Jungian personality types and leadership styles: an empirical examination.
Thomson, Neal F. ; Gopalan, Suresh
ABSTRACT
This paper proposes that personality types have an effect on the
types of leader behaviors that an individual believes to be appropriate.
This belief will impact actual leader behaviors. A review of the
literature on Jungian personality types is conducted, followed by a
description of leader behaviors using the Ohio State Framework, as
modified by Schriesheim. The nature of these relationships and their
potential impact on the workplace are discussed. The model is then
tested on 128 actual supervisory employees, with results and
implications presented.
INTRODUCTION
Research in the area of leadership has shown that leader behaviors
are an important field of study (Bass, 1990; Tucker and Russell, 2004;
Skinner and Spurgeon, 2005). Leader personality variables have been
shown to impact organizational performance (Hough and Ogilvie, 2005).
This paper looks at leader behaviors, using a modified version of the
Ohio State leadership framework, and examines the role of personality,
as conceptualized by Carl Jung (Jung 1923; Bridges, 1992) on perceptions
of appropriate leader behaviors. We hypothesize that the personality of
an individual will have a noticeable impact on their behavioral
intentions, and therefore, on the actual behaviors that they engage in.
We conclude with a discussion of the impact that these relationships may
have in the workplace and offer suggestions for future research.
PERSONALITY
The Keirsey Temperament Sorter II consists of four scales, based on
Jung's (1923) Theory of Type. This theory is widely accepted,
enjoys extensive support, and is applied through several questionnaires
which are used to measure the scales. These scales include the
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and the Keirsey Temperament sorter (Gauld
and Sink, 1985; Gardner and Martinko, 1996; Sample, 2004). The four
scales used by both of these are Introversion/Extroversion (I/E),
Sensation/Intuition (S/N), Thinking/Feeling (T/F), and
Judgment/Perception (J/P). The assumption underlying these scales is
that "people have a preference for one or the other pole on each of
four indices, analogous to a natural preference for right- or
left-handedness" (Carlyn, 1977, p.461). The first scale I/E refer
to the preference for either the outer world, or internal, mental
structures and processes (Hall and Norby, 1973; Myers, 1993).
The next two scales refer to Jung's (1923) four psychological
functions. The first pair, (S/N) refers to a preferred method of
perceiving things. Sensing types rely on direct information from the
sense organs, or the concrete details of the situation, whereas
Intuitive types deal with hunches, possibilities, abstract meanings, and
leaps of faith (Carlyn, 1977; Myers, 1993; Opt and Loffredo, 2000). This
pair was called the irrational functions, not because they are contrary
to reason, but because they do not involve reasoning (Hall and Norby,
1973; Myers, 1993). The second pair, T/F, is called the rational
functions because they involve reasoned decisions. Thinking types prefer
decisions on whether there is a true connection between two or more
ideas, and Feeling types make decisions as to whether an idea is
pleasing or distasteful, beautiful or ugly, exciting or dull (Hall and
Norby, 1973; Wheeler, Hunter and Bryant, 2004).
The fourth scale, J/P, never explicitly defined by Jung, but
implicit in his writings, refers to an individual's preferred way
of dealing with the outer world. Judging types prefer a planned orderly
environment, and want to regulate and control life. Perceptive types are
more curious and open minded, preferring adapting to life and responding
spontaneously and flexibly (Carlyn, 1977; Rosenak and Shontz, 1988).
LEADER BEHAVIOR
A popular model used in leader behavior research is the one
proposed by Ohio State Leadership studies. This model suggests that
there are two dimensions of leader behaviors: initiating structure and
consideration (Hemphill and Coons, 1957; Halpin and Winer, 1957;
Fleishman, 1957; Seeman, 1957). House and Dessler (1974) suggested that
the initiating structure dimension (also called instrumental behavior)
is a multidimensional construct, and cannot be adequately measured using
a single unidimensional scale. Therefore, the initiating structure
dimension was divided into the component parts identified by House and
Dessler (1974) and later confirmed by Jermier and Burkes (1979),
Schriesheim, House and Kerr (1976), and Schriesheim, Cogliser and Neider
(1995).
