Silent messages in negotiations: the role of nonverbal communication in cross-cultural business negotiations.
Chu, Yun ; Strong, William F. ; Ma, Jianyu 等
ABSTRACT
This study specifically explored the perceived importance of the
following nonverbal factors in the negotiation process: proxemics (location and negotiation site), physical arrangement (seating and
furniture arrangement), and kinesics (eye contact, facial expressions
and gestures). The participants are professional business negotiators of
different nationalities. The findings show that the negotiators'
perception about the three factors and their roles in negotiation are
consistent with the nonverbal communication literature.
INTRODUCTION
Negotiation with China is a topic which has received more and more
attention in recent years (Chang, 2003; Palich, Carini, &
Livingstone, 2002; Tinsley & Pillutla, 1998, Chen & Faure 1995,
Leung & Yeung, 1995; Pye, 1982; Gordon, 1986). Although it has been
studied in terms of negotiation styles (Pye, 1982) and intercultural
differences (Mente, 1992; Gordon, 1986), there has been a scarcity of
studies (Wheeler & Nelson, 2003; Mayfield, Martin, & Herbig,
1998; Stettner, 1997; Kharbanda & Stallworthy, 1991; Johnson,
McCarty, & Allen, 1976) that examine the role of nonverbal
communication in the negotiation process. Furthermore, no empirical
studies were found which examine specifically the role of nonverbal
communication in multi-national business negotiations by collecting data
from the real world professional business negotiators. It is worth
pointing out that the participants in this study are the real world
professional business negotiators who report more precisely about what
the negotiators' perception than the student sample and simulation
method used in the past.
THE CURRENT PROBLEM
As we enter the 21st century, cross-cultural concerns and business
will become more and more significant (Chu, Ma, & Green, 2004; Chu,
2003). "One reason is indicated by such terms as world economy,
global village, and spaceship earth which indicate the interdependence
facing all of us on this planet. The ozone layer and global warming are
a concern of all countries.... Today more than ever, no country can
isolate itself from the rest of the world" (Terpstra, 1993, p.3).
The nature of this article is necessary given that increasingly there is
a move toward a multi-national economy in this century. Therefore, since
nonverbal communication is a critical component of negotiations, it is
important to examine its role within the context of multi-national
negotiations. This article will specifically explore the perceived
importance of the following nonverbal factors in the negotiation
process: proxemics (location and negotiation site), physical arrangement
(seating and furniture arrangement), and kinesics (eye contact, facial
expressions and gestures).
The rest of this paper is organized as follows: First, the paper
provides the theoretical background and research questions. Second,
presents descriptions of data, instrument and methodology. Third,
discusses the empirical results and findings. Finally, points out the
managerial implications of this empirical study, its limitation and the
suggestions for future research.
HISTORICAL OVERVIEW OF THE NEGOTIATION PROCESS
Negotiation is "a broad conflict management process involving
discussions between and among individuals who are interdependent and
need to come together for a decision or course of action; frequently
associated with the need to compromise effectively"
(Shockley-Zalabak, 1988, p. 247).
The process of negotiation involves exchanging messages, both
verbal and nonverbal. It is argued that the ability to analyze these
nonverbal behaviors adds to a negotiator's overall negotiating
ability. Nonverbal signals are deemed as important tools, for they can
imply a meaning without verbally committing the negotiator to a
particular action, i.e., nonverbal cues cannot be interpreted as
promises in the same way that verbal messages can (Smith, 1998). In
addition, careful observation of these critical communication elements
may yield indications that the message senders are nervous, frustrated,
bored, angry, or unsure. Johnson (1993) mentions that negotiators who
are proficient at observing and using nonverbal information are more
likely to achieve their goals in negotiations than those who have
difficulty reading people. In addition, by understanding how nonverbal
messages function and knowing what they can expect to learn through the
reading and sending of these messages, negotiators are more likely to
attribute greater meaning to the subtle nuances of the negotiation
process.
CATEGORIES OF NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION
Nonverbal communication refers to communication effected by means
other than words (assuming words are the verbal element). It includes
three categories. First, there is the communication environment which
consists of the physical environment and spatial environment. Second,
there are the communicators' physical characteristics: physique or
body shape, general attractiveness, height, weight, hair, skin color,
tone or odors (body or breath), physical appearance (clothes, lipstick,
eyeglasses, wigs and other hairpieces, false eyelashes, jewelry), and
accessories such as attache cases. Third, there are the body movements
and positions. These can include gestures, posture, touching behavior,
facial expressions, eye behavior and vocal behavior (Knapp & Hall,
1997). In this research, each of these nonverbal communications is
referred to as silent messages.
There are primarily two areas of nonverbal communication with which
negotiators are concerned: proxemics (including physical arrangement)
and kinesics, which are defined as three categories of nonverbal
communication in this article. The conceptual definitions for these
follow. First of all, proxemics is defined as "the study of the
ways in which space is handled (related to Latin proximus,
nearest)" (Clark, Eschholz, & Rosa, 1972, p. 457) and "how
man perceives, structures, and uses space. ...how you arrange the
furniture" (Burgoon & Saine, 1978, p. 89). Finally, kinesics is
defined as "the study of movement (related to Greek kinesis,
movement)" (Clark et al., 1972, p. 457), and "refers to all
the forms of body movement, excluding touch." (Burgoon & Saine,
1978, p. 54).
