An examination of employee culture-based perceptions as a predictor of motivation.
Emery, Charles R. ; Oertel, Simon
ABSTRACT
As worldwide competition continues to increase, corporations are
feverishly seeking ways to increase productivity. A critical element to
increasing productivity is employee motivation. This task of
understanding and influencing the employee's motivation is often
made easier, if the company attempts to select employees with specific
values, beliefs and needs that align with those of the company. This
study explores the relationship between the Hofstede cultural dimensions (as a predictor of values) and Vroom's expectancy theory (valence,
expectancy, instrumentality) for the purpose of determining whether
Hofstede's instrument can be used to predict an individual's
motivation potential in a given organizational environment. A variety of
hypotheses were tested using a web-based survey of US and German
workers. Although the hypotheses concerning the relationship between
culture-based perceptions and expectancies and instrumentalities were
not heavily supported, several of the relationships between an
employee's cultural values and valences were supported. This
suggests that motivation, to some extent, can be predicted by knowledge
of an employee's culture-based values. Additionally, this research
presents some interesting findings on motivation across various
demographic categories (e.g., nationality, gender) and suggests some
issues for future research on selecting for motivation.
INTRODUCTION
As worldwide competition continues to increase, corporations are
feverishly seeking ways to increase productivity. A critical element to
increasing productivity is employee motivation. Most researchers believe
that to enhance employee motivation, one must understand the motivation
process. In other words, one must understand how an employee processes
his or her environment to make choices. The most popular of the
motivation process theories is Victor Vroom's Expectancy Theory
(1995). The Expectancy Theory suggests that a person's motivation
is based on the product of his or her valence (the value of an
individual goal), expectancy (probability of successfully accomplishing
a task), and instrumentality (probability that the successful
accomplishment of the task will result in achieving a desired goal). In
practice, a manager should attempt to understand an employee's
valences, expectancy, and instrumentality for each task in order to
influence his or her motivation. This task of understanding and
influencing the employee's motivation is often made easier, if the
company attempts to select employees with specific values, beliefs and
needs that align with those of the company. In other words, a good
"cultural fit" may be an important prerequisite for
motivation. More specifically, a company should consider the
candidate's perceptions of expectancies, instrumentalities, and
valences against the organization's environment for the best
"motivational fit."
The most popular method used to select for productivity focuses on
matching an individual's traits or characteristics with those
required of the job (Cook, 1998). For example, many companies use all or
portions of the "Big Five Personality Test Model" (McCrae
& Costa, 1997) to screen applicants for extroversion, agreeableness,
emotional stability, openness to change and conscientiousness. Another
approach to screen for productivity, however, may be to assess an
applicant's perceptions or value system. Recent research indicates
that a person's perception (i.e. interpretation of environment) of
an organization's operation (e.g., policies and procedures) is
thought to be a major contributor to one's motivation and an
excellent predictor of behavior (Andre et al., 2003; Henle, 2005;
Hubbell & Chory-Assad, 2005). The perception construct is based on
an individual's attitude, personality, values, beliefs and norms
(Allport, 1955; Freud, 1963). While values, beliefs and norms are
normally reserved to describe the culture of a society, organization or
group, one can see that they might serve to describe an
individual's culture or culture-based perceptions. If so,
Hofstede's (1984) instrument for identifying a society's
cultural dimensions may be an appropriate tool to describe an
individual's culture-based perceptions. In other words, identifying
an applicant's Hofstede profile might help predict an
employee's potential motivation for a given organizational
environment (e.g., policies and procedures). For example, an employee
with high "uncertainty avoidance" might be more easily
motivated in highly structured organizations (e.g., rules and
procedures). An employee high in "power distance" might be
less likely to speak out against inequities or to volunteer ideas. An
employee high in "collectivism" might be more easily motivated
with group-based financial rewards.
The purpose of this study is to explore the relationship between
the Hofstede Cultural Dimensions and the elements of Vroom's
Expectancy Theory (valence, expectancy, instrumentality). If a
significant relationship exists between an individual's cultural
characteristics and the way they perceive key motivational factors, it
is logical to suggest that Hofstede's Cultural Dimensions may
predict a person's motivational profile. Survey questions that
provide a significant predictive power will be offered for future
instrument development and testing.
THEORETICAL BACKGROUND
Culture, Values and Perception
Culture is defined as collective programming of a society's
mind or modal personality (Wallace, 1970). At the individual level, the
components of culture are expressed as an individual's values and
perceptions (Allport, 1955). Hofstede (1984) defines a value as "a
broad tendency to prefer certain states of affairs over others. Because
our values are programmed early in our lives, they are non-rational and
determine our subjective definition of rationality or perception."
Perception is the cognitive process by which an individual selects,
organizes, and gives meaning to environmental stimuli (Andre et al.,
2003). Through perception, individuals attempt to make sense of their
environment and the objects, people, and events in it. In other words,
the space between stimuli and response is occupied by perception. As
such, knowledge of one's cultural-based or biased perception might
be a strong predictor of motivation and behavior. An instrument that
indicates a society's culture or work-related values might well be
a predictor of a person's values and perceptions and therefore,
motivation and behavior.