The initiating structure dimension, as originally defined by Halpin
and Winer (1957) and Hemphill and Coon (1957) refers to leader behavior
which helps to define the relationship between leader and subordinate,
and the degree to which a leader originates, facilitates or resists new
ideas and practices. However, this dimension can be divided into three
distinct categories of leader behaviors: role clarification, work
assignment and specification of procedures (House and Dessler, 1974;
Schriesheim, House and Kerr, 1976). Each of these types of behavior has
been shown to be viewed differently by subordinates, and to have
different effects on related organizational variables (Schriesheim and
Bish, 1974; Schriesheim, Cogliser and Neider, 1995). The three types of
initiating behavior were defined by Schriesheim (1978, pg. 49) as
follows:
1. Leader role clarification: Leader behaviors which clarify what
is expected of subordinates in their work roles.
2. Leader work assignment: Leader Behaviors which involve the
assigning of subordinates to specific tasks.
3. Leader specification of procedures: Leader behaviors which
specify rules, procedures and methods for subordinates to use or follow
in the execution of their jobs.
The consideration variable also referred to as supportive
leadership (House and Dessler, 1974), is characterized as behaviors
which make a person "friendly and approachable, and considerate of
the needs of subordinates" (p. 41). Others have associated this
dimension with terms such as "humanitarian and helpful"
(Haccoun, Haccoun and Sallay, 1978). Templer defined consideration as an
"orientation towards the people in an organization" (1973,
p.359). As Schriesheim (1978, 1995) points out, this variable has been
found by the literature to be unconfounded and unidimensional.
Schriesheim's conclusion have been supported by other leadership
studies (Judge, Piccolo and Ilies, 2004).
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PERSONALITY AND LEADER BEHAVIORS
In this section, we discuss the possible relationships between each
dimension of Jung's theory of type and leader behaviors. We
hypothesize that relationships may exist between each of these
personality dimensions, and one or more dimensions of leader behaviors.
The first personality dimension is Introversion/Extraversion. This
dimension, in general, indicates a preference for either working alone,
or in small groups (introversion) or working with many others
(extraversion). Two relationships are likely to be related to this
preference. First, extroverts may be more likely to engage in hands-on
leadership (specification of procedures). Second, since extroverts enjoy
dealing with people and are more focused on the world around then, they
tend to give more attention to relationships. Therefore, the following
two propositions are made:
P1: Extroverts will be more likely to engage in specification of
procedures, than introverts.
P2: Extroverts will be more likely to engage in consideration
behaviors, than introverts.
The second dimension of type is Sensing/Intuition. Sensing types
prefer the concrete; that which they can see, hear, feel, smell and
taste. They also are more likely to pay attention to their immediate
environment, and spend less time "with their head in the
clouds." This preference may cause Sensing types to focus more on
job-related behaviors, rather than relationship building, since the
latter has long-term effects. Therefore we propose that:
P3: Sensing types will be more likely to engage in work assignment,
role clarification and specification of procedures than Intuitives
P4: Intuitives will be more likely to engage in consideration, or
relationship building behaviors, than sensing types.
The third dimension, Thinking/Feeling is likely to be related to
the use of consideration, or relationship oriented behavior. Individuals
who are categorized as feeling, are likely to be more attuned to the
emotions of others, and therefore may be more likely to engage in
consideration behaviors. Therefore, we propose:
P5: Feeling types will be more likely to engage in Consideration
behaviors than Thinking types. The last dimension, Judging/Perceiving
relates to a preference for order and/or a planned environment versus a
preference for spontaneity and unplanned action. Therefore, by
definition a Judging person would prefer things to be well planned, and
therefore is likely to engage in specification of procedures, work
assignment and role clarification. Therefore, we propose:
P6: Judging types will be more likely to engage in role
clarification, job assignment, and specification of procedures, than
perceiving types.
