Moderators of the relationship between religiosity and alcohol use in college students.
Bodford, Jessica E. ; Hussong, Andrea M.
Alcohol use and abuse among college students is of serious concern
for a variety of social and health-related reasons. High levels of
consumption have been attributed to impaired academic performance, early
onset of substance dependence, risk of vehicle-related accidents, and
elevated threat of violence, sexual assault, and other illegal behaviors
(Ellison, Bradshaw, Rote, Storch, & Trevino, 2008). In 2007 the U.S.
Surgeon General declared underage alcohol use a chief health concern for
the nation, emphasizing the importance of research into the predictors
of and protectors against college-age drinking (Brown, Salsman,
Brechting, 8c Carlson, 2007). Recent investigations into these factors
have established a number of prevalent variables related to alcohol use
including both demographic (e.g., gender, ethnicity, socioeconomic
status) and biological factors (prenatal exposure, generic
predisposition, cognitive functioning) that remain relatively constant
throughout the lifespan (Chassin, Hussong, 8c Beltran, 2009). However,
environmentally determined correlates are more likely to differ as a
function of time as aspirations, stress, and peer groups undergo change
and maturation, likewise producing varying effects on substance use.
Consequently, although religiosity--that is, the quality of being
religious or of devout faith--has been regarded as one such
environmental protective factor in adulthood, the mechanisms behind this
relationship remain unclear among emerging adults (Brechting et al.,
2010; Convyn, 2002). Whereas research asserts that adults frequently use
alcohol as a means of coping with stress, anxiety, or depression (e.g.,
Grunbaum, Tortolero, Weller, & Gingiss, 2000; Strawser, Storch,
Geffken, Killiany, & Baumeister, 2004; Wills, Yaeger, & Sandy,
2003), drinking behaviors within the college population are primarily
driven by social motives (Bahr, Maughan, Marcos, & Li, 1998;
Perkins, 1987; Regnerus, 2006; Westmaas, Moeller, & Woicik, 2007).
For this very reason, studies have suggested that public
religiosity's impact on alcohol use is largely mediated by peer and
parent lifestyles, values, and socialization (Burkett, 1993; Ellison et
al., 2008; Perkins, 1987; Regnerus, 2006; Wood, Read, Mitchell, &
Brand, 2004). Even so, measurements of religiosity as a construct rarely
separate these social factors from more internal, private forms of
devotion and, when assessed with respect to substance use and other
risky behaviors, religiosity is not often analyzed beyond
participants' religious denomination and frequency of service
attendance. This study therefore aims to expand this limited area of
research within the emerging adult population, at which time college
students have the choice to pursue or refrain from opportunities to
establish new peer groups, drink while underage, maintain contact with
family, and become religiously involved, all of which may alter
perceptions of and behaviors toward alcohol.
Religiosity and Alcohol Use
The sparse literature concerning the association between
religiosity and substance use has generally not distinguished between
religion and spirituality, which have often been combined into a single
construct (e.g., Bert, 2011; Good, Willoughby, & Busseri, 2010).
Hodge, Andereck, and Montoya (2007) were among the first to distinguish
spirituality and religiosity based on the ways in which people exhibit
their beliefs. Specifically, they proposed that spirituality be defined
in individual, existential, and relational terms of an almost
transcendental nature (e.g., I often experience a feeling of unity with
the earth and all livingthings) while religion should be displayed as a
combination of communal, organizational, and structured expressions
(i.e., frequency of church attendance and religious organization
participation).
Building off this distinction, a study conducted by Borders,
Curran, Mattox, and Booth (2010) assessed religiosity through a set of
clear dimensions that not only addressed examples of faith and internal
thoughts but also separated private religious practices from spiritual
beliefs. This distinction between private religiosity and spirituality
is key for future research, because although the two are often
considered analogous, it is possible to hold spiritual beliefs without
worshipping the deity or set of deities of a particular religion
(Worthington, Kurusu, McCollough, & Sandage, 1996).
Public Religiosity and Socialization
Defined broadly, private religiosity is any action that is carried
out in accordance with one's religion but is not necessarily seen
by the wider community. Examples include internal thoughts and beliefs,
prayer and reflection, religious self-ranking, attachment to and
importance of religion, and personal religious awareness (Chitwood,
Weiss, & Leukefeld, 2008). Public religiosity, on the other hand,
constitutes participation in religious activities that entail social
contact and communication with others, such as frequency of service
attendance, involvement in religiously affiliated retreats and mission
work, participation in religiously based organizations, and roles within
each of these spheres.
It therefore follows that interactions with similarly involved
peers, which may take place through public religious means, may account
for a more personal outward religious experience (Borders et al., 2010).
Consequently, a small number of recent studies (Brownfield &
Sorenson, 1991; Edlund et al., 2010; Menagi, Harrell, & June, 2008)
were designed to examine the possibility that social support facilitates
the inverse relationship between religiosity and emerging adult alcohol
use, focusing specifically on feelings of solidarity and quality of
communication with parents, friends, and religious communities. It is
possible that these studies failed to find significant support for their
hypotheses at least in part because, as is the case in a majority of
research into this relationship, religiosity was defined through a
composite of private and public manifestations without studying their
unique impacts on reported drinking behaviors. Indeed, when Ozorak
(1986) measured religiosity only through involvement and active
participation, publicly religious students reported that their
relationships with many sources of social support were created from or
maintained through religious involvement.