Proxemics (Location and Site) in Negotiation. Lewicki &
Litterer (1985) state that the physical environment can contribute to
the tone and mood of negotiations, and the anticipated mood of a
negotiation can lead parties to prefer one site over another. The site
selection was considered a critical variable for the intercultural
negotiation process (Mayfield et al., 1998). Negotiators should be aware
of the impact that a particular site has on a negotiation, and
consciously choose sites that create the desired mood. Most of the site
characteristics have their strongest impact on a bargainer's
perceptions of the environment, rather than actual, tangible,
substantive effects on the negotiations themselves.
Johnson (1993) declares that the sense of personal space influences
people's behavior regardless of whether or not they are conscious
of it. By watching how people use space, a negotiator can find clues
about his or her opponent's intentions and strategies. If one side
suggests a change of the seating arrangement of the negotiations, it
might be a sign of attitude change for "our use of space (our own
and others') can affect dramatically our ability to achieve certain
desired communication goals" (Knapp & Hall, 1997, p. 154).
Johnson (1993) believes that negotiators structure their territory
to make others comfortable or uneasy. A negotiator may be able to learn
more about members of the other side by meeting in their territory or
may feel more in control by meeting in his or her own territory. Johnson
(1993) also states that thinking about territory and considering the
impact of each negotiation setting helps a negotiator overcome the
manipulation of territory by the other side. Those with control over the
arrangement of the room usually want to minimize the amount of direct
confrontation and to keep the other side talking as much as possible.
Those who make members of the other side feel comfortable, free, and
somewhat trusting have gained command over the environment, for
territory can be used to express power. Lewicki & Litterer (1985)
declare that sites are not inherently neutral, they are perceived as
neutral; a lounge is not inherently "warm" or
"cold", but rather perceived that way by virtue of the decor
that is used. In order to handle messages from "territory",
negotiators should not allow the setting to intimidate or seduce them.
If one must negotiate on the other side's turf, one should not be
awed by the environment; otherwise, one may lose his or her best deal in
the negotiation.
Lewicki & Litterer (1985) state that cold, sterile, and formal
surroundings are generally related to competitive interactions. A very
large and formal wooden table, formal chairs, white walls, muted colored
carpets and curtains, and a businesslike atmosphere tend to be the
location for formal talks and deliberations. These rooms tend to create
a "no nonsense" tone for people, and suggest that cold, hard,
businesslike transactions are to be carried out within them. In
contrast, cheerful, bright-colored rooms, overstuffed chairs,
"living room" arrangement of furniture, soft lighting, and
artwork create a significantly more comfortable environment in which
parties are more relaxed, and can make people feel comfortable. The
parties let down their guard and relax, creating an affable mood, which
may cause participants to act more cooperatively.
Griffin & Daggatt (1990) assert that the location of
negotiations also can favor one side or the other. In order to make sure
no side can take advantage of the location, negotiations are usually
carried out at a neutral site. Johnson (1993) also suggests that a
neutral site is ideal for negotiations because it can be agreeable and
comfortable for both sides and advantageous to neither. Griffin &
Daggatt (1990) argue that for a diplomatic activity, negotiators tend to
prefer a neutral setting, e.g., Malta for the 1989 meeting between
Presidents Bush and Gorbachev, Paris for the Vietnam peace talks,
Panmuinjom for the Korean War talks, and even a raft in the middle of
the Neman River when Napoleon Bonaparte and Czar Alexander I met in
1807. A successful negotiator, therefore, does not ignore the important
function of the negotiation location and site.
Proxemics (Physical Arrangement) in Negotiation. Burgoon, Buller,
& Woodall (1996) state that humans are affected by their physical
surroundings. Lewicki & Litterer (1985) find that furniture may be
used to communicate status and power. Chairs, tables, interior design,
or even the number and size of ash trays are specifically equalized
between both parties to assure that no side is seen as
"bigger", "better", more important, or how much
power each may have. Lewicki & Litterer (1985) state that in more
formal negotiations such as international deliberations, status may be
communicated by the size of flags and nameplates, the degree of
comfortableness of a chair, the height of the back of a chair, or the
number of parties at the table. On the other hand, in more informal
negotiations, status is most commonly observed through office decor. The
decoration of the office reflects the owner's personality and the
message he or she would like to communicate. It provides home-turf
advantage and an additional group of symbols that enhances the
occupant's perceived status and power as well. Lewicki &
Litterer (1985) mention that if the occupant chooses to seat his or her
visitor across the desk from him or her (a competitive location), and on
a chair lower than his or her own (so that there is no eye-to-eye
contact, but the visitor is "looking up at" the occupant), the
scene is well set for a competitive negotiation that places the visitor
at a significant disadvantage. In contrast, if the office occupant moves
out from behind his desk, seating himself in a "conversational
grouping" of chairs, maintaining level eye contact, and minimizing
the number of status symbols within the office, he will help to create
an environment that encourages more equal-status communication.
Knapp & Hall (1997) claim that leaders and dominant
personalities tend to choose specific seats, but seating position also
can determine one's role in a group. Johnson (1993) says that
choosing where to sit (even if it means moving a chair, or even deciding
whether to sit) may help make a negotiator feel more confident. Anderson
(1993) states that leaders and powerful people take up more space than
others do. By taking up more space, they appear to be taking charge.
