Framing decisions to communicate change.
Spears, Martha C. ; Parker, Darrell
INTRODUCTION
When an organization is undergoing change, the manner in which
change decisions are communicated determines the nature of the perceived
risks involved in the change process and the extent of resistance to
change. To illustrate the importance of communication in managing
change, this paper draws upon the literature on risk preference and
preference reversal for individuals facing uncertain decisions. Based
upon the manner in which a change decision is communicated or framed
individuals will alter their resistance to change alternatives. Framing
occurs when the context or presentation of a decision creates a climate
that influences the decision. In general, the issue of framing occurs
when decision-makers evaluate a proposition on the context in which the
outcome is presented, not just on the underlying outcomes (Parker and
Spears, 1999).
Framing in the communication of human relation decisions is
important for an organization facing restructuring, job loss, or change.
Framing has been used to explore questions involving the importance of
perceptions on insurance, gambling, and medical decisions. Experimental
studies have documented that decision-makers react differently to the
same proposition depending upon the manner in which it is presented.
This phenomenon is known as preference reversal and violates a strict
expected utility analysis of decision making (Machina, 1987). Framing is
one aspect of the growing literature in behavioral finance. This
research specifically recognizes the influence of psychology on the
behavior of financial practitioners (Shefrin, 1999). Furthermore,
framing is influenced by individual factors such as personality types
and gender (Parker and Spears, 2002).
A survey instrument is used that parallels the classic framing of
disease and death to analyze the framing of restructuring and job loss.
This approach permits us to determine that framing within the
communication of human relation decisions exhibits the preference
reversal phenomenon for corporate restructuring issues. Since framing is
a significant factor for decisions involving restructuring and change,
the communication of change decisions will influence an
organization's attitude toward and resistance to change. It is the
purpose of this paper to explore the link between framing and managing
the organizational change process.
THE COMMUNICATION OF CHANGE
Communication is an important aspect of organization design,
change, and strategic management. Managing the communication of change
is of vital importance to the organization and determines how
organizations adapt to the environment (Daft, Bettenhausen, and Tyler,
1995). Communication by managers after both internal and external
decisions are made is as big a part of a strategic initiative as
gathering information is prior to decision-making. For example, the news
of a Fortune 500 company making a large number of employee layoffs can
be framed quite differently by internal and external communication
sources. Is the company in trouble and destined for failure, or is the
company making strategic changes that will strengthen the organization?
Consequently, an announcement of layoffs may result in either increases
or decreases in the stock price of the company depending upon the frame
accepted by investors.
The framed structure can be defined as the configuration of
information and control systems within an organization as well as the
formalized configuration of roles and procedures for managing the
communication process (Ranson, Hinings, and Greenwood, 1980). To be
successfully objective about what change is required, a new frame has to
be viewed. Since it is difficult to correctly identify cause-and-effect
relationships, there is a blurring between "success" and
"failure" as results become interpreted using personal
perspectives and frames of reference (Van de Ven and Poole, 1995, Van de
Ven, Angle, and Poole, Van de Ven, Dooley, and Holmes, 2000). Management
controls the implementation process by influencing the personal
networks, personal relationships, and organizational frame created among
employees.
Although change is a natural process and instability is a normal
state, employee resistance is the cause for most failures of
organizational change initiatives (Critchley, 1996). "For change to
succeed, it has to be accepted by the workforce physically,
intellectually and emotionally" (Spiker, Lesser 1995, p. 18).
Poole, Van de Ven, Dooley and Holmes (2000) and Johnson and
Blanchard (1998) conclude that resistance to new ideas is expected and
that those who champion the change must embrace this philosophy as
evidence of innovation and progress. Mintzberg (1994) indicates that
planning and change represent a calculating style of management, but
should represent a communication style that engages people in an
undertaking that everyone helps to shape.
FRAMING AND PREFERENCE REVERSAL
When the emotional context that is communicated rather than the
outcome drives managerial decisions, the issue of framing arises. A
reference point may influence the manager. For example, it is the issue
of whether change is communicated as a source of loss or gain that
provides a reference point. The frame of reference influences whether an
uncertain choice is perceived as a gamble, (with a chance to win) or as
insurance (where the certain choice limits loss) and influences the
subject's decisions (Schoemaker and Kunreuther 1979; Hershey and
Schoemaker 1980; McNeil, Pauker, Sox, and Tversky 1982; and Slovic,
Fischoff and Lichtenstein 1983). To demonstrate this concept, alternate
scenarios are presented with the same expected value outcomes. Tversky
and Kahneman (1981, 1986) present the following classic decision for a
life or death scenario:
"Imagine that the U.S. is preparing for the outbreak of an
unusual Asian disease, which is expected to kill 600 people. Two
alternative programs to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume
that the exact scientific estimates of the consequences of the programs
are as follows:
If Program A is adopted, 200 people will be saved.