Cultural Dimensions
Geert Hofstede's 1980 study is one of the most frequently
cited research efforts regarding the relationship between societal culture and work-related values (Albers-Miller & Gelb, 1996). His
research has been instrumental in furthering an understanding of
cross-cultural management theory and practice, revealing that members of
different societies hold divergent values concerning the nature of
organizations and interpersonal relationships. Recently researchers have
begun to use Hofstede's model of culture as a framework for testing
the affect of cross-cultural differences on behavior (e.g., consumer
behavior). Hofstede's study yielded a structure consisting of four
dimensions on which societies differ: individualism, power distance,
uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity. Later, a fifth dimension of time
was added (Hofstede, 1994).
Hofstede explained that the dimension of individualism is the
degree to which individual decision-making and actions are encouraged by
a society. This dimension reflects the way people live together. In a
collectivistic society, the lower end of the individualism-collectivism
continuum, individualistic behavior may be seen as selfish. In some
cultures, like for example the US, individualism is seen as a blessing
and a source of well being. In other cultures like the Mexico, it is
seen as alienating. In general, employees with a lower Individualism
Index (IDV) have lower career aspirations and tend to have a high
emotional dependence and a high moral involvement in the company.
Employees with higher individualism scores have a higher career
orientation and egoistic working style and often tend to reject follower
ideas and initiatives. In low IDV countries, group decisions are
considered much better than individual decisions, while in high IDV
countries the opposite is true. Finally, in high IDV societies there
exists a high social mobility across occupation and more working hours
per month are common.
The power distance dimension indicates the degree to which power
differences are accepted and sanctioned by society. In other words, it
indicates how different societies have addressed basic human
inequalities in social status and prestige, wealth, and sources of
power. The societal norm in a country with a high score on the PD
dimension is for powerful people to look as powerful as possible. People
with power are considered to be right and good. Powerful people are
expected to have privileges. In countries with high power distance, the
exercise of power gives satisfaction and powerful people try to maintain
and increase power differences. Within companies, disparities in power
are often essential and functional. In countries with high Power
Distance Indexes (PDI) people don't believe in the chance of career
opportunities based on, for example, advanced training. Examples of
countries with a high PDI are Mexico and India. In India people are born
into castes, which influence their whole life and the chances of getting
promoted. Also, in high PDI countries, (e.g., China) people are raised
with a high value of obedience. As such, people in high PD cultures put
a high value on authority and compliance becomes an attitude or social
norm (Hanisch, 2003). The managers in high PDI countries make their
decisions on their own without any feedback from the followers. The
employees in these countries are scared to disagree with their bosses.
Additionally, high PDI societies are characterized by tall organization
pyramids. Managers usually rely on formal rules and there is little
defense against power abuses by a supervisor (Hofstede, 1994). Another
distinctiveness of high PDI countries is that many managers are
dissatisfied with their careers and feel underpaid.
The uncertainty avoidance dimension represents the degree to which
a society is unwilling to accept and cope with uncertainty. People use
laws, rules, rituals, religion, and technology to address uncertainty.
This dimension is related to anxiety, need for security, dependence on
experts, and the application of information (Hofstede, 1984). The higher
the uncertainty avoidance index (UNC) in a country, the more a high
authority is required and the more the society believes in and relies
upon experts. In high UNC countries the citizens tend to feel less able
to participate in political decisions at a local level. In many case, a
high UNC seems to provide a positive job stress that gives employees a
stronger achievement motivation. On the other hand, a high UNC reduces
the desire for creativity because of the possibility of failure. In work
situations, employees with a high UNC have a higher loyalty and a longer
average duration of employment (Hofstede, 1994). The power of a
supervisor in a high UNC country depends on his ability to manage or
avoid uncertainties. Finally, in high UNC employees have a high degree
of task orientation along with precision and punctuality. Low UNC
employees have a lower anxiety level and a greater acceptance of change
and failure. Lastly, employees in low UNC countries are more likely to
be optimistic about the reaching company goals (Hofstede, 1994).
The masculinity (MAS) dimension indicates the degree to which
traditional male values (assertiveness, performance, ambition,
achievement, and materialism) are important to a society. The opposite
end of this continuum has been labeled femininity. The societal norm in
a country with a high score on the MAS dimension is to try to be the
best--typical values include achievement, productivity and
"machismo". In these countries, big and fast are considered
beautiful. Further, in a high MAS society (e.g., Japan), the managers
have higher ideals of leadership, independence, and self-realization
than countries with a low MAS (e.g., Sweden). Also, High MAS means a
higher work centrality, the belief in individual decision, higher stress
on job, a strong achievement motivation, and a focus on money and
material goods. On the other hand, low MAS societies suggest an
organizational culture with lower job stress and that the managers have
a work attitude of being service providers for their followers.