Johnson (1993) maintains that the manipulation of the seating
arrangement is one way that a negotiator can give or receive this type
of clue. Negotiators often vie for a "power" position such as
head of the table, center of a large delegation, back against a wall,
and so on. Some negotiators gravitate toward the most prominent seat. On
the other hand, those who want to avoid a show of power will want to
have an alternative plan. A skilled negotiator will de-emphasize any
single act that could be interpreted as a bid for power and focus
greater attention on the other side's patterns of behavior.
According to Johnson (1993), those who are perceived to have strong
personal power may be met with resistance if they demonstrate additional
power with nonverbal clues. In contrast, they gain greater acceptance
when they attempt to diffuse resistance by sending signals showing that
they are "just part of the gang."
Sommer (1965) shows that parties who are cooperatively disposed
toward one another seem to prefer seating arrangements that are side by
side; while parties who are competitively oriented tend to prefer
seating directly across from one another. Directly opposing seating, on
the other hand, allows each party to "keep an eye on the
other" and "keep the opponent at arm's
length"--common colloquialisms that, in fact, express the
competitive sentiments of each party. Lewicki & Litterer (1985) show
that competitive parties seek greater physical distance from one
another, and/or are more likely to place "barricades" of
furniture between themselves and the other person. Thus, cooperative
parties may be very comfortable sitting next to one another (twelve to
eighteen inches apart); competitive parties may place tables of all
sizes and shapes or other barriers between them in order to prevent the
others from encroaching on their territory.
Kinesic Messages (eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures).
Even if one keeps silent, one is still saying something. Albert
Mehrabian (1981) finds that 55 percent of a message comes from facial
expressions and 38 percent comes from vocal tone, which only 7 percent
comes from verbal cues. Johnson (1993) recommends that if negotiators
want to cool down a particularly hostile exchange, they can reduce the
tension of their words, but they will also want to ease back with their
bodies, lower their volume, and soften their facial expressions.
Nierrenberg (1986) mentions that facial expressions are obvious means of
nonverbal communication. However, the "poker face" confronts
us with a total lack of expression, a blank look. This very lack of
expression tells us that people do not want us to know anything about
their feelings. In spite of the assumed mask, we can read their intent.
Knapp & Hall (1997, p. 332) state: "The face may be the basis
for judging another person's personality and that it can (and does)
provide information other than one's emotional state."
Former United Nations Secretary General Dag Hammarskjold says:
"The unspoken dialog between two people can never be put right by
anything they say" (Burgoon, 1996, p. 297). Hendon, Hendon &
Herbig (1996) find that in negotiation, what is not said is in many
cases more important than what is openly expressed by the parties
involved. This matches the perspective of Knapp & Hall (1997) that
how something is said is often more important than what is being said.
Hendon, Hendon & Herbig (1996) find that most important, emotional
messages at the negotiating table are expressed nonverbally by gestures,
tone of voice, or facial expressions. The other side's
interpretation of your statement depends on the nonverbal behaviors more
than what is actually said.
Hendon et al. (1996) find that effective negotiators are
particularly good at controlling (consciously or subconsciously) their
kinesic messages and at the same time adjusting to the many nonverbal
signals they receive from the opposing negotiator(s). Illich (1980)
states that personal power is conveyed primarily by nonverbal channels
and understanding by identifying nonverbal clues. However, Anderson
(1993) finds that to make one powerful, one should avoid unnecessary
gestures, make every movement count, and slow down one's movements.
When one's movements are deliberate and thoughtful, people will
perceive the speaker that way as well. Griffin & Daggatt (1990)
emphasize that it is a good idea to simplify your actions and gestures,
while still being yourself. This minimizes the risk that your gestures
are contradicting your words.
Anderson (1993) suggests that people perceived as powerful shift
their position occasionally, making themselves appear relaxed,
confident, and in charge.
When you know your naturally happy, confident, and powerful poses,
you can adopt them even when you are feeling unhappy or weak. This
keeps you from being at a disadvantage in a time of negotiation and
may actually make you feel better (p. 98).
Lewicki & Litterer (1985) state that the eye is universally
regarded as the lens permitting us to look into a person's soul,
and dishonest people and cowards are not supposed to be able to look us
in the eye. Knapp & Hall (1997) find that during fluent speech,
speakers tend to look at listeners much more than during hesitant
speech. We seem to gaze more at people we like (Knapp & Hall).
Burgoon, Buller & Woodall (1996) conclude that those who were more
persuasive used more eye contact, longer gazes and spent a greater
amount of time gazing, thus promoting more attitude change and improving
the overall effectiveness of a persuasive presentation. Moreover, gaze
has been shown to be a powerful influence on other people's
willingness to help someone or to comply with a request. Finally,
Burgoon et al. add that when the nature of the problem and solution were
clear, a stare increased the probability of a bystander's offering
help.
Hendon et al. deem that nonverbal communication can be quite
telling as it can help one determine the exact meaning of what the other
side is saying and also can help the negotiator get his own message
across. Liking and disliking, tensions, and appraisal of an argument are
shown by numerous signs such as blushing, contraction of facial muscles,
giggling, strained laughter, or just silence. Whenever a party
negotiates, the negotiator must see and observe the other party. While
seated, people may lean forward when they like what you are saying or
are interested in listening, or they may sit back in their seats with
crossed arms if they do not like the message. Nervousness can manifest
itself through nonverbal behavior, and blinking can be related to
feelings of guilt and fear. The more simple and direct the language, the
more precisely a position is defined, the stronger the commitment is
likely to be. However, Griffin & Daggatt (1990) deem that the more
subtle and less direct your explanation, the more likely one is to
succeed. People are more likely to be convinced by reasons they
discovered themselves than by reasons pointed out to them by others.