SAMPLE
In order to test the above propositions, we administered a series
of questionnaires to a sample of 128 individuals employed in supervisory
positions at a light manufacturing firm, located in the southeastern
United States. The sample consisted of 85 males, and 43 females. The
minimum age was 23, and the maximum was 61, with a mean age of 39.83.
Organizational tenure varied from a low of 1 month, to a high of 33
years, with a mean level of 10.86 years. Experience in a supervisory
position ranges from a low of zero, to a high of 25 years, with a mean
of 4.78 years. Of the 128 respondents in the sample, 44 identified
themselves as white, 73 as black or African-American, 4 as Hispanic, two
as "other" and 5 did not identify any ethnic/racial identity.
INSTRUMENTS
Leader behaviors were measured using a modified version of a
questionnaire developed by Schriesheim (1978). This questionnaire
divides the initiating structure dimension of leadership into three
separate dimensions consisting of role clarification, work assignment
and specification of procedures. Due to the length of the original
questionnaire (80 items) a short scale was developed. Based on
Schriesheim's (1978) study, eight items from each scale that had
the highest correlation with the scale score were chosen. The original
questionnaire was developed to measure subordinate assessments of leader
behavior. For our study, we modified the instruction set based on the
LBDQ ideal to assess subjects' behavioral intentions as leaders.
The Jungian personality or type variables were measured using the
Keirsey Temperament Sorter II. This instrument has undergone a
significant level of testing and validation and is shown to be a
reliable and valid measure of Jungian type (Keirsey.com, 2002). This
questionnaire includes seventy questions, which measure the four
personality dimensions (types) discussed in earlier sections.
STUDY
As a first step, we developed specific hypotheses based on the
above propositions. Since our propositions were quite specific, we faced
little difficulty in transforming each of these into a testable
hypothesis. Next, to test the hypotheses, we conducted a field survey
using questionnaires to collect data from individuals working in
supervisory positions. Our hypotheses are as follows:
H1: The Introversion/Extraversion scale will be positively
correlated with the specification of procedures scale.
H2: The Introversion/Extraversion scale will be positively
correlated with the consideration scale.
H3: The Sensing/Intuition scale will be positively correlated with
the work assignment, role clarification, and specification of procedures
scale.
H4: The Sensing/Intuition scale will be negatively correlated with
the consideration scale.
H5: The thinking/Feeling scale will be negatively correlated with
the consideration scale.
H6a: The Judging/Perceiving scale will be positively correlated to
the work assignment scale.
H6b: The Judging/Perceiving scale will be positively correlated to
the role clarification scale.
H6c: The Judging/Perceiving scale will be positively correlated to
specification of procedures scale.
RESULTS
The questionnaires were administered to 128 supervisors working in
a manufacturing organization who were attending a management-training
seminar. The response rate was 100% due to the nature of interaction
that the researchers had with the respondents and the ability to
follow-up with no difficulty. The data were collected over a five-day
period, in groups of approximately 25-30 people. Anonymity was ensured
by instructing the participants not to write their names or any type of
information that might reveal their identity. Data analysis was
performed using a SPSSpc v11.0. The GLM procedure was used to specify
personality variables as independent variables and leadership variables
as dependent variables respectively. Initial examination of the data
indicated that two of the surveyed dimensions, role clarification and
job assignment were highly inter-correlated. Using factor analysis we
collapsed these two dimensions to create a new variable that we refer to
as 'directing behavior.' However, for the purposes of this
study, this change is of minimal importance, as the hypothesized
relationships between this dimension (directing behavior) and
personality is not supported (see results in Tables 1, 2, 3).
The multivariate tests supported a relationship between leadership
and one of the personality variables--Judging/Perceiving. The Wilkes
lambdas for the other three personality variables did not indicate
significance at the multivariate level. Based on these results,
Hypotheses 6a and 6c are fully supported while others are not supported.
In addition to the multivariate test indicating that there is a
significant effect, the univariate effects are both significant and in
the predicted direction. Results from this study indicate that Judging
types are more likely to engage in both general direction, for example
the assignment of a goal or task, and the more scrutinizing
specification of procedures. Full multivariate and univariate results
are reported in the tables below.