Employees from low MAS countries are more relationship oriented and
usually see work as a means rather than the end. In other words, they
work to live rather than the more masculine focus of living to work
(Hofstede, 1984).
The fifth dimension of national culture is a long-term versus
short-term orientation (LTO/STO) of thought. According to Hofstede
(1994), the dimension of time is based a society's adherence to
principles of Confucius. For example, a society with a long-term
orientation toward time would focus on future rewards and consider the
family as the prototype of all social organizations. Virtuous behavior
would consist of not treating others as one would not like to be treated
oneself. One's task in life consists of trying to acquire skills
and education, working hard, not spending more than necessary, being
patient, and preserving resources (e.g., thrift). On the other hand, a
short-term focus can be defined as the fostering of virtues related to
the past and present, in particular, respect for tradition, preservation
of "face" and fulfilling social obligations.
Motivational Theories
Research indicates that the validity of motivational theories is
closely tied to the values, beliefs, and norms of the society, i.e. a
society's culture (Gagne & Deci, 2005). Two of the most popular
motivational theories which embody the notion that performance is
related to one's perception and value system are McClelland's
(1962) Learned Needs Theory and Vroom's (1964) Expectancy Theory.
In other words, both theories suggest performance motives reflect
persistent characteristics or perceptions of reality. The learned needs
theory suggests that needs are acquired from one's culture, i.e.
learned at an early stage through coping with one's environment. As
such, learned needs become the focus of one's motivation and help
create one's value system and vice versa. Three of these learned
needs are the need for achievement, the need for affiliation, and the
need for power. When an employee's job does not allow needs to be
fulfilled, he or she will reduce productivity and often become a
behavior problem (McClelland, 1962).
The Expectancy Theory (Vroom, 1964) holds that people are motivated
to behave in ways that produce the highest probability of desired
outcomes based on their perceptions of the situation. Critical to the
magnitude of motivation are the variables of expectancy,
instrumentality, and valence. Expectancy is defined as the probability
that an individual believes he or she can successfully accomplish a
particular task. Instrumentality represents a person's belief that
successful completion of a particular task will lead to the individually
desired outcome (goal). Valence is the value an individual places on the
desired outcome. As such, the motivation potential of pursuing a
particular behavior is calculated as expectancy (E) times
instrumentality (I) times valence (V). This study proposes that an
individual's E, I, and V and therefore motivation, may be
influenced or moderated by culturally-biased perceptions.
RESEARCH MODEL AND HYPOTHESES
The literature indicates that an individual's motives are
influenced by his or her perceptions and value system (Hofstede, 1994;
Maslow, 1987; Vroom, 1964). Specifically, Vroom (1964) indicates that
motivational potential is the product of an individual's perception
of expectancy times instrumentality times valence. Since a person's
perception is influenced by one's value system, it seems reasonable
to believe that culture-based values might moderate the values of an
individual's expectancies, instrumentalities and valences and
therefore have an effect on the individual's motivation. As such,
an instrument that assesses cultural dimensions might be suitable for
identifying an individual's culture-based perceptions and value
system and therefore be suitable for predicting the motivational
potential offered by various organizational policies and procedures.
Expectancy is an individual's perception of (the probability)
whether he/she can successfully accomplish a task (Vroom, 1964). It is
proposed that Hofstede's dimensions influence the expectancy of an
individual in many ways. For example, individuals with a low power
distance score have a higher expectancy for getting promoted than people
with a high power distance score. Employees with a high score in the
individualism dimension have a high expectancy regarding the truth of
their own decisions. Individuals with a high uncertainty avoidance
believe that there is only one way of doing things and therefore have
reduced expectations of success under dynamic environments. Individuals
with high masculine values are more likely to persist in the face of
early task failures. Employees with a long-term view of time are more
apt to exhibit patience when confronted with failure. As such, the
following hypotheses are proposed:
1a. The employee's power distance index (PDI) will
significantly predict the employee's expectancy.
2a. The employee's uncertainty avoidance index (UNC) will
significantly predict the employee's expectancy.
3a. The employee's individualism index (IDV) will
significantly predict the employee's expectancy.
4a. The employee's masculinity index (MAS) will significantly
predict the employee's expectancy.
5a. The employee's time orientation index (LTO) will
significantly predict the employee's expectancy.
Instrumentality is an individual's perception (of the
probability) that the successful completion of a task will lead to
desired rewards or valences (Vroom, 1964). For example, individuals with
high masculinity are more likely to believe that hard work results in
job promotion. Employees with high uncertainty avoidance require rules
stating the relationship between accomplishments and rewards. The
average worker with high power distance scores believes that he or she
will be rewarded or not rewarded at the discretion of top management.
Individuals with a long-term view of time are more likely to believe in
the connection between task accomplishment and reward. As such, the
following hypotheses are proposed:
1b. The employee's power distance index (PDI) will
significantly predict the employee's instrumentality.
2b. The employee's uncertainty avoidance index (UNC) will
significantly predict the employee's instrumentality.