Sparks (1993) verifies that it is great advice for negotiators to be
economical with words, for that helps people understand what is said. It
saves time, too.
Nierenberg (1986) says that to the negotiator, as the old song has
it, "every little movement has a meaning all its own." Kinesic
messages have a tremendous impact on the negotiation process. Hendon et
al. argue that everything counts during the negotiation: the time of the
negotiation (morning, lunch time, late in the evening), the table
(round, square), the lights (white, in the middle of the room), the use
of microphones, the breaks, the phone calls, the space between the
chairs, the way the negotiators dress, and so on. Everything is
important. Effective negotiators are fully aware of the existence of all
these factors and of the fact that they are able to use them to their
advantage. Nierenberg (1986) emphasizes that the slight movement of an
eyebrow, the tilt of the head, the sudden movement of the hand--all are
messages that an individual who deals with people must understand and
continue to study.
Based on the above review of the literature this manuscript will
explore three research questions.
1. How do negotiators of different nationalities perceive the role
of proxemics in the negotiation process?
2. How do negotiators of different nationalities perceive the role
of physical arrangement in the negotiation process?
3. How do negotiators of different nationalities perceive the role
of kinesic messages in the negotiation process?
METHODOLOGY
Subjects. Three samples were collected for this study. The first
sample pool was obtained from a multi-national company in China.
Participants included 50 individuals from seven different nationalities:
Chinese, German, British, Italian, French, Canadian and Pakistani. The
second sample pool was obtained from a Chinese textile company and a
Mexican company. The 22 participants were Chinese nationals whose first
language is Chinese, and Mexican nationals whose first language is
Spanish. Some of the participants also speak English as their second
language. The last samples were randomly gathered from the business
people in the U.S. The nine participants were American nationals whose
first language is English. All the participants in the sample were
required to have had business negotiation experience. Both male and
female participants ranging in age from twenty-six (26) to seventy (70)
took part in the study. Survey Instrument. The questionnaire was
developed in consultation with the faculty of the
Communication Department at a southwestern university and from the
review of the literature. It was piloted on a group of students from
various departments across campus. Revisions were made based on the
feedback received. The questionnaire in the Chinese version was
crosschecked by two professional translators to see if the questions
make sense to the Chinese participants. A cover letter was attached to
each questionnaire upon distributing to the participants, which clearly
stated that their participations were completely voluntary and
confidence. A five-point Likert scale (strongly agree 1, agree 2,
uncertain 3, disagree 4, and strongly disagree 5) was used in the study.
The final questionnaire consists of 46 questions both in the English
version and the Chinese version, which addresses the two nonverbal
dimensions of negotiation under study: proxemics (location and site),
physical arrangement (seating and furniture arrangement) and kinesics
(eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures). See Table 1 below.
Data Collection Procedures. The first batch of questionnaires was
distributed by the researcher during a business negotiation meeting in
Culiacan, Sinaloa, Mexico. The chief negotiator for the Mexican company
and a representative of the Chinese delegation were each entrusted to
distribute the questionnaire to 22 employees from their respective
companies. They were instructed to mail the completed questionnaires
back to the researcher as expeditiously as possible. The participants
from the Chinese textile company received the questionnaires in Chinese.
The second batch of questionnaires was mailed via Express Mail to a
negotiator in a multi-national joint venture company located in China.
He was instructed to distribute them to 50 employees of various
nationalities. In all cases, employees completing the questionnaire were
advised that their participation was voluntary and that they were free
to withdraw at any time.
The third batch of questionnaires was sent to a business people in
the U.S. He randomly distributed nine questionnaires to the business
people from various businesses. The participants were asked to finish
the questionnaires within two weeks and mail them back to the
researcher.
There were totally eighty-one questionnaires passed out, three
questionnaires from Hong Kong and one from the multi-national company
were not returned, and two of the nine questionnaires from the U.S. were
considered as unusable. There was some missing data from demographic
part that did not cause problem. Altogether seventy-five questionnaires
were analyzed for this study.
Analysis. Two analyses were conducted in this study. First, in
order to test the reliability of the instrument and scale
(questionnaire), the correlation coefficient test and reliability
coefficient test were employed accompanying with the test of Homogeneity of Variances and multiple comparison. Second, to find out how the
professional business negotiators perceived the role of the three
nonverbal categories in the negotiation and averages across these
negotiators, the means from three groups of subjects were computed. Each
of the two results are reported and discussed below. Results. The
correlation coefficient test was employed to test the degree of
relationship between two sets of scores. The overall strength of
correlations in the three categories is above .433 (r > .433) and
significant at the 5% (.05) level. The items with low correlation
coefficients (r < .433) were eliminated. The correlation coefficient
of Location and Site is from .527 to .702; Physical Arrangement is from
.434 to .693; and Kinesic Message is from .433 to .505. Overall strength
of the correlations in the three categories falls in the moderate range
(Bartz 1999, p. 184), as shown in Table 2.
The reliability coefficient test was employed to assess the
consistency of the entire scale. The reliability coefficient for
Location & Site is .7983 with 9 items. The alpha for Physical
arrangement is .8227 with 12 items. The reliability coefficient of the
Kinesic Message is .4293 with 6 items. The generally agreed upon lower
limit for Cronbach's alpha is .70, although it may decrease to .60
in exploratory research, therefore among the three categories, Kinesic
Message had a low reliability coefficient and the reliability
coefficient of the other two categories fell in desirable range.