IMPLICATIONS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
This study suggests that people who are diagnosed as a
"judging type" are likely to engage in greater frequency of
general supervision, as well as specification of procedures, or close
leadership. While the first tendency is a good one, as it may lead to
higher performance, the second, specification of procedure does not seem
to be a positive leader behavior. Similar in construct to "close
leadership" this variable focuses on the degree to which a manager
forces his/her process and specific techniques on the employee, limiting
their job autonomy. Many leadership studies (most of them conducted in
the United States) have suggested that "close" leader behavior
leads to lowered satisfaction, without enhancing performance (Brown,
1982). Leaders who exhibited high levels of specification of procedure
behavior experienced lower employee job satisfaction and performance.
As Hofstede (1993) has stated, the bulk of leadership and
management studies have been conducted in the United States and other
Western countries where the dominant culture favors lower power distance
and higher degree of comfort with uncertainty. In such cultures, people
prefer higher degrees of autonomy and flexibility and resist a rigid
supervisory style. Naturally, people are uncomfortable with a close
leadership style.
With increased diversity and globalization, one logical question
would be if the "judging type" leaders with their desire for
close and directive supervision would be effective in other cultures
that are different than Western cultures. Gopalan and Rivera (1997)
conclude that in India, the most effective leader was one who behaved
like a "benevolent" patriarch with a high degree of
supervision and structure towards their subordinates (similar to that of
a "father-son" relationship). Similar types of leader behavior
were found in countries like Turkey, Mexico, etc. where the prevailing
cultures had high power distance and low tolerance for uncertainty
(Romero, 2004; Pasa, 2000). Under these scenarios, subordinates are
expected to be dependent on their leaders for all aspects of work. This
raises the possibility that "judging type" leaders who specify
procedures may be more effective in some cultures than others. Clearly,
additional research has to be conducted in other countries to answer
this question. One of the few studies conducted, comparing leadership
behaviors across western (Australia) and eastern (Hong Kong) cultures
(Lok and Crawford, 2004) found that culture did have an impact on the
effectiveness of leader behaviors. However, the limited scope of this
study (2 countries) leaves open much room for further research.
Another limitation of our study is that it was based in a
manufacturing setting--our respondents were stating ideal leadership
behaviors that were most compatible in this setting. It is entirely
possible that different types of personality and leadership behaviors
may be effective in non-manufacturing settings. As an example, Skinner
and Spurgeon (2005), cite the growing importance of empathy and
emotional intelligence as important leader behavioral traits to be
effective in a health care setting. We recommend additional studies
explore the personality-leader behavior relationship in a variety of
occupational settings.
We would also encourage future studies to study the moderating
impact of demographic variables such as age, education, and gender which
were found to impact leadership behaviors and styles. According to Somech (2003), large disparities in age and education between leaders
and subordinates results in non-participative leadership, poorer
articulation of goals and performance strategies, unclear task-oriented
communication, and decreased frequency of communication. The
relationship between leader personality and behavior has a great deal of
potential importance to the fields of leadership and management. Bass
(1990) and Dansereau, Alutto, and Yammarino (1984) have concluded that
consideration behaviors are likely to lead to increased employee
satisfaction, while behaviors such as role clarification, and job
assignment have a positive impact on job performance. From an
organizational perspective, there is great interest in gaining a greater
understanding of what personality types lead to greater frequency of
specific behaviors. While it certainly would be premature to suggest
that personality should be used for screening purposes, an instrument
like the Keirsey temperament sorter may be valuable in selecting
participants for management training, particularly in topics relating to leadership style, or leader behaviors.
REFERENCES
Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass and Stogdill's handbook of
leadership. New York: Free Press.
Bridges, W. (1992). The character of organizations: Using Jungian
type in organizational development. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Brown, D. (1982). The Fifth Freedom: Freedom from Supervision.
Journal of Systems Management, 33(1), 6-11.
Carlyn, M. (1977). An Assessment of the Myers-Briggs Type
Indicator. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 461-473.