If Program B is adopted, there is 1/3 probability that 600 people
will be saved and 2/3 probability that no people will be saved.
Versus
If Program C is adopted, 400 people will die.
If Program D is adopted, there is 1/3 probability that nobody will
die and 2/3 probability that 600 people will die."
If presented with saving lives through choices A and B, 72% choose
the certain outcome A, however when phrased in terms of deaths 84% were
willing to gamble on Program D. The resistance to the treatment
described by Program B and D varies greatly depending upon whether the
issue is communicated in terms of certain death for the alternative.
FRAMING TO COMMUNICATE ORGANIZATIONAL CHANGE
These same types of influences play an important role in decisions
involving human relations. A survey instrument was developed and
included multiple decisions in the face of radical change and potential
loss including the classic framing question listed above. In order to
investigate the sensitivity of organizational decisions to framing a
question on job loss was included. The following question illustrates
the framing of organizational loss:
The manufacturing division of a U.S. company is having problems
competing in the global marketplace. The company must decide how to
reorganize this division of 12,000 U.S. workers. Two strategies have
been proposed.
A By contracting operations overseas 4,000 jobs will be saved.
B With an internal reorganization of U.S. operations there is 1/3
probability that all 12,000 jobs will be saved and a 2/3 probability
that no jobs will be saved.
Versus
C Operations can be moved out of the U.S. eliminating 8,000 jobs.
D With an internal reorganization of U.S. operations there is 1/3
probability that nobody will lose their jobs and a 2/3 probability that
all 12,000 will be unemployed.
METHODS
This research extends the analysis of framing and organizational
loss by directly analyzing the extent of framing among managers and
human resource professionals. The question is to what extent the framing
results are found in an analysis of employed professionals.
An institutional survey was mailed to a sample of 635 full time
employed business professionals with either a MBA or BS degree in
business administration. The survey topic involved performance
appraisal, training, and organizational change. In addition, the two
framing research questions presented above were included in the survey
instrument. The respondents were randomly assigned one of the two sets
of survey scenarios. The survey instrument was formatted and designed to
yield a high response rate. A total of 283 surveys were returned for a
response rate of 44.6%. A total of 260 respondents answered each framing
question by selecting one of the two strategies.
FRAMING LIFE AND DEATH DECISIONS
The results for the sample of working professionals are similar to
the results for framing found elsewhere. Overall, when faced with a life
or death choice, 68% of respondents preferred the gamble to the certain
outcome. As shown in Table 1, there is apparent framing based upon the
presentation of the question.
When the certain solution was expressed in terms of lives saved,
46% were willing to select the certain decision and 54% were willing to
gamble on the uncertain strategy. However among those respondents who
were faced with the certain solution stated in terms of deaths, only 16%
selected the certainty answer while 84% were then willing to gamble on a
better outcome.
An F test was performed to determine whether the two groups of
respondents for this question exhibited the same variance. The table
further reports the results from that test. The sample variance was
statistically different between the two groups of respondents. Hence, a
t test for samples with unequal variances was performed to determine the
statistical significance of the framing behavior exhibited. The results
from this study indicate that decisions involving the loss of life are
framed. The framing is statistically significant at the 99% level.
FRAMING ORGANIZATIONAL CHANGE
An analysis of the questions regarding the loss of jobs shows that
framing is evident although not as dramatic for issues involving the
loss of jobs. Overall when faced with decisions regarding job loss, 43%
selected the certain outcome while 57% were willing to gamble. As Table
2 illustrates there is framing apparent, based upon the manner in which
the decision is presented.
When faced with an organization decision communicated in terms of
jobs saved, 47% chose the certain outcome and 53% selected the gamble.
However, when phrased in terms of job losses, the respondents displayed
a stronger appetite for risk. When communicated as job losses the number
of respondents selecting the uncertain strategy increase to 61%.
Correspondingly, the number selecting the certain choice declined to
38%.
An F test was performed to determine whether the two groups of
respondents for this question exhibited the same variance. The sample
variance was not statistically different between the two groups of
respondents. Hence, the hypothesis that these respondents were pulled
from the same sample cannot be rejected. Consequently, a t test for
samples with equal variances was performed to determine the statistical
significance of the framing behavior exhibited.
The results from this study indicate that decisions involving the
loss of job are framed. The framing is statistically significant at the
90% level. Attitudes depend on the manner in which decisions are
presented or framed.
FRAMING IMPLICATIONS FOR MANAGERS
The understanding of framing offers insight into response to
proposals involving change or loss. Framing is a part of managerial
decision-making and influences decision-makers' attitudes toward
acceptance or resistance to change. The reference point communicated is
shown to be a key determinant of whether individuals are resistant to a
strategy with uncertain outcomes. Individuals are more willing to gamble
to prevent loss. If the certain strategy is seen as saving something,
then the uncertain strategy will be resisted. If the certain strategy is
shown to be associated with losses, then individuals are willing to
gamble on an uncertain strategy to prevent loss.