Altogether twenty-seven out of forty-six items were left for further
analysis.
Test of Homogeneity of Variances showed that the three groups
(Chinese, British, and the Other) had the same variances. The test of
homogeneity of variances in all three categories showed that the same
variances existed among the three categories (p>.05). One-Way ANOVA showed that there was a significant difference among the three groups in
Physical Arrangement (p < .05), not in the other two categories. The
Post Hoc Tests (Scheffe test) showed that there were significant
differences among Chinese versus British (p < .0.020), Chinese versus
Other (p < .079); British versus Chinese (p < .020) in Physical
Arrangement. There were no significant differences among the three
groups in Location & Site and Kinesic Message, as shown in Table 3.
The results of mean average showed that the three nonverbal
categories were believed important and the Agree mean percentages of all
the three categories exceeded Disagree. The findings are reported in the
following.
FINDINGS
When the focus of the research is on averages across people or
group behavior (as in this study), a 3-point scale is sufficient, which
reduces the complexity thereby increasing the accuracy (Dant et al.,
1990; Lehmann & Hulbert, 1972). For the analysis and in order to
find out how the negotiators perceive the three nonverbal categories,
the two categories of "strongly agree" and "agree"
were combined, to represent an "agree" response rate. Also,
the categories of "strongly disagree" and "disagree"
were merged into a "disagree" response rate. The findings of
Scheffe test and mean comparison are discussed in the following.
Proxemics (location and site). The Scheffe test shows that the
Chinese negotiators think the Location and Site are more important for a
sound negotiation than the British negotiators, and the British
negotiators think it is important, whereas the negotiators from the
group of the Other nationalities think it is less important than both
the Chinese and British negotiators. Overall, the differences among the
three groups are not statistically significant. It is not consistent
with the literature or theory, which indicates that it is important for
a successful negotiation. The total percentage of the responses in the
"location and site" category shows that 50% of the
participants believe the location and site are important for a sound
negotiation. 19% of them do not think the location and site are
important; and 31% are not certain about it.
Physical arrangement (seating and furniture arrangement). Both the
Chinese negotiators and the British negotiators deem the seating and
furniture arrangement important for a satisfactory negotiation. The
group of the Other shows no statistically significant and they may not
think the seating and furniture arrangement play an important role in
negotiation. Overall this category is statistically significant and is
consistent with the literature and theory.
The total percentage of the responses in the "physical
arrangement (seating and furniture arrangement)" category is less
significant than "location and site" category. This category
shows that 46% of the participants deem the seating and furniture
arrangement important for a satisfactory negotiation. 24% of them do not
think the seating and furniture arrangement play an important role in
negotiation, and 30% are not certain about it.
Kinesic Messages (eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures).
Overall the Kinesic Message category is not statistically significant.
However, both the Chinese negotiators and the British negotiators think
that the "eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures" are
less important for a successful negotiation than the group of the Other
nationalities for they believe that the "eye contact, facial
expressions, and gestures" are important for a successful
negotiation. The finding of this category shows an inconsistent with the
literature and theory.
The total percentage of the responses in the "Kinesic Messages
(eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures)" category is less
than the other two categories. The Kinesic Messages category shows that
45% of the participants believe that the "eye contact, facial
expressions, and gestures" are important for a successful
negotiation. 21% of disagree with the major role the "eye contact,
facial expressions, and gestures" played in negotiation, and 34%
are not certain about it.
DISCUSSION
The findings of two out of three groups are inconsistent with the
literature and theory from Scheffe test. I would assume that the
negotiators from these two groups might think the Location & Site
and Kinesic Message are both important in their daily life or social
life, however, in their professional life, they respond differently. It
might be true that "Business is business", and they might
ignore the roles of Location & Seating and Kinesic Message in their
professional interaction with the other people.
All participants in three groups--Chinese, British and Other
believe that Physical Arrangement is important for a satisfactory
negotiation. The finding of Physical Arrangement is the only category
showing consistent with the literature and theory. Burgoon, Buller &
Woodall (1996) have argued that humans are affected by their physical
surroundings. Lewicki & Litterer (1985) state that furniture may be
used to communicate status and power; furthermore they find that face to
face seating arrangement creates competition and places the visitors at
a significant disadvantage. In this case, the parties seek greater
physical distance from one another. Sommer (1965) shows that parties who
are cooperatively disposed toward one another seem to prefer seating
arrangements that are side by side. Knapp & Hall (1997) claim that
leaders and dominant personalities tend to choose specific seats.
Johnson (1993) says that choosing where to sit may help make a
negotiator feel more confident. In addition, negotiators often vie for a
"power" position and gravitate toward the most prominent seat.
However, the results of mean average show us a different picture
from Scheffe test. After data analysis, the results of mean average show
that the findings are consistent with the theories covered in the
literature review. The three categories discussed here are deemed to
have the strongest impacts on a successful negotiation.
Proxemics (location and site). Johnson (1993) declares that a
negotiator may feel more in control by meeting in his or her own
territory. Furthermore, Lewicki & Litterer (1985) believe that if
one negotiates on the other side's turf, one should not be awed by
the environment; otherwise, one may lose his or her best deal in the
negotiation. Johnson (1993) also finds that a neutral site is ideal for
negotiations because it can be agreeable and comfortable for both sides
and advantageous to neither. "Our use of space (our own and
others') can affect dramatically our ability to achieve certain
desired communication goals" (Knapp & Hall, 1997, p. 154). The
above argument is strongly supported by the participants in this
research.