Dansereau, F., J. A. Alutto, & F. J. Yammarino (1984). Theory
testing in organizational behavior. The varient approach. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Fleishman, E.A. (1957). A leader behavior description for industry.
In R. Stogdill and A. Coons (Eds.,), Leader Behavior: Its Description
and Measurement. Columbus, Ohio: Bureau of Business Research.
Gardner, W. L., & M. J. Martinko (1996). Using Myers-Briggs
Type indicator to study managers: A literature review. Journal of
Management, 22(1), 45-83.
Gauld, V., & D. Sink (1985). The MBTI as a diagnostic tool in
organization development interventions. Journal of Psychological Type,
9, 24-29.
Gopalan, S., & J. Rivera (1997). Gaining a perspective on
Indian value orientations: Implications for expatriate managers. The
International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 5(2), 156-179.
Haccoun, D.M., R. R. Haccoun, & G. Sallay (1978). Sex
differences in the appropriateness of supervisory styles: A
nonmanagement view. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63(1), 124-127.
Hall, S.C. & V. J. Norby. (1973). A Primer of Jungian
Psychology. New York, NY: New American Library.
Halpin, A.W. and B. J. Winer (1957). A factorial study of the
leader behavior descriptions. In R. Stogdill and A. Coons, (Eds.,),
Leader Behavior: Its Description and Measurement. Columbus, Ohio: Bureau
of Business Research.
Hemphill, J.K., and A. E. Coons (1957). Development of the leader
behavior description questionnaire. In R. Stogdill and A. Coons,
(Eds.,), Leader Behavior: Its Description and Measurement. Columbus,
Ohio: Bureau of Business Research.
Hofstede, G. (1993). Cultural constraints in management theories.
Academy of Management Executive, 7, 81-94.
Judge, T., R. Piccolo and R. Ilies (2004) The Forgotten Ones? The
Validity of Consideration and Initiating Structure in Leadership
Research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(1), 36-51.
Hough, J and D. Ogilvie (2005) An Empirical Test of Cognitinve
Style and Strategic Decision Outcomes. The Journal of Management
Studies, 42(2), 417-448.
House, R.J. and G. Dessler (1974). The path-goal theory of
leadership: some post hoc and a priori tests. In J.G. Hunt and L. L.
Anderson, (Eds.,), Contingency Approaches to Leadership. Carbondale, IL:
Southern Illinois.
Jermier, J.M., and L.J. Berkes (1979). Leader behavior in a police
command bureaucracy: A closer look at the quasi-military model.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 1-23.
Jung, C.G. (1923) Psychological Types. London: Routledge and Kegan
Paul.
Lok, P., and J. Crawford (2004). The Effect of Organisational
Culture and Leadership Style on Job Satisfaction and Organisational
Commitment: A Cross-national Comparison. Journal of Management
Development, 23(4), 321-338.
Myers, I. (1993) Gifts Differing. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Pyschologists Press.
Opt, S., and D. Loffredo (2003). Communicator Image and
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Extraversion-Introversion. The Journal of
Psychology, 137(6), 560-568.
Pasa, S. F. (2001). Leadership influence in a high power distance
and collectivist culture. Leadership & Organization Development
Journal, 21(8), 414-426
Romero, E. J. (2004). Latin American Leadership: El Patron & El
Lider Moderno. Cross Cultural Management, 11(3), 25-37.
Rosenak, C. And Shontz, F. (1988) Jungian Q-Sorts: Demonstrating
Construct Validity for Psychological Type and the MBTI. Journal of
Psychological Type, 15, 33-45.
Sample. J. (2004). The Myers-Briggs Type Indicator and OD:
Implications for Practice and Research. Organizational Development
Journal, 22(1), 67-75.
Schriesheim, C.A. (1978). Development, validation and application
of new leadership behavior and expectancy research instruments.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State University.
Schriesheim, C.A., & L. Bish (1974). The content validity of
form XII of the ohio state leadership scales. Presented at National
Academy of Management Conference, Seattle.