3c. The employee's individualism index (IDV) will
significantly predict the employee's instrumentality.
4d. The employee's masculinity index (MAS) will significantly
predict the employee's instrumentality.
5e. The employee's time orientation index (LTO) will
significantly predict the employee's instrumentality.
The valence is the value an individual places on a particular
outcome or reward/need. One would expect that valences are significantly
affected by one's learned or culturally-based value system. For
example, employees high on masculinity generally favor pay increases,
job titles and promotions over time off or better working conditions.
Employees, with low uncertainty avoidance value autonomy and
flexibility. Employees with a high degree of individualism value
individual recognition. Individuals with a high sense of power distance
are more comfortable with inequities of rewards. Individuals with a
long-term orientation place a higher value on security and pension
benefits. As such, the following hypotheses are proposed:
1a. The employee's power distance index (PDI) will
significantly predict the employee's valences.
2a. The employee's uncertainty avoidance index (UNC) will
significantly predict the employee's valences.
3a. The employee's individualism index (IDV) will
significantly predict the employee's valences.
4a. The employee's masculinity index (MAS) will significantly
predict the employee's valences.
5a. The employee's time orientation index (LTO) will
significantly predict the employee's valences.
METHODOLOGY
The essence of this study was to determine whether
culturally-biased perceptions influence or predict an employee's
perceptions of motivational factors. As such, a three part, 61-question
survey was developed to test the hypotheses. The first part was used to
measure an individual's cultural-based perception. It consisted of
26 questions (5 MAS, 5 IDV, 5 UNC, 5 PDI, 6 LTO) and was taken directly
from Hofstede's Value Survey Module (1994). The second part
measured a worker's perception of typical organizational
expectancies, instrumentalities and valences. This section consisted of
27 questions (6 valence-related, 11 expectancy, and 10 instrumentality)
and was adapted from Vroom's (1995) listing of typical work-related
perceptions of motivational factors. The third part consisted of 8
questions to measure various the samples various demographic
characteristics (e.g., management level, gender, age, nationality,
earnings, and work experience with their current organization).
Additionally, it was believed that the questionnaire should be
administered across two nationalities to control for and identify
variations in work-related values that weren't captured by the
Hofstede Value Survey Module. Further, any supportive evidence obtained
across the two cultures would provide a stronger case for a
generalization of the findings.
The questionnaire was developed in English and pilot tested using
10 employees of a local manufacturing firm. Adjustments were made to
some of the wording and then the questionnaire was translated into
German. The German version was back translated into English by the
"Sprachdienst Gehring" company which specializes in
translations of materials for the German business world. After comparing
the back-translated version with the original English version, there
were some wording adjustments made to the instructions within the final
German questionnaire. Subsequently, the questionnaire was developed for
administration via a website to ensure employee confidentiality.
Human Resource managers at 10 US and 8 German companies requested
their employees voluntarily participate in this on-line survey as part
of ongoing local industry-university research projects. Multiple
companies were used to reduce the possible effect of organizational
cultures. The survey/questionnaire was made available via two university
websites for a two week period. Although the survey approach and
sampling method is suitable for testing the hypotheses, the methodology
presents some clear limitations regarding response rate and control. For
example, since the questionnaire was administered via a website, it has
an unknown response rate and therefore could be subject to various
respondent biases. Further, since the questionnaire responses were
recorded anonymously via a website, it was not possible to separate or
control the results by organization and, as such the results might have
an organizational bias. Also, because the questionnaire was administered
to companies within two specific geographic areas, the responses might
have a local bias.
RESULTS
The web survey/questionnaire approach to data collection resulted
in 233 usable responses; 104 German workers (60 men and 44 women) and
129 US workers (55 men and 74 women). The individual scores for each of
the cultural dimensions (e.g., MAS, LTO, IDV, UNC, PDI) exhibited
equality of variance and were then partitioned into quartiles by
nationality. The high and low quartiles were used for hypothesis
testing. Although it significantly reduced the sample size, it is
considered an appropriate approach for exploratory research when it is
believed that character extremes might provide the most visible
reactions (Judd et al., 1991). Also, the aggregated mean scores of the
expectancy, instrumentality, and valence questions were used to test the
hypotheses via correlation analysis. In addition, individual E, I, and V
questions were examined for significance in an attempt to build a usable
evaluation instrument.
The results of the hypothesis testing for the relationship between
culture-based perceptions and expectancies revealed that none of the
U.S. employees' expectancies were significantly related to
culture-based perceptions and values. The German employees, however, had
two culture-based value systems that related to perceptions of
expectancies. The uncertainty avoidance dimension was significantly
related to their perception of successfully accomplishing the tasks
(r=.28 @ p<.05). Most striking in this area was the belief that
organizational change was not a positive influence to increasing
performance (-.50 @ p<.01). Also, the power distance dimension was
significantly related to how they perceived their ability to
successfully accomplish tasks (r=.31@ p<.06). The question that was
the most striking in this area was that a close working relationship
with their supervisor was important to successfully accomplishing tasks
(.50 @ p<.01). There were, however, several other expectancy-related
questions that provided some expected and unexpected results. As
expected, the power distance dimension index was negatively correlated (.29 @ p<.06) with their willingness to voice dissatisfaction with
unsatisfactory working conditions; this was only significant for US
workers. Unexpectedly, however, the individualism dimension index was
negatively correlated (-.27 @ p<.08) with the employees' belief
that performance is best performed in groups; this, however, was only
significant for the German workers. One might have expected this result
for US workers, since the US has the highest cultural index of
individualism in the world.