In this study, 71% of participants who feel they have a greater
advantage in the negotiation when that negotiation occurs on their own
turf. 71% of the participants also feel more "comfortable"
when they are negotiating on their own turf; 66% feel more
"confident" when negotiating on their own turf; 59% of them
feel that they "perform more effectively" when negotiating on
their own turf. Moreover, 56% of participants feel a "greater
challenge" when negotiating on the other's turf. 69% of
participants believe that the person who designates the negotiation site
has an advantage in negotiations, and 50% of participants regard the
home site in negotiation as equivalent to a home team advantage in
sports.
Physical Arrangement (seating and furniture arrangement). Burgoon,
Buller & Woodall (1996) have argued that humans are affected by
their physical surroundings. Lewicki & Litterer (1985) state that
furniture may be used to communicate status and power; furthermore they
find that face to face seating arrangement creates competition and
places the visitors at a significant disadvantage. In this case, the
parties seek greater physical distance from one another. Sommer (1965)
shows that parties who are cooperatively disposed toward one another
seem to prefer seating arrangements that are side by side. Knapp &
Hall (1997) claim that leaders and dominant personalities tend to choose
specific seats. Johnson (1993) says that choosing where to sit may help
make a negotiator feel more confident. In addition, negotiators often
vie for a "power" position and gravitate toward the most
prominent seat.
In this category, the data are consistent with the literature about
furniture arrangement as mentioned above. 62% of participants report
feeling calm when they negotiate in a room with paintings of countryside
scenes on the walls. 74% of participants feel "more pleasant"
when they sit at a table decorated with flowers, 60% of them feel more
at ease when there are soft drinks on the table when they negotiate, and
56% think the arrangement of furniture should receive more attention
from negotiators. 75% of the participants believe that seating
arrangement should receive more attention from negotiators. In regards
to a round table, 57% of participants believe a round table eases
tension, 54% generally prefer round tables to square tables in
negotiations, and 53% of them deem that a round-table seating
arrangement makes an atmosphere more conducive for discussion. Also, 50%
of the participants say that notebooks and pens neatly arranged on a
large table encourage them to reach an agreement in a negotiation. A
room equipped with a large screen TV, VCR, and computer is deemed more
professional when negotiating by 51% of participants.
Kinesic Messages. Even if one keeps silent, one is still conveying
messages. Knapp & Hall (1997, p. 332) state: "The face may be
the basis for judging another person's personality and that it can
(and does) provide information other than one's emotional
state." Knapp & Hall (1997) find that how something is said is
often more important than what is being said. In addition, Anderson
(1993) suggests that people perceived as powerful shift their position
occasionally, making themselves appear in charge. Burgoon et al.
conclude that those who were more persuasive used more eye contact,
longer gazes. Gaze has been shown to be a powerful influence on other
people's willingness to help someone or to comply with a request.
The responses in the kinesic messages category show less
consistency with the theory than the last two categories. However, 51%
of the participants say that they move physically closer to emphasize
the importance of their point in negotiations. In addition 66% believe
that people who smile more get greater cooperation in negotiations. 49%
of participants think that people who look straight into the
other's eyes gain power in negotiations; and 44% of them believe
that people who can tolerate silence longer have more power in
negotiations. Finally, 44% of the participants lean back in their chairs
as a gesture of encouragement to respond to their proposals.
LIMITATIONS AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
Overall the validity of nonverbal theory is strengthened by the
findings because all the agreed responses exceeded the "not
important" level. However, the percentage is a little less than
what researcher anticipated. The mean showed that the tendency is toward
not certain (2.66, 2.77, and 2.74). It is not clear which genders and
age groups favored "important", "uncertain" and
"not important". Future research will be needed to distinguish
the differences among each demographic group. Since all the participants
are experienced negotiators instead of a college population, the
generalizability of the research findings may be increased and broadly
applied to business negotiations. However, another limitation of the
study is that the sample (i.e., the subject pool) was too small. This
made it difficult to distinguish the differences among each gender and
age group. The number of female participants was also too small. The
questionnaires will need to be examined according to the different
nationalities. The researcher might have obtained different results if
she would have interviewed or observed the negotiators. Future research
will need to focus on the responses from these dimensions. High context
cultures and low context culture may respond differently to the
nonverbal messages in negotiation. Different genders may respond
differently, and negotiators may respond to nonverbal messages
differently in social life and professional life.
Since this study is the first empirical research on nonverbal
communication in cross-cultural business negotiation by collecting data
from the real world professional business negotiators, its implication
to practitioners and contribution to nonverbal theory is evidently.
Besides culture factors and negotiation styles, silent messages play the
role in successful business negotiations too.
CONCLUSION AND MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS
With the increase of international business and globalization cross-cultural business negotiation becomes more important than ever. It
is estimated that global managers spend more than half of their time
negotiating, and negotiation is often ranked as one of the most
important skills for global managers to possess (George, Jones, &
Gonzalez, 1998, p. 750). Moreover, China's economy has been growing
at an annual rate of 7.7 percent, more than double the world average of
3.3 percent (china-embassy.org, 8/18/00). "Western observers thus
confront the paradoxical possibility that by the year 2010, if it
maintains just two-thirds of its current rate of growth, China could
have become at one and the same time, the world's largest
capitalist and Marxist-Leninist state (Boisot, 1996)." The
implications of this study provide guidance to cross-cultural
negotiators, and offer them advice as well.
