protestant Ethic and the Spirit of Capitalism, Translated by Talcott Parsons. New York: Scribner's.

Corbett Gaulden, The University of Texas of the Permian Basin William T. Jackson, The University of Texas of the Permian Basin Walter (Buddy) Gaster, Southeastern Oklahoma State University
Table 1

Entrepreneurial Characteristics

Date Author(s) Trait

1954 Sutton Desire for Responsibility
1959 Hartman Source of Formal Authority
1961 McClelland Need for Achievement
1963 Davids Ambition, Independence, Self-Confidence
1964 Pickle Drive, Human Relations Skills
1971 Palmer Risk Taker
1973 Winter Need for Power
1974 Borland Internal Locus of Control
1977 Gasse Personal Value Orientation
1978 Timmons Drive, Moderate Risk Taker
1980 Sexton Energetic

Source: Carland, Hoy, Boulton & Carland, 1984

Table 2

The Four Dichotomies of the MBTI

E-Extroversion Directing energy I-Introversion Directing energy
 toward the outer toward the inner
 world of people world of
 and objects experiences
 and ideas

S-Sensing Focusing on what N-Intuition Focusing on
 can be perceived perceiving
 by the senses patterns and
 interrelationships

T-Thinking Basing F-Feeling Basing
 conclusions on conclusions on
 logical analysis personal social
 with objectivity values with a
 and detachment focus on harmony

J-Judging Preferring the P-Perceiving Preferring the
 decisiveness and flexibility that
 closure that results from
 results from dealing with the
 dealing with the outer world
 outer world using one of
 using one the perceiving
 judging processes
 processes (sensing or
 (thinking or intuition)
 feeling)

Source: Myers, McCaulley, Quenk & Hammer, 1998

Table 3

Dominant Preference Characteristics

Preference Preference Characteristic

E-I Outward-directed focus toward new products, emerging
 markets and opportunities
S-N Innovative, strategic focus emphasizing future
 possibilities
T-F Tough minded, impersonal, non-accommodating approach
 to decision making
J-P Unstructured, adaptable, rebellious toward authority,
 will break the rules, and tends to initiate change

Source: Reynierse, 1997
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有