It is therefore possible that while family, peer, and religious
cohesion could be related to religiosity, their social impacts (c.g., on
alcohol use) are most evident when interactions with these groups occur
through direct communication in religious meetings, services, and other
gatherings. This concept closely follows the theory of primary
socialization, which stems from parental influences on norms and values,
and secondary socialization, pertaining to involvement in academic and
religious institutions (Lubbers, Jaspers, & Ultee, 2009). Both forms
of socialization suggest that the morals and values established within
these three distinctive social groups (i.e., parents, friends, and
religious communities) can directly or indirectly influence college-age
drinking because these groups serve as a foundation for attitude and
belief formation. If public religiosity thus impacts social networks
through associations with individuals or larger groups who are
religiously involved in similar ways, the socialization theory would
suggest that publicly religious students are more likely to adopt the
belief systems, behaviors, and values instituted by the norms of their
social groups (Roberts, Koch, 8c Johnson, 2001). Consequently, peers and
parents may protect students from alcohol use if they are less inclined
toward underage drinking behaviors, regardless of whether those students
are religious.
Hypotheses
Collectively, because of the socially determined nature of
college-age alcohol use, this study focuses on the differing impacts of
public and private facets of religiosity on drinking behaviors in
college settings. More specifically, public manifestations entail some
level of social interaction through which attitudes in favor of or
against alcohol use can be created and emphasized, although such
manifestations may not accurately reflect internal belief structures
(e.g., students may be outwardly religious without holding private
religious beliefs). Because both college-age drinking and public
religiosity are motivated by social influences, the relationship between
these two constructs may therefore be compromised under certain socially
or religiously relevant circumstances while the relationship between
alcohol use and private religiosity continues to hold.
Taking into account the fundamental differences between public and
private religiosity and their relationships with socialization effects
and moral values, it is hypothesized that the negative association
between public or private religiosity and alcohol use is moderated under
the conditions that:
1. Private religious tendencies are less common than public
religious practices.
2. Peer and/or parent alcohol use and approval of underage drinking
is high.
3. Peers and/or parents are not religiously involved and previous
exposure to religiously affiliated schools is limited, as per the
socialization theory.
By examining the effects of these socially motivated factors on
alcohol use, we can further our understanding of the conditions under
which public and private religiosity separately protect against
college-age drinking. This study is the first to empirically overcome
the limitations of existing research both by focusing on distinct social
influences on drinking behaviors and by classifying religiosity as a
topic of primary interest. Not only has prior research on alcohol use
failed to place more than just minimal emphasis on religiosity's
protective impacts (Chitwood, Weiss, & Leu kefeld, 2008), but
scientists with a key interest in the study of religiosity have rarely
published in the field of substance abuse.
Because this intermittent overlap has hindered thorough
investigation, it is possible that by promoting and expanding this
limited area of research we can facilitate our understanding of
religiosity's protective influences, generalize these specific
components into a broader comprehension of social drinking, and enhance
existing prevention and intervention strategies to more effectively
reduce college-age alcohol use and its serious repercussions on
students, universities, and the larger community.
Method
Procedure
Students enrolled in an introductory psychology course were given
the option to complete a self-report inventory as part of a class
research participation requirement. The mean completion time of this
online questionnaire was 31.09 minutes (SD = 14.01 min). Of those who
started the survey, 4.49% left before completion and 1.63% were of legal
age (21 or older) and therefore dropped from further analysis. Questions
were separated into construct-themed blocks that were displayed in the
following order: demographics and background information; parent, peer,
and personal alcohol use and views toward alcohol; and parent, peer, and
personal religiosity. All aspects of this study were reviewed and
approved by the Institutional Review Board.
Participants
The sample was composed of 230 students (60.0% female) at a large
southeastern public university, all of whom were between the ages of 17
and 20 years old (M = 18.77, SD = 0.76). Of the respondents, 70.4% were
non-Hispanic White, 9.7% were Black or African American, 7.8% were
Hispanic, 7.4% were Asian, 3.9% were of two or more ethnicities, and
0.8% were of Native American or Pacific Islander descent. In terms of
gender and racial characteristics, the sample was representative of the
wider university undergraduate population (Office of the University
Registrar, 2010).
70% of students were raised in middle- to upper-class households
with a yearly income of at least $70,000 in 2010. At the time of
assessment, 79.1% of respondents lived in an on-campus residence hall
whereas 7.0% lived in a fraternity or sorority house and the remaining
13.9% lived in an off-campus house or apartment. No participants
reported living at home with parents or guardians.