The results of the hypothesis testing for the relationship between
culture-based perceptions and expectancies revealed that none of the
German employees' instrumentalities were significantly related to
culture-based perceptions and values. The US employees, however, had two
culture-based value systems that related to perceptions of
instrumentalities. The masculinity dimension was significantly related
to their perception of the probability that successfully accomplishing
the task would lead to rewards or desired outcomes (r=.34 @ p< .02).
Most striking in this area was the belief that competition is more
likely to result in better performance and rewards (r=.63 @ p<.01).
Additionally, and most surprisingly, the individualism dimension was
negatively related to the workers' perception that the successful
completion of tasks lead to rewards (r=-.44 @ p< .01). The question
that was most striking in this area was their belief that promotions
were based on skills and abilities (r=-.53 @ p<.01). Also,
interestingly, there was a significant disconnect between how the US and
German workers' perceived the influence of power distance on
instrumentality. The German workers believed that if they voiced
dissatisfaction with unsatisfactory working conditions, the conditions
would be changed (r=.39 @ p<.02). The US workers, however, had the
opposite reaction (r=-.28 @ p<.07). In other words, the Germans
believed that the higher the power distance, the more likely their voice
would be heard. On the other hand, the US employees believe that the
lower the power distance, the more likely their voice would be heard.
The results of the hypothesis testing for the relationship between
culture-based perceptions and valences revealed that three of the
culture-based perceptions were related for the US workers; uncertainty
avoidance (r=-.26 @ p<.06), individualism (r=-39 @ p<.01) and
masculinity (r=.55 @ .01). On the other hand, only one dimension of
culture-based perceptions was related to the German's desire of
valences; masculinity (r=.46 @ p<.01. Tables 1 and 2 indicate the
significant relationships between the cultural dimensions and
employees' valences by question and nationality. As expected,
employee scores on power distance, individualism, and masculinity had
the most correlation with their desire for certain outcomes. In
addition, there were several interesting relationships worth noting. For
example, higher scores on individualism were inversely related to the
need for personal time in both cultures. Also, particularly noteworthy
was that the need for personal time was negatively correlated for US
workers on each dimension. As expected, both cultures had a negative
correlation between the need for a good working relationship and power
distance. Unexpectedly, however, the need for variety and adventure on
the job were oppositely correlated for the cultures on the individualism
dimension. In other words, the US employees high in individualism
preferred not to have adventure on the job whereas the German employees
high on IDV preferred to have it. One might expect this type of
dichotomous result to occur in the relationship between the need for
variety/adventure on the job and the uncertainty avoidance dimension.
Hofstede's (1984) findings suggest that the US is significantly
lower on the uncertainty avoidance dimension and therefore one might
expect that they would have significant differences about adventure on
the job; however, none existed. Lastly, the need for job security was
surprisingly correlated with the masculinity dimension for both
cultures.
The entire samples (non-partitioned) were used to evaluate the
demographic factors. Importantly, the findings indicate that several of
the culture-based values and perceptions varied significantly by
demographic factors. For example, women were higher on uncertainty
avoidance (r=.139 @ p<.03) and higher on long-term orientation
(r=.139 @ p<.03). Also, as expected, age was positively correlated
with uncertainty avoidance (r=.402 @ p<.01). Having a university
degree was negatively correlated with uncertainty avoidance (r=.325 @
p<.02) and positively correlated with workers in the service sector
and those exhibiting masculinity (r=.317 @ p<.03). Only one of the
culture-based values systems varied significantly by nationality and
that was the long-term orientation dimension; the US was significantly
more short-term oriented than the German employees (p<.01). None of
the results varied significantly by salary, supervisory position or
years in the organization.
Although the Hofstede dimensions didn't vary much across the
two country samples, several of the demographic factors were
significantly different between the countries. For example, there were a
number of significant gender differences. First, the US women were
significantly higher on uncertainty avoidance (.177 @ .03),
individualism (.166 @ p<.06) and were more long term oriented (.148 @
p<.04) than US men. Surprisingly, there were no significant
differences between the German women and men. Further, there were
significant demographic differences in the work-rated needs between
employees of the two countries (Tables 3 & 4). For example, the
German workers didn't significantly value time for a personal or
family. On the other hand, the US workers had significant correlations
between personal time, gender and the work sector. As expected more
personal and family time was needed by women and those workers in the
manufacturing sector. Also, the German workers didn't significantly
value good working conditions. The US workers' value for good
working conditions was negatively correlated with years in the
organization. The workers of both countries seemed to value a good
working relationship with their supervisor, but often on different
demographic measures. For example, US workers had a positive correlation between the supervisor relationship and years in the organization and
supervisor rank and a negative correlation with gender.