Schriesheim, C.A., R. J. House & S. Kerr (1976). Leader
initiating structure: a reconciliation of discrepant research results
and some empirical tests. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
15, 297-321.
Schriesheim, C. A., C. C. Cogliser, & L. L. Neider (1995). Is
it 'trustworthy?' A multiple levels-of-analysis reexamination of an Ohio State leadership study, with implications for future
research. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 111-145.
Seeman, M. (1957). A comparison of general and specific leader
behavior descriptions. In R. Stogdill and A. Coons, (Eds.), Leader
Behavior: Its Description and Measurement. Columbus, Ohio: Bureau of
Business Research.
Skinner, C., and P. Spurgeon (2005). Valuing empathy and emotional
intelligence in health leadership: a study of empathy, leadership, and
outcome effectiveness. Health Services Management Research, 18, 1-12.
Somech, A. (2003). Relationships of participative leadership with
relational demography variables: a multi-level perspective. Journal of
Organizational Behavior, 24(8), 1003-1018.
Templer, A.J. (1973). Self-perceived and other perceived leadership
style using the leader behavior description questionnaire. Personnel
Psychology, 26, 359-367.
Tucker, A. B., & R. F. Russell (2004). The influence of the
Transformational Leader. Journal of Leadership & Organizational
Studies, 10(4), 103-111.
Wheeler, P., J. Hunton and S. Bryant (2004) Accounting Information
Systems Research Opportunities Using Personality Type Theory and the
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Journal of Information Systems, 18(1) 1-19.
Neal F. Thomson, Columbus State University Suresh Gopalan,
Winston-Salem State University
Table 1: Descriptive Statistics
Mean Std. N
Deviation
GENERAL DIRECTION 4.19189 .401169 128
CONSIDERATION 4.0244 .50880 128
SPECIFICATION OF PROCEDURES 4.2061 .47904 128
N Mean Std.
Deviation
I/E 132 5.04 2.116
S/N 132 13.76 2.151
T/F 132 9.41 3.636
J/P 132 8.42 2.490
Table 2: Multivariate Tests
Effect Value F
Intercept Wilks' Lambda .314 87.932
I/E Wilks' Lambda .989 .448
S/N Wilks' Lambda .999 .044
T/F Wilks' Lambda .996 .172
J/P Wilks' Lambda .938 2.689
Hypothesis df Error df Sig.
Intercept 3.000 121.000 .000
I/E 3.000 121.000 .719
S/N 3.000 121.000 .988
T/F 3.000 121.000 .915
J/P 3.000 121.000 .049
Table 3: Tests of Between-Subjects Effects
Source Dependent Type III Sum df
Variable of Squares
Corrected Model GD 1.35 4
CONS 4.679E-02 4
SOP 1.178 4
Intercept GD 36.244 1
CONS 28.305 1
SOP 36.425 1
I/E GD 9.549E-02 1
CONS 2.753E-02 1
SOP 6.441E-02 1
S/N GD 8.687E-03 1
CONS 4.202E-03 1
CLOSE 7.928E-04 1
T/F GD 2.184E-02 1
CONS 1.049E-02 1
CLOSE 7.819E-02 1
J/P GD 1.109 1
CONS 4.859E-03 1
CLOSE .820 1
Dependent Mean Square F Sig.
Variable
GD 0.338 2.177 .075
CONS 1.17E-02 .044 .996
SOP .294 1.295 .276
GD 36.244 233.552 .000
CONS 28.305 106.045 .000
SOP 36.425 160.208 .000
GD 9.549E-02 .615 .434
CONS 2.753E-02 .103 .749
SOP 6.441E-03 .028 .867
GD 8.687E-03 .056 .813
CONS 4.202E-03 .016 .900
CLOSE 7.928E-04 .003 .953
GD 2.184E-02 .141 .708
CONS 1.049E-02 .039 .843
CLOSE 7.819E-02 .344 .559
GD 1.109 7.143 .009
CONS 4.859E-03 .018 .893
CLOSE .820 3.605 .060