With regard to religious affiliation, 75.2% of students were
affiliated with a Christian denomination, 75.7% of whom belonged to
Protestant denominations (e.g., Baptist, Methodist, Presbyterian,
Non-denominational). Of the 20.9% who were unaffiliated, 7.8% were
Agnostic (i.e., not sure if there is a God) and 6.5% each were Atheist (do not believe in God) or nothing in particular. Participants reported
that 84.9% of mothers and 81.7% of fathers belonged to Christian
denominations, with 7.8 and 11.3% unaffiliated, respectively. Those
comprising Jewish, Muslim, Buddhist, Hindu, and other world faiths were
uncommon, ranging between 3.9% of participants and 7% each of mothers
and fathers.
Measures
Alcohol use. Participants were asked to indicate how often they
drank alcohol during a typical month in the school year on a 9-point
Likert-type scale ranging in frequency from Not at all to Every day.
This item was recoded to reflect increasing intensity in frequency, so
that instead of attributing equal weight between 2 (Once a month), 6
(Twice a week), 8 (Four or five days a week), and so on, numbers were
distributed closer to the actual frequency of monthly drinking episodes.
Students who reported drinking at least once a month were then asked how
much they usually drank for each 24-hour period they drank during a
typical month, ranging from: Less than 1 drink to 9 or more drinks.
During both questions, participants were reminded that a
"drink" refers to a can or bottle of beer, a wine cooler, a
glass of wine, a shot of liquor straight, or a mixed drink.
A single alcohol use item was created by multiplying the frequency
of drinking episodes per month by the number of drinks per episode. The
average number of alcoholic drinks consumed per month within this sample
was 22.4 (SD = 26.1).
Greek involvement. Participants indicated their degree of
affiliation with on-campus Greek life on a 4-point Likert-type scale,
namely Not at all affiliated, Non-member who occasionally associates
with members, Non-member who frequently associates with members (e.g,
regular attendance at fraternity parties), or Active member (Sher,
Bartholow, & Nanda, 2001). As is summarized in Table 1, 31.3% of
respondents were active members at the time of data collection, 11.3%
were non-members who frequently associated with on-campus Greek events,
29.6% were non-members who occasionally associated with these
organizations, and the remaining 27.8% were not at all affiliated.
TABLE 1 Preliminary analyses of demographic covariates
Variable M (SD) / % Alcohol use Religiosity
M(SD) M(SD)
Gender F(1) = 18.37 *** F (1) = 2.58
Female 60.0% 16.56(20.30) 0.32(0.24)
Male 40.0% 31.06(31.04) 0.27(0.21)
Age 18.77 F (3) = 2.29 F (3) = 5.15 **
(0.76) [dragger]
18 41.7% 18.89(24.25) 0.34(0.24)
19 38.7% 22.30(24.35) 0.29(0.21)
20 19.1% 30.55(31.66) 0.23(0.22)
Income F (5) = 4.10 ** F (5) = 1.45
< $20,000 6.1% 9.32(14.08) 0.36(0.24)
520,000-539.999 7.0% 8.53(10.51) 0.32(0.27)
$40,000-570.000 17.0% 14.46(22.31) 0.34(0.26)
$70,000-5100,000 22.2% 26.59(30.45) 0.24(0.24)
5100.000-5150.000 20.0% 21.79(25.58) 0.34(0.18)
> $150,000 27.8% 30.52(26.35) 0.28(0.21)
Residence F (3) = 3.68 * F (3) = 2.50
On-campus dorm 79.1% 19.85(24.51) 0.32(0.24)
Greek house 7.0% 39.53(34.38) 0.27(0.20)
Off-campus residence 13.9% 28.56(27.45) 0.21(0.16)
Greek affiliation F (3) = 16.53 F (3) = 0.55
***
Active member 31.3% 8.40(20.35) 0.33(0.30)
Non-member, frequently 11.3% 18.26(23.14) 0.29(0.22)
associates
Non-member, 29.6% 30.97(25.88) 0.28(0.13)
occasionally
associates
Not at all affiliated 27.8% 35.53(26.27) 0.30(0.19)
*** p < 0.001, ** p <0.01, * p < 0.05, [dragger] p < 0.10
Religious affiliation. Religious affiliations and specific
denominations were extracted from the U.S. Religious Landscape Survey
(Pew Research Center, 2008), which has exhibited high test-retest
reliability (a = 0.93). Participants were asked to identify the
religious affiliation to which their mother and father separately
adhered during most or all of participants' childhood and
adolescence before reporting their own current religious affiliation.
Public religiosity. At the time of questionnaire compilation, no
scale separated religiosity into its public and private manifestations.
Eight items were therefore fashioned after examples of public religious
involvement cited in previous studies, namely: frequency of church or
religious service attendance in the previous month; on- or off-campus
religious organization attendance in the previous month and any active
roles (e.g., specific jobs, duties, or titles) held in such events;
mission work with a religious purpose or affiliation; and attendance at
religious retreats or conventions, studies or meetings, and religious
choir. Responses were averaged into a composite scale ranging from 0 to
1 with lower scores indicating lower degrees of public religiosity.
Inter-item correlations and internal consistency reliability were modest
(r = 0.28, a = 0.77).