The German workers had a positive correlation with years in the
organization, university degree and a negative correlation with the
manufacturing sector. The US workers had a positive correlation between
need for security and the manufacturing sector and a negative
correlation with gender. Conversely, the German workers had a negative
correlation between security and the manufacturing sector. US workers
with university degrees believed that cooperation amongst fellow workers
was important. Similarly, German workers with university degrees
believed in the importance of cooperation. On the other hand, the
importance of cooperation was inversely related to the supervisor's
rank. The US workers' gender and age were negatively correlated
with the need to be consulted by the superior in his or her decisions.
For the German workers, only pay grade was negatively correlated with a
need to be consulted. The US worker's had a number of factors that
correlated with a need for advancement; gender was positively correlated
while age, years in the organization and pay grade were negatively
correlated. For the German workers, only the manufacturing sector was
negatively correlated with the need for advancement. Lastly, the US
workers' need for variety/adventure on the job was positively
correlated with gender and negatively correlated with age, years in the
organization, pay grade and manufacturing sector. The German
workers' need for variety was negatively correlated with supervisor
rank and pay grade.
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS
The purpose of this study was to determine whether an
"employee-organization motivation fit" could be predicted
using an instrument normally reserved for measuring dimensions of a
society's culture. Specifically, the study examined the
relationship between an individual's Hofstede's cultural
dimensions (used as an indicator of individual culture-based perceptions
and values) and the elements of Vroom's Expectancy Theory to
determine whether one could predict motivation potential in various
work-related settings. To test the hypothesis of this relationship a
survey was developed and returned by 233 German and US employees.
The results of the survey data, aggregated across nationalities and
partitioned by quartile, failed to support any of the five hypothetical
relationships posited between an individual's culture-based values
and his/her perception of expectancies. Only two of the
individual's perceptions of instrumentalities appeared to have been
influenced by culture-based values (masculinity and uncertainty
avoidance). Similarly, only one of the individual's perceptions of
valences appeared to have been influenced by culture-based values
(masculinity).
Although there wasn't a lot of support for the hypotheses, a
number of questions did provide expected and unexpected significance and
implications for a culturally-based performance management system. For
example, there were two questions that significantly related power
distance to expectancy. The question that asked to what degree the
employees agreed with the statement that best performance is
accomplished in groups was correlated at r=.264 @ p<.01. It makes
sense that people with a high PDI like team work ("together we are
strong") and to work together with people and workers from the same
level. Additionally, people from high PDI cultures do not see themselves
as upwardly mobile and therefore are not in competition with their
colleagues; this characteristics typically increases the group harmony.
Similarly, the question that asked to what degree the employees agreed
with the statement that group rewards will be more effective than
individual rewards was correlated at r=.212 @ p<.01. This correlation
also makes sense--people with a high PDI prefer to work in a team and
think that team work and team rewards are more effective than single
rewards.
In addition to the questions that correlated uncertainty avoidance
with expectancy, there were several other motivational questions that
correlated with uncertainty avoidance. For example, the question that
asked to what degree the employees believed they could develop a close
working relationship with their co-workers was negatively correlated at
r=-.273 @ p<.01. This confirms Hofstede's (1994) notions that
employees with a high UNC don't believe in their ability to develop
close working relationships, while employees with a low UNC believe in
their flexibility to develop closer working relationships. The question
that asked the employees about their possibility of establishing a close
relationship to their supervisor was also negatively correlated at
r=.208 @ p<.01. Similarly, it is difficult for a high UNC employee to
believe in the possibility of establishing a relationship with his or
her superior because there are so many different uncertainties that
might influence this relationship in a negative way. Also, high UNC
employees have a strong belief in experts and require a high degree of
formalization (e.g., titles and chains of command) which naturally
create a gap between the supervisors and the employees. Also, the
question that asked the employees the degree to which they preferred
short-term over long-term results was significantly related to
one's uncertainty avoidance r=.226 @ p<.01. Expectedly, people
with a high UA prefer short-term results over long-term results, because
the longer the time until the result of an action becomes measurable,
the more anxiety and uncertainty there is about the success of the
action. Lastly, there were a number of questions regarding
instrumentalities that had negative correlations with the uncertainty
avoidance dimension. For example, the degree to which the employee
believes that if they exceeded job requirements their pay would increase
correlated at r=-.221 @ p<.01. The degree to which the employee
believes that if they exceeded job requirements they would be promoted
correlated at r=-.332 @ p<.01. The degree to which employee believes
their knowledge and skills lead to promotion correlated at r=-.287 @
p<.01. All of these relationships might have been expected because
individuals high in UA prefer rules, law, contract and unions. In other
words, these workers feel more comfortable if promotions or pay
increases are based on something as easily quantifiable and predictable
as seniority. The idea that a supervisor might have to subjectively
judge their performance makes them feel uneasy.