Overall it was found that the averages of the means of the three
nonverbal communication categories are consistent with the literature,
demonstrating that nonverbal communication is important for business
negotiation. On the other hand, the findings show that not all three
groups of negotiators view nonverbal communication as important in their
professional area. The implications for managers from the three
categories are addressed as follows.
Location and site. The findings of this study indicate that most
negotiators feel that they have a greater advantage in negotiation when
they do so on their own turf. Negotiating on their own turf can make
them feel more comfortable, confident, and perform more effectively
because they believe that the home site in negotiation is equivalent to
a home team advantage in sports. They also feel a greater challenge when
negotiating on the other party's turf, for they believe that the
person who designates the negotiation site has an advantage in
negotiations. Therefore, negotiators might try to select their own
location or a neutral site. If the meeting does not occur on your own
turf you should not be awed or influenced by the location and site. On
the other hand, when you are confident about negotiations and would like
to cooperate with your counterparts, you will make them feel comfortable
when negotiating at the site of their choosing. And you might get more
cooperation than when negotiating on your own turf.
Physical arrangements. The findings of this study indicate that
paintings of countryside scenes on walls and a table decorated with
flowers and soft drinks will make negotiators feel calm, more pleasant,
and at ease. They prefer round tables to square tables in negotiations
because they believe that a round table eases tension, and a round-table
seating arrangement makes for an atmosphere more conducive for
discussion. Thus, when negotiating on your own turf, decorate the room
with paintings of countryside scenes on the walls, use a round table
decorated with flowers and put soft drinks on the table. This physical
arrangement will ease tension and make your counterparts feel calm, more
pleasant, and at ease. The setting will be more conducive for
discussion, and you will get more cooperation from them too.
Kinesic message. The findings indicate that people move physically
closer to emphasize the importance of their point in negotiations, and
that people who smile more get greater cooperation. To bargain
effectively it is important to determine the counterpart's
priorities and goals. Therefore, you might observe when they move
physically closer that they are emphasizing the importance of their
point. Moreover, you may obtain greater cooperation in negotiations when
you smile more; a straight face might not be a good cue in
cross-cultural negotiations.
If negotiators work out a satisfactory agreement or want to
terminate negotiations, employing silent messages might be more
effective than direct confrontation. If negotiators pay attention to the
three nonverbal categories the results will be cooperative,
constructive, effective, and reach a desirable end.
REFERENCES
Anderson, K. (1993). Body language on the job. Cosmopolitan,
214(5), 96-99.
Bartz, A.E. (1999). Basic statistical concepts (4th Edition). New
Jersey, Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Boisot, M.H. (1996). Institutionalizing the labor theory of value:
Some obstacles to the reform of state-owned enterprises in China and
Vietnam. Organization Studies, 17 (6), 909-928.
Burgoon, J.K., & Saine, T. (1978). The unspoken dialogue: An
introduction to nonverbal communication. Dallas: Houghton Mifflin Company.
Burgoon, J.K., Buller, D. B. & Woodall, W. G. (1996). Nonverbal
Communication: The Unspoken Dialogue. New York: The McGraw-Hill
Companies, Inc.
Casse, P., & Deol, S. (1985). Managing intercultural
negotiations: guidelines for trainers and negotiators. Washington, DC:
Sietar International.
Chang, L.C. (2003). An examination of cross-cultural negotiation:
Using Hofstede framework. Journal of American Academy of Business,
Cambridge, Hollywood: 2(2), 567-570.
Chen, D., & Faure, G.O. (1995). When Chinese companies negotiate with their government. Organization Studies, 16(1), 27-54.
Chu, Y. (2003). The Drivers and Antecedents of Satisfaction, Trust,
Commitment, and Loyalty among Chinese Customers. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, The University of Texas-Pan American, ProQuest, AAT
3107649.
Chu, Y., Ma, J., & Greene, W.E. (2004). Sino-American labor law comparisons: Where do East and West meet? International Journal of
Commerce and Management, 14 (2), 32-47.
Clark, V.P., Eschholz, P.A., & Rosa, A.F. (1972). Language:
Introductory readings. (editors) New York: St. Martin's Press.
Dant, R.P., Lumpki, J.R., & Bush, R. (1990). Private physicians
or walk-in clinics: Do the patients differ? Journal of Healthcare
Marketing, 10(2), 25-35.
George, J.M., Jones, G.R., & Gonzalez, J.A. (1998). The role of
affect in cross-cultural negotiations. Journal of International Business
Studies, 29(4), 749-772.
Gordon, T.D. (1986). China. Hong Kong: Euromoney Publications.
Griffin, T.J., & Daggatt, W.R. (1990). The global negotiator:
Building strong business relationships anywhere in the world. New York:
Harper Business.
Hendon, D.W., Hendon, R.A., & Herbig, P. (1996). Cross-cultural
business negotiations. Westport, CT: Quorum Books.
Illich, J. (1980). The art and skill of successful negotiation.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Ivey, A.E., & Simek-Downing, L. (1980). Counseling and
psychotherapy, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Johnson, D.W., McCarty, K., & Allen, T. (1976). Congruent and
contradictory verbal and nonverbal communications of cooperativeness and
competitiveness in negotiations. Communication Research, 3(3), 275-291.