Private religiosity. After indicating how often they prayed
privately, read religious scripture, listened to religious radio or
music, and watched religious television, participants indicated the
importance they prescribed to relying on religious teachings in times of
trouble, believing in God or a Higher Power or Creator, relying on
religious beliefs as a guide for day-to-day living, and turning to
prayer when facing personal problems. To expound upon aspects of
religiosity extending beyond concrete actions and perceived importance,
Rohrbaugh and Jessor (1975) provide a set of highly reliable religiosity
subscales embodying the non-social element of private religiosity, three
of which were used for the present study: ritual religiosity (i.e.,
prayer or religious meditation), consequential religiosity (When you
have a serious personal problem, how of ten do you take religious advice
or teaching into consideration?), and experiential religiosity (Do you
agree with the following statement? "Religion gives me a great
amount of comfort and security in life"). Each of the 11 items was
measured on a 5-point Likert-type scale, and all responses were compiled
into a single composite score ranging from 0 to 1 with lower scores
indicative of lower levels of private religiosity. Similar to public
religiosity scale analyses, this construct demonstrated a modest
inter-item correlation (r = 0.49) and level of reliability ([alpha] =
0.71).
Parent alcohol use. The Children of Alcoholics Screening Test
(CAST; Jones, 1982) was used to identify parents or guardians who are
problematic drinkers by asking participants to report their perceptions
of and experiences with each parent's drinking separately. The
scale comprises 30 items scored dichotomously and scored by the number
of positive answers, with six or more affirmative responses indicating
parental alcoholism. The CAST scale yielded a strong inter-item
correlation (r = 0.48) and reliability ([alpha] = 0.96) within this
sample. Descriptive statistics of all variables of interest are
summarized in Table 2.
TABLE 2 Construct and scale descriptive analyses
Construct M(SD) Range Items [alpha] Inter-item r
Drinking
Parent 2.3 (5.5) 0-30 30 0.96 0.48
alcohol use
Peer alcohol 0.5 (0.3) -0.3-0.9 7 0.79 0.79
use
Peer pressure 0.3 (0.2) 0-1 4 0.86 0.62
to drink
Peer approval 2.5 (1.3) 1-7 7 0.84 0.44
or alcohol
use
Parent 1.4 (0.7) 1-7 7 0.85 0.47
approval of
alcohol use
Religiosity
Public 0.1 (0.1) 0-0.6 8 0.77 0.28
religiosity
Private 0.2 (0.2) 0-0.7 11 0.71 0.49
religiosity
Peer 10.0 (8.0) 0-35 8 0.90 0.54
religiosity
Parent 31.1 (11.2) 10-50 10 0.93 0.59
religious
emphasis
Peer alcohol use. Peer alcohol use was assessed using the
seven-item scale presented in Bartholow, Sher, and Krull (2003). Of
these items, two pertain to peer perceptions of drinking and getting
drunk; three pertain to the number of close friends who drink, drink to
get drunk, and regularly get drunk; one relates to the amount these
friends typically drink per episode; and one relates to how often these
friends get drunk. Each question was scored on a Likert-type scale, with
a composite score indicating overall peer alcohol use with higher scores
indicative of heavier drinking behaviors. Within the current sample,
this scale showed a ately high inter-item correlation (r = 0.79) and
acceptable internal consistency reliability ([alpha] = 0.79).
Peer pressure to drink. Two items each from the Survey of Personal
and Social Development (SPSD2; Jessor, Costa, & Turbin, 2003) and
Jamison and Myers (2008) were used to measure the extent to which
participants feel pressured by peers to drink. All four items were
scaled from 0 (Never) to 3 (All the time) and pertained to how often
respondents were encouraged to drink; were encouraged to get drunk; were
encouraged to drink the same number of drinks as friends; and had the
same number of drinks as friends because their friends thought they
should. A composite score was calculated from answers to each of the
four items weighted equally and ranging from 0 to 1, with lower scores
indicating a lower degree of peer pressure to drink. Responses indicate
that these items were relatively closely correlated (r = 0.62) with
moderately high inter-item reliability ([alpha] = 0.86) and the combined
scale was highly correlated with peer approval of alcohol use (r = 0.48,
p <0.001) and peer alcohol use (r = 0.38, p < 0.001).
Peer and parent disapproval of drinking. Peer and parent
disapproval of underage alcohol use was assessed using the Injunctive
Norms Questionnaire (Baer, 1994). Additional items were used from the
Rhode Island Substance Abuse Survey (RISAS; Rhode Island Department of
Health, 2000), which was modified from the Monitoring the Future study
(Johnston, O'Malley, & Bachman, 1996). These measures ask the
extent to which parents or friends approve of various alcohol-related
scenarios on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from Strongly
disapprove to Strongly approve. All responses to these seven items were
averaged along a scale From 1 to 7, with lower scores indicating a
stronger degree of disapproval. For both peers and parents, these scales
revealed a relatively high inter-item correlation (r = 0.44; 0.47) and
reliability ([alpha] = 0.84; 0.85).