Since framing is a significant factor, the communication of change
decisions influences an organization's attitude toward and
resistance to change. Specifically, when communicating change, it is
important to articulate the cost of maintaining the status quo. When the
reference point is the loss associated with not changing, then
resistance to change and uncertainty decreases. If the reference point
focuses on the benefits and gains of the status quo, then resistance to
change is increased. Therefore, within the organizational change process
managers determine the response to change through the frame selected.
FUTURE RESEARCH
This paper illustrates the importance of communication in managing
change. Business professionals do exhibit framing when presented with
hypothetical scenarios involving organizational change. The research
method employed was based upon hypothetical scenarios and does not
necessarily reflect the extent of framing behavior when the
organizational change is the individual's own company. The
proximity of the change to the individual's well being may further
enhance the framing effect. A natural extension of this research would
be to develop a case analysis of specific organizational change
decisions and the extent to which framing apparently occurred or was
influenced by the managerial change process.
REFERENCES
Critchley, B. (1996, March). The myth of management change.
Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 17(2), 48-53.
Daft, R. L., K. R. Bettenhausen, & B. B. Tyler (1995).
Implication of top managers' communication choices for strategic
decisions. In George P. Huber And William H. Glick (eds.) Organizational
Change and Redesign (112-146). New York, Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Hershey, J., & P. Schoemaker (1980, March). Risk-taking and
problem context in the domain of losses--An Expected Utility Analysis.
Journal of Risk and Insurance, 47, 111-32.
Johnson, S. & K. Blanchard (1998). Who moved my cheese? Penguin Putnam, Incorporated.
Machina, M. (1987, Summer). Choice under uncertainty: Problems
solved and unsolved. Economic Perspectives, 1(1), 121-154.
McNeil, B., S. Pauker, H. Sox, & A. Tversky (1982, May). On the
elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. New England
Journal of Medicine, 306, 1259-62.
Mintzberg, H. (1994, January). The fall and rise of strategic
planning. Harvard Business Review, 66-75. Parker, D. & M. Spears
(1999). Framing as a marketing tactic for equipment leasing. Journal of
Equipment Lease Financing, 17(2) 20-25.
Parker, D. & M. Spears (2002). Personality, Gender, and Risk: A
Framing Analysis. Economics and Economic Education Research Journal,
forthcoming.
Poole, S. M., A. D. Van de Ven, K. Dooley, & M. E. Holmes
(2000). Organizational change and innovation processes. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Ranson, S., B. Hinings, & R. Greenwood (1980). The structuring
of organizational structures. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25,
1-17.
Schoemaker, P. & H. Kunreuther (1979, December). An
experimental study of insurance decisions. Journal of Risk and
Insurance, 46, 603-18.
Shefrin, H., (1999). Beyond Greed and Fear: Understanding
Behavioral Finance and the Psychology of Investing. Harvard Business
School Press.
Slovic, P., B. Fischoff, & S. Lichtenstein (1983, September).
Preference reversals: A broader perspective. American Economic Review,
73, 595-605.
Spiker, B. & E. Lesser (1995, March-April). We have met the
enemy.... Journal of Business Strategy, 16(2), 17-23.
Tversky, A., & D. Kahneman(1981, January). The framing of
decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453- 58.
Tversky, A., & D. Kahneman (1986, October). Rational choice and
the framing of decisions. Journal of Business, 4(2), 251-78.
Van de Ven, A. H. & M. S. Poole (1995, July). Explaining
development and change in organizations. Academy of Management Review,
20(3), 510-541.
Van de Ven, A. H., H. L. Angle, & M. S. Poole (2000). Research
on the management of innovation, New York, NY; Oxford University Press.
Martha C. Spears, Winthrop University
Darrell Parker, Georgia Southern University
Table 1
Framing the Loss of Life
Lives Saved Deaths Total
Certainty 46.21% 16.41% 31.54%
Gamble 53.79% 83.59% 68.46%
N 132 128 260
F-Test Two-Sample for
Variances
Death Variable 1 Variable 2
Mean 0.537313 0.839695
Variance 0.250477 0.135643
Observations 134 131
df 133 130
F 1.846589
t-Test: Two-Sample Assuming
Unequal Variances
Hypothesized Mean Difference 0
df 244
t Stat -5.61056
Table 2
Framing the Loss of Jobs
Jobs Lost Jobs Saved Grand Total
Certainty 38.64% 47.66% 43.08%
Gamble 61.36% 52.34% 56.92%
N 132 128 260
F-Test Two-Sample
for Variances
Jobs Variable 1 Variable 2
Mean 0.613636 0.527132
Variance 0.238897 0.251211
Observations 132 129
df 131 128
F 0.950979
t-Test: Two-Sample
Assuming Equal Variances
Pooled Variance 0.244983
Hypothesized Mean Difference 0
df 259
t Stat 1.41167
From sample responding to satisfied.