Although the hypothesis testing of the aggregate sample didn't
reveal much in the way of significance, the testing of the individual
national samples revealed some interesting relationships. For example,
the US employees (N=129) showed a significant relationship between
instrumentality and the dimensions of masculinity and individualism
(negative). The German employees (N=104) showed a significant
relationship between expectancy and the dimensions of power distance and
uncertainty avoidance (negative). The employees of both countries had
significant relationships between valence and a number of the
culturally-based questions (Tables 1 & 2). Particularly significant
for US employees were those valence questions relating to the
masculinity, individualism and power distance dimensional values. On the
other hand, the German employees' perception of valences
demonstrated more of a significant relationship with the power distance
and individualism dimensional values. Additionally, there were a number
of significant relationships between the valences and several of the
demographics by nationality (Tables 3 & 4). Again, these findings
present some interesting insights to how work-related needs vary by
demographic and as such, must be taken into consideration when
motivating individuals.
Lastly, the findings present an interesting puzzle. Some of the
results were significant by country, but not by Hofstede's cultural
dimensions. This seems to suggest that some other national influences
are being picked up that aren't measured directly by the Hofstede
dimensions. Likely candidates might be the country's views on labor
(e.g., unions) or various organizational cultures. For example, the
German sample had a much higher regard for the purpose of labor unions
than the US (South Carolina-a right to work state) sample. Additionally,
the sample of 18 organizations (10 US and 8 German) may not have been
large enough to reduce the effect of organizational culture on employee
perceptions. In any case, the study points out that the Hofstede
instrument isn't 'fine grained' enough to have much of a
value in predicting motivation.
CONCLUSIONS
Most managers will agree that the essence of management is to
influence employees to accomplish organizational goals. In other words,
the job of motivating one's employees to satisfy the needs of
internal and external customers. This task is even more important in
today's highly competitive, globalized environment where an
employee's daily performance is often the difference between
success and failure. As such, there is an increasing need for
organizational research on employee motivation.
Over the last decade, the research on methods to improve employee
or organizational motivation has taken two general paths: (1) creating
motivation, and (2) selecting motivation. The notion of creating
motivation suggests that a supervisor understand each employee's
needs (extrinsic and intrinsic) and then provide opportunities so that
he or she can satisfy those needs. While this research approach is the
most popular, it has provided little new information or improvement to
organizational productivity, satisfaction, and learning over the last
several years. The biggest organizational improvements appear to
becoming from new research on better selection methods. Specifically,
"getting the right people on the bus" (Collins, 2001) or
getting a maximum of "emotional intelligence" (Goleman, 1995).
A key factor in "getting the right people on the bus" is to
insure the employee's fit with the organization's motivation
systems. In other words, select only those employees that will be
motivated by the company's existing motivational systems. This
approach has several advantages over trying to tailor motivational
techniques to each worker. First, and foremost, it isn't dependent
upon the manager's skill at understanding the employee's needs
and the skill at providing opportunities to satisfy those needs. Second,
it allows the company to focus on being the best at providing a smaller
array of rewards. In turn, this permits the company to become the
employer of choice for a certain type of employee.
This study was designed to examine the question of whether
Hofstede's cultural dimensions can predict an individual's
preference for certain motivational environments. If an
individual's motivational needs can be accurately profiled using
Hofstede's cultural dimensions, it would be a significant
advancement to employee selection methods. Unfortunately, most of the
hypothesize relationships between culture-based value systems and
expectancies and instrumentalities were not supported. At first glance,
this suggests that the Hofstede survey is not a particularly good
instrument for predicting an individual's motivation within an
organization. However, one's motivation is the product of three
factors (E * I * V = Mp) and many of the relationships between
culture-based values and valences were supported. This is important
because it suggests that an organization that has knowledge of an
individual's valences can create a better employee-organization fit
through selection and improve motivation and productivity by offering a
tailored or cafeteria style rewards system. As such, this research
underscores the role that culture-based selection might play in
improving motivation (Tables 3 & 4).
Future research should continue to examine the relationship between
culturally-driven values and perceptions and the factors of motivation.
While the Hofstede instrument proved ineffective at predicting an
employee's motivation, the notion of predicting motivation through
a candidate's expectancies, instrumentalities and valences remains
sound. As such, future research might develop an instrument that
measures an employee's Es, Is, and Vs in various work scenarios.
REFERENCES
Albers-Miller, N. D. & Gelb, B. D. (1996). Business advertising
appeals as a mirror of cultural dimensions: A study of eleven countries.
Journal of Advertising, 25(4), 57-71.
Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming--Basic considerations for a
psychology of personality. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Andre, J., D.A. Owens & L.O. Harvey (2003). Visual perception.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Collins, J. C. (2001). Good to great: Why some companies make the
leap ... and others don't. New York: HarperCollins
Cook, M (1998). Personnel selection (Third Edition). Chichester:
Wiley.