School affiliation and parent religious emphasis. To assess the
degree to which students were exposed to an emphasis on religion in
family and academic settings during adolescence, participants were asked
to report whether their middle and/or high school was religiously
affiliated. Analyses revealed that a majority (75.7%) of respondents did
not attend a religiously affiliated middle or high school. A smaller
proportion attended either a religiously affiliated middle school (5.2%)
or high school (5.7%) while the remaining 13.5 % of students spent the
approximately seven years prior to college in a religious academic
environment. Participants were then asked to report the extent to which
their parents or guardians emphasized family religion as they were
growing up. This construct was evaluated through the 10-item Religious
Emphasis Scale (RES; Altemeyer, 1988). Respondents selected the amount
to which each statement described their adolescence on a 6-point
Likert-type scale from None at all to A great deal. A total parent
religious emphasis score was determined by adding all 10 items. This
scale displayed a high inter-item correlation (r = 0.59) and degree of
reliability ([alpha] = 0.93) within the present sample.
Peer religiosity. The same items comprising the Public Religiosity
scale were asked concerning friends' religious involvement. Each
question prompted participants to respond with the number of their five
closest friends who met that particular criterion, with possible scores
ranging from 0 to 5. All eight responses were added to produce a single
scale of peer public religiosity, which demonstrated high inter-item
correlations (r = 0.54) and internal consistency ([alpha] = 0.90).
Results
Preliminary Analyses
To assess the possible influence of demographic co-variates,
participants' alcohol use and total religiosity (that is, the sum
of public and private religiosity scores) were regressed separately on
gender, age, income, residence, and Greek affiliation. Results from
these analyses of variance are reflected in Table 1, indicating that
although gender, income, residence, and Greek affiliation were
significantly related to alcohol use, only age bore a close negative
relationship with total religiosity (F[3] = 5.15,p = 0.002).
More specifically, the results showed that males drank
significantly more than females (F[1] = 18.37, p < 0.001) and alcohol
use increased only marginally with age (F[3] = 2.29, p = 0.08, ns).
There were significant differences in alcohol use as a function of
income distribution (F[5] = 4.10, p = 0.001) in that students tended to
drink significantly more if they came from a combined family income of
$150,000 or higher (M = 30.42, SD = 26.35, p = 0.02) compared to those
whose incomes ranged from $20,000 to $70,000 (M= 11.50, SD = 16.41, p =
0.02). Tukey HSD post-hoc analyses also indicated that residence played
a significant role in alcohol usc (F[3] = 3.68, p = 0.01) with higher
levels of drinking reported by students who lived in a fraternity or
sorority house at the time of assessment (M= 39.53, SD = 34.38) rather
than in an on-campus dormitory (M = 19.85, SD = 24.51, p = 0.02).
As is suggested by these findings in particular, a Tukey HSD
post-hoc analysis indicated that levels of drinking increased with
degree of Greek affiliation (F[3] = 16,531p < 0.001), with
respondents who were active members reporting significantly higher use
than those who were not at all affiliated (At = 8.40, SD = 20.35, p <
0.001) and non-members who occasionally associated with on-campus
fraternities and sororities (M= 18.26, SD = 23.14, p < 0.001), but
not compared with non-members who frequently associated with these
organizations (M = 35.53, SD = 26.08, p = 0.84, ns).
Correlations between alcohol use and religiosity constructs
revealed that, as predicted, alcohol use was significantly negatively
related to public religiosity (r =-0.18, p <0.01) and private
religiosity (r =-0.16, p = 0.02). Public religiosity was significantly
positively correlated with private (r = 0.55, p < 0.001).
Public Religiosity as a Predictor Variable
Prior to conducting ordinary least squares regression analyses to
test moderating hypotheses, public religiosity and private religiosity
were centered to increase the interpretability of interaction effects
and to avoid potential problems with multicollinearity. The OLS regression model comprised the following elements:
ALC = [[beta].sub.0] + [[beta].sub.1](PUBREL) + [[beta].sub.2](MOD)
+ [[beta].sub.3](PUBREL x MOD),
where standardized beta coefficients ([beta]) represented the
number of standard deviations the dependent variable ([gamma], alcohol
use) would change for each increase in standard deviation of the
predictor variable ([X.sub.1], public religiosity [PUBREL]; [X.sub.2],
the moderator variable [MOD]; and [X.sub.3], the interaction of the two
[PUBREL x MOD]). Significant interaction effects were further probed
using the computational procedures described by Hayes and Matthews
(2009). Because no demographic variables were significantly related to
both alcohol use and religiosity, no covariates were included in the
regression analyses.
Nine models estimated the moderating effects of the variables that
corresponded to the following three hypotheses of the current study,
each model separately taking into account the impacts of public and
private religiosity on alcohol use. These hypotheses include: (1)
private religiosity, (2) peer and parent alcohol use, disapproval of
use, and peer pressure, and (3) peer, parent, and high school religious
involvement. Results are depicted in Table 3.