Johnson, R.A. (1993). Negotiation basics: concepts, skills, and
exercises. Newbury Park: Sage Publications, Inc.
Kharbanda, O.P., & Stallworthy, E.A. (1991). Verbal and
Non-Verbal Communication. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 6(4): 10-13,
49-52.
Knapp, M.L., & Hall, J.A. (1997). Nonverbal Communication in
Human Interaction (Fourth Edition). Austin: Harcourt Brace College
Publishers.
Lehmann, D.R., & Hulbert, J. (1972). Are three-point scales
always good enough? Journal of Marketing Research, 9 (11), 444-6.
Leung, T. & Yeung, L.L. (1995). Negotiation in the
People's Republic of China: Results of a survey of small businesses
in Hong Kong. Journal of Small Business Management, 33(1): 70-77.
Lewicki, R.J., & Litterer, J.A. (1985). Negotiation. Irwin
Homewood, IL: 60430.
Mayfield, J., Mayfield, M., Martin, D., & Herbig, P. (1998).
How location impacts international business negotiations. Review of
Business, Jamaica: 19(2), 21-24.
Mehrabian, A. (1981). Silent messages: Implicit communication of
emotions and attitudes (Second Edition). Belmont, California: Wadsworth
Publishing Company.
Mente, B. (1992). Chinese etiquette and ethnics in business.
Lincolnwood, Ill: NTC Business Books, 116, 1995 printing.
Nierenberg, G.I. (1986). The Complete Negotiator. New York:
Nierenberg & Zeif Publishers.
Palich, L.E., Carini, G.R., & Livingstone, L.P. (2002).
Comparing American and Chinese negotiating styles: The influence of
logic paradigms. Thunderbird International Business Review, Hoboken:
44(6), 777-798.
Pye, L. (1982). Chinese commercial negotiation styles. Cambridge:
Oelgeschlager, Gunn, and Hain, Inc.
Shockley-Zalabak, P. (1988). Fundamentals of organizational
communication: Knowledge, sensitivity, skills, values (Third Edition).
New York: Longman Publishers, USA.
Smith, H.B. (1998). Selling through negotiation: The handbook of
sales negotiation. New York: American Management Association.
Sommer, R. (1965). Further studies of small group ecology.
Sociometry, 28, 337-348.
Sparks, D.B. (1993). The dynamics of effective negotiation (Second
Edition). Houston: Gulf Publishing Company.
Stephenson, G.M., & Kniveton, B.K. (1978). Interpersonal and
interparty exchange: An experimental study of the effect of seating
position on the outcome of negotiations between teams representing
parties in dispute. Human Relations, 31, 555-566.
Stettner, M. (1997). Body of evidence. Successful Meetings, 46(11),
128.
Terpstra, V. (1993). International dimensions of marketing. (Third
Edition). California: Wadsworth Publishing Company.
Tinsley, C.H., & Pillutla, M.M. (1998). Negotiating in the
United States and Hong Kong. Journal of International Business Studies,
29(4), 711-728.
Wheeler, M.A., Nelson, D. (2003). Nonverbal communication in
negotiation, Harvard Business School Cases, p1, 20p; (AN 11213035)
Yun Chu, Frostburg State University William F. Strong, The
University of Texas at Brownsville Jianyu Ma, The University of Texas
Pan American Walter E. Greene, The University of Texas Pan American
Table 1. Questions Categorized by Type of Nonverbal Communication
Proxemics Kinesic Message
Location and Site Physical Arrangement
Q 22 - Q 31 Q1-Q 21 Q 37 - Q 44
Q 45- Q 46 Q 32 - Q 36
Table 2. Correlation, Reliability Coefficients, Homogeneity of
Variances, and ANOVA
Test (p<0.05) Location & Site
Correlation Coefficients 0.521~0.702
Reliability Coefficient 0.7983 (9 items)
Homogeneity of Variances 0.083
ANOVA F=1.629, sig.=0.203
Test (p<0.05) Physical Arrangement Kinesic Message
Correlation Coefficients 0.434~0.693 0.433~0.505
Reliability Coefficient 0.8227 (12 items) 0.4293 (6 items)
Homogeneity of Variances 0.87 0.418
ANOVA F=4.390, sig.=0.016 F=.055, sig.=0.947
Table 3. Multiple Comparisons of Three Categories--Scheffe
Test (p<0.05)
Category (I) Nationality (J) Nationality
Composite Chinese 2
Location 3
British 1
3
Others 1
2
Composite Chinese 2
Physical 3
British 1
3
Others 1
2
Composite Chinese 2
Kinesic 3
British 1
3
Others 1
2
(J) Nationality Mean Difference Std. Error Sig.
(I-J)
2 -6.419 3.56 0.204
3 -4.277 3.774 0.529
1 6.419 3.56 0.204
3 2.412 3.319 0.812
1 4.277 3.774 0.529
2 -2.142 3.319 0.812
2 -18.562 6.469 0.02
3 -15.73 6.859 0.079
1 18.562 6.469 0.02
3 2.832 6.031 0.896
1 15.73 6.859 0.079
2 -2.832 6.031 0.896
2 -0.134 4.491 1
3 -1.35 4.762 0.961
1 0.134 4.491 1
3 -1.26 4.187 0.961
1 1.35 4.762 0.961
2 1.216 4.187 0.959