Freud, S. (1963). The problem of anxiety. New York: The
Psychoanalytic Quarterly Press and W.W. Norton & Company Inc.
Gagne, M. & E. L. Deci. (2005). Self-determination theory and
work motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32, 331-363.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
29
Hanisch, D. A. & Q. Han. (2003). Fuhrungsverstandnis
chinesischer Manager im Wandel. Personal, 1, 100-23.
Henle, C. A. (2005). Predicting workplace deviance from the
interaction between organizational justice and personality. Journal of
Managerial Issues, 17(2)247-64
Hofestede, G. (1984). Culture's Consequences (Fifth Edition).
London: Sage Publications.
Hofstede, G. (1994). Cultures and organizations--intercultural
cooperation and its importance for survival (software for the mind).
London: Harper Collins Business.
Hubbell, A. & P. R Chory-Assad (2005). Motivating factors:
perceptions of justice and their relationship with managerial and
organizational trust. Communication Studies, 56(1), 47-71.
Judd, C. M., E. R. Smith & L. H. Kidder (1991). Research
methods in social relations (Sixth Edition). Chicago: Holt, Rinehard
& Winston, Inc.
Maslow, A. H. (1987). Motivation and Personality. New York: R.R.
Donnelley & Sons Company.
McClelland, D. C. (1962). Business drive and national achievement,
Harvard Business Review, July-August, 99-112.
McCrae, R. R. & P. T., Costa, Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
Merton, R. R. (1968). Social theory and social structure (Second
Edition). New York: Free Press.
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and Motivation. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Wallace, A. (1970). Culture and personality. New York: Random
House.
Charles R. Emery, Erskine College Simon Oertel, University of
Applied Sciences Trier
Table 1: The correlation between cultural-based values and work-related
needs of the US workers
Needs Question PDI UNC IDV
Time for personal -.37 ** -.37 ** -.83 **
Good physical working -.26 *
conditions
Good working relationship -.42 **
with direct superior
Employment security .47 **
Cooperation amongst
fellow employees
Consulted by your superior .68 **
in his/her decisions
Opportunity for .34 *
advancement
Variety and adventure -.41 **
on the job
Needs Question MAS LTO
Time for personal .52 **
Good physical working .51 **
conditions
Good working relationship .71 **
with direct superior
Employment security .61 **
Cooperation amongst .80 **
fellow employees
Consulted by your superior
in his/her decisions
Opportunity for .26 * .41 **
advancement
Variety and adventure
on the job
* Correlation is significant at p<.05
** Correlation is significant at p<.01
Table 2: The correlation between cultural-based values and work-related
needs of the German workers
Needs Question PDI UNC IDV
Time for personal -.75 **
Good physical working .61 **
conditions
Good working relationship -.74 ** .43 **
with direct superior
Employment security .48 **
Cooperation amongst -.30 * .28 *
fellow employees
Consulted by your superior .59 **
in his/her decisions
Opportunity for advancement
Variety and adventure on the job .33 *
Needs Question MAS LTO
Time for personal
Good physical working
conditions
Good working relationship
with direct superior
Employment security .33 *
Cooperation amongst .65 **
fellow employees
Consulted by your superior
in his/her decisions
Opportunity for advancement
Variety and adventure on the job
* Correlation is significant at p<.05
** Correlation is significant at p<.01
Table 3: Correlation between demographic variables and
work-related needs of US workers
Needs Question Gender Age Yrs Univ
Org. Deg.
Time for personal or family life .30
Good physical working conditions -.24
Good working relation with direct
superior -.17 .28
Employment security -.18
Cooperation amongst fellow
employees .26
Consulted by superior in his/her
decisions -.22 -.25
Opportunity for advancement .20 -.23 -.23
Variety and adventure on the job .23 -.28 -.29
Correlations are significant at p<.01
Needs Question Supv Pay Mfg
Rank Grade
Time for personal or family life .29
Good physical working conditions
Good working relation with direct
superior -.27
Employment security .23
Cooperation amongst fellow
employees
Consulted by superior in his/her
decisions
Opportunity for advancement -.26
Variety and adventure on the job -.33 -.27
Correlations are significant at p<.01
Table 4: Correlation between demographic variables and
work-related needs of German workers
Needs Question Gender Age Yrs Univ
Org Deg
Time for personal or family life
Good physical working conditions
Good working relationship with direct 0.32 0.33
superior
Employment security
Cooperation amongst fellow employees 0.22
Consulted by your superior in his/her
decisions
Opportunity for advancement
Variety and adventure on the job
Correlations are significant at p<.01
Needs Question Supv Pay Mfg
Rank Grade
Time for personal or family life
Good physical working conditions
Good working relationship with direct -0.2
superior
Employment security -0.3
Cooperation amongst fellow employees -0.26
Consulted by your superior in his/her -0.27
decisions
Opportunity for advancement -0.19
Variety and adventure on the job -0.31 -0.29
Correlations are significant at p<.01