TABLE 3
Results of moderation analyses of alcobol use and public religiosity
OLS Interaction
Model
Prediction R-sq F (df) P b se t
1. Private 0.52 4.09 (3,226) 0.01 -225.5 122.8 -1.84
religiosity
2. Peer 0.45 62.67 (3,226) 0.00 -87.8 35.3 -2.49
alcohol use
Parent 0.03 2.67 (3,226) 0.05 0.9 1.9 0.49
alcohol use
Peer pressure 0.26 26.87 (3,226) 0.00 -5.7 57.3 -0.10
to drink
Peer 0.20 19.36 (3,226) 0.00 3.3 11.8 0.28
disapproval
or alcohol
use
Parent 0.06 4.40 (3,226) 0.01 20.2 20.0 1.01
disapproval
of alcohol
use
3. Peer 0.05 4.31 (3,226) 0.01 -1.8 1.5 -1.24
religiosity
Parent 0.05 3.60 (3,226) 0.01 -2.4 1.4 -1.68
religious
emphasis
Prediction P
1. Private 0.07
religiosity
2. Peer 0.01
alcohol use
Parent 0.63
alcohol use
Peer pressure 0.92
to drink
Peer 0.78
disapproval
or alcohol
use
Parent 0.31
disapproval
of alcohol
use
3. Peer 0.22
religiosity
Parent 0.09
religious
emphasis
Private religiosity. The first model tested the hypothesis that low
levels of private religiosity would diminish the negative association
between public religiosity and alcohol use. Although this overall
regression model was significant ([R.sup.2] = 0.52, F[3,226] = 4.09, p =
0.01), the interaction was only marginally so (b =-225.5, p = 0.07, ns).
Probing of this interaction showed that public religiosity was a
significant predictor of alcohol use at high levels of private
religiosity (b =-34.8, p = 0.05) but not at mean (b = -2.0, p = 0.93,
ns) or low levels (b = 30.8, p = 0.42, ns), indicating that greater
private religiosity only predicted lower levels of alcohol use for those
students who had high levels of public religiosity (see Figure 1).
[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]
Peer and parent alcohol use and disapproval. Five models tested the
hypothesis that high levels of (1) peer or (2) parent alcohol use, low
levels of (3) peer or (4) parent disapproval of underage drinking, and
high exposure to (5) peer pressure would diminish the negative
association between public religiosity and alcohol use.
Although the moderating effect of parent alcohol use (b = 0.9, p =
0.63, ns) on the relation between private religiosity and alcohol use
was non-significant, the moderating effect of peer alcohol use on this
association was significant (b =-87.8, p = 0.01). Further probing of
this moderating effect indicated that public religiosity most strongly
predicted alcohol use when peer use was below average (b = 23.1, p =
0.06, ns), at which time alcohol use was positively related to public
religiosity (see Figure 2). The effect of public religiosity on alcohol
use was non-significant at mean (b = 0.1, p = 0.99, ns) and high levels
of peer use (b =-23.0.p = 0.26, ns), peer disapproval (b = 3.3, p =
0.78, ns), or parent disapproval (b = 20.2, p = 0.31, ns) on the
relation between religiosity and alcohol use.
[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]
Peer, school, and parent religiosity. This hypothesis stated that
if peers or parents were religiously involved, the relationship between
alcohol use and public religiosity would be significantly more negative
than that observed in participants whose peers and parents showed higher
levels of religious involvement. Similarly, secondary socialization
theory would suggest that exposure to religiosity through religiously
affiliated middle and/or high schools could also play a role in the
development of drinking behaviors as a function of religiosity. Although
the regression of public religiosity on alcohol use was significant
across levels of peer religiosity ([R.sup.2]= 0.05, F[3,226] = 4.31, p =
0.01), an interaction was not present (b =-1.8, p = 0.22, ns).
To assess the influence of exposure to religiously affiliated
academic settings, a one-way ANOVA evaluating participants'
religious educational background suggested that although those who
attended a religiously affiliated middle and/or high school did not
report significantly different levels of alcohol use (F[3] = 1.97, p =
0.12, ns) or private religiosity (1131 = 0.37, p = 0.77, ns) compared to
those who were not as frequently exposed to such environments, mean
scores of public religiosity varied significantly between the four
groups (F[3] = 2.66, p = 0.05). More specifically, a Tukey HSD post hoc test revealed that those who attended a religiously affiliated middle
school displayed higher levels of public religiosity than those who
attended an affiliated high school (NI = 0.02, SD = 0.03, p = 0.05) but
not those who attended both a middle and high school with a religious
affiliation (M = 0.09, SD = 0.12, p = 0.66, ns). Those who did not
attend an affiliated middle or high school did not report significantly
different levels of public religiosity compared to others (0.19 [less
than or equal to] p [less than or equal to] 0.70, ns).
After separately assessing the effect of peers and religious
environments, parent religious emphasis, public religiosity and their
interaction were regressed on alcohol use, yielding a significant
overall model (R2 = 0.05, F[3,226] = 3.60, p = 0.01) and a marginally
significant interaction effect (b =-2.4, p = 0.09, ns). Further probing
revealed that the effect of public religiosity on alcohol use was most
evident at high levels of parent religious emphasis in the home (b
=-56.1, p <0.01) but not at mean (b =-29.4, p = 0.12, ns) or low
levels (b = = 0.93, ns). A graph depicting these trends can be found in
Figure 3, which suggests that greater public religiosity is associated
with lower alcohol use only for those whose parents highly emphasize
religiosity.
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
Private Religiosity as a Predictor Variable.
All previously tested models were repeated in such a way that
private religiosity and the interaction term of private religiosity and
a moderating variable corresponding to any given hypothesis were
regressed on alcohol use as per the equation ALC = ([[beta].sub.0] +
[[beta].sub.1](PRIVREL) + [[beta].sub.2](MOD) + [[beta].sub.3](PRIVREL x
MOD). All regression models were significant (0.04 [less than or equal
to] R2 [less than or equal to] 0.45, 3.31 [less than or equal to]
F[3,226] [less than or equal to] 61.51, 0.001 [less than or equal to] p
[less than or equal to] 0.02), and results are presented in Table 4.
TABLE 4
Comparison of public and private religiosity as predictor variables
Private
religiosity
Prediction R-sq F (df) p b se t p b
2. Peer 0.45 61.51 0.00 -76.7 30.8 -2.49 0.01 -87.8
alcohol use (3.226)
Peer 0.21 19.48 0.00 11.8 8.2 1.45 0.15 3.3
disapproval (3,226)
of use
Parent 0.07 5.62 0.00 53.0 19.2 2.76 0.01 20.2
disapproval (3.226)
of use
Public
religiosity
Prediction se t p
2. Peer 35.3 -2.49 0.01
alcohol use
Peer 11.8 0.28 0.78
disapproval
of use
Parent 20.0 1.01 0.31
disapproval
of use
Peer alcohol use was a significant moderating factor (b =-76.7, p =
0.01), indicating that the relationship between alcohol use and private
religiosity was only significantly negative for those with peers who
drank heavily (b =-25.5, p = 0.06, ns), but not at mean (b = 14.7, p =
0.18, ns) or low levels (b =-5.4, p = 0.56, ns). Although these trends
appear to be roughly identical to those characterizing the relation
between alcohol use and public religiosity (see Figures 4 and 2), it is
worthy of note that public religiosity significantly predicted increased
drinking levels at low levels of peer use while private religiosity
significantly predicted decreased drinking at high levels of peer use.
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
In contrast, parent disapproval of alcohol use yielded a
significant interaction effect when predicted through private
religiosity (b = 53.0, p = 0.01) but not public religiosity (b = 20.2, p
= 0.31, ns). These trends, which are shown in Figure 5, indicate that
the effect of private religiosity on alcohol use was significantly
dependent on parents' disapproval of underage drinking (b = -60.21,
p < 0.001). Parent ambivalence--that is, neither approving nor
disapproving of alcohol use--also played a heavy role on the negative
association between alcohol use and private religiosity, although not as
strongly (b = -23.03, p = 0.05). For participants whose parents approved
of underage drinking, alcohol USC was not associated with private
religiosity (b = 14.1, p = 0.44, ns).
[FIGURE 5 OMITTED]
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to expand a previously limited area
of research investigating the impact of religiosity on alcohol use
within the college student population. Our findings underscore a number
of conditions under which religiosity does not as effectively protect
against alcohol use as a result of social, familial, and religious
factors.
Demographic Variables
Analyses of demographic covariates supported a number of trends
observed in previous studies with respect to the relationship between
alcohol use and gender, income, and Greek affiliation. More
specifically, higher levels of drinking have been noted in college males
compared to females, possibly as a result of increased risk-taking
behavior (Neumann, Leffingwell, Wagner, Mignogna, & Mignogna, 2009)
and perceived masculine norms related to intoxication (Iwamoto, Cheng,
Lee, Takamatsu, 8c Gordon, 2011; Carey & DeMartini, 2010). For those
who hail from affluent socioeconomic backgrounds, if leisurely
lifestyles were evident in the home it is possible that so too was the
pursuit of pleasure and relaxation in place of responsibilities (e.g.,
early morning part-time jobs necessitating sleep and sobriety), which
may have emphasized recreational drinking from an early age. Research
has also indicated that Greek organizations may encourage the perception
of alcohol as a facilitator of social interaction with others, often
underestimating and misperceiving the risks associated with underage use
(Barry, 2007; Park, Sher, Wood, & Krull, 2009; Capone, Wood,
Borsari, & Laird, 2007; Park, Sher, & Krull, 2008). These
findings collectively support the results of preliminary analyses within
this study.
As has been consistently observed in past research, alcohol use was
significantly negatively correlated with religiosity. Because this study
is original in its separation of religiosity into public and private
manifestations when analyzing alcohol use, it is further worthy of note
that this strong negative trend was observed in public and private
domains of religiosity. Because we would expect these scales to be
theoretically similar, their high degree of convergent validity suggests
that this is indeed the case.
Hypothesis Evaluation
The purpose of this study was to identify the conditions under
which religiosity effectively protected against alcohol use, looking
specifically at factors that would either strengthen or weaken the
negative relationship between public and private religiosity and alcohol
use. Our analyses indicated that not all tested factors were significant
moderators of this relationship. These significant and non-significant
findings shed light on the potential for social, familial, and religious
factors to impact college-age drinking behaviors.
Private religiosity. Public religiosity was a significant predictor
of alcohol use only at higher levels of private religiosity, in which
case the relationship between public religiosity and alcohol use was
significantly negative. Participants who scored low in private
religiosity tended to drink slightly more, rather than less, as public
religiosity increased. This marginally significant interaction effect
therefore supports our hypothesis that religiosity is not necessarily
negatively correlated with alcohol use in college students, as has been
indicated in past studies. Even when public religiosity is high,
religiosity's protective influence diminishes in the absence of
private religious actions such as prayer, belief in supernatural beings,
and perceived importance of religion.
A possible explanation for this finding is that students who attend
services, organizational meetings, retreats, and other forms of publicly
religious involvement do so for reasons that are not related to religion
at all (hence the lack of private religiosity) but rather peer
involvement, convenience, or customs relayed by family members in the
past. When public religiosity is balanced or exceeded by indications of
private religiosity, which pertain solely to religious beliefs and
internal practices, its long-held protective influence against alcohol
use persists. However, for those who view religious involvement as
synonymous with a pastime, voluntary commitment, or means of meeting
others without engaging in private religiosity, these defenses may prove
ineffective.
Peer and parent alcohol use and disapproval. Although the three
trends (i.e., low, mean, and high levels of peer use as shown in Figures
2 and 4) depicting alcohol use and peer use appear similar across levels
of public and private religiosity, two key differences set these
findings apart. First, public religiosity was a significant predictor of
alcohol use only for those whose peers drank little alcohol, yielding an
overall positive association between use and public religiosity. Second,
private religiosity significantly predicted drinking behaviors only at
high levels of peer use to produce a negative relationship between use
and private religiosity. It would therefore appear that the protective
impact of private religiosity buffers the effects of high peer use above
and beyond the influence of public religiosity, which may in fact
suggest a more ready compliance with social norms for those whose peers
do not engage in extreme drinking behaviors.
In suggesting that drinking tendencies are lower in those who are
not publicly religious, this positive relationship contrasts with what
might be expected as per the socialization theory--namely that young and
emerging adults typically mimic peer drinking behaviors (e.g.,
Bartholow, Sher, & Krull, 2003; Brownfield & Sorenson, 2010;
Ellison et al., 2008; Perkins, 1987; Sher, Bartholow, & Nanda,
2001). Because those who are highly religiously involved (i.e., high in
public religiosity) are more likely to be exposed to religious
communities, this greater degree of contact to such peer groups may lead
to a self-other comparison in which students who are highly publicly
religious view their peers' drinking tendencies as more
conservative than their own if those friendships were established and
maintained within religious contexts. Unlike many on-and off-campus
parties, alcohol is generally not featured in religious institutions and
organizational meetings, thereby leading to the perception that peers
established within those networks always behave in the same way that
they appear to act during these religiously affiliated events. Students
might then report perceived peer use in comparison to their own. It
would be beneficial for future research to further analyze this possible
explanation by assessing the ways in which participants met their
closest peers as well as the contexts in which they most often spend
time together.
As predicted, the regression of private religiosity on alcohol use
was significantly altered across levels of parent disapproval of alcohol
use, although not in the way that we expected. Similar to past findings,
students who were highly privately religious tended to drink less if
parents disapproved of underage alcohol use (Blobaum & Anderson,
2006; Welte, Barnes, Hoffman, & Dintcheff, 1999; Wood et al., 2004),
but also tended to drink more if parents approved of such behaviors. It
is possible that students who are high in private religiosity feel more
compelled to please their parents by complying with parental attitudes
toward underage alcohol use, although this explanation has yet to be
studied empirically. That these trends did not appear when assessing the
influence of public religiosity on alcohol use suggests that these
manifestations of religiosity either are not inherently protective
against use, as was the case for those with ambivalent parents (i.e.,
neither approving nor disapproving of alcohol use), or are not related
to compliance with parental attitudes.
Peer and parent religiosity. It is possible that peer religiosity
did not explain variability in alcohol use because when compared with
private and religiously affiliated universities, student religious norms
are less salient in larger, public university settings. Consequently,
even though examples of religious involvement can be outwardly observed,
participants might not be aware of friends' religious involvement
unless they are involved in similar organizations or religious
institutions during the school year.
Within families, however, frequent contact within the home
increases the likelihood that participants have a close understanding of
parent religiosity. This possibility is supported by the marginally
significant moderating effect observed with respect to parent religious
emphasis in the home. More specifically, higher parent religious
emphasis predicted lower levels of drinking only in those who reported
high levels of public religiosity, but in those with low public
religiosity, parent religious emphasis had the opposite effect. Past
studies in the domain of religiosity research have pinpointed a tendency
for young and emerging adults to rebel against parents' own
religious behaviors by reducing such religiosity while increasing levels
of substance use and risk-taking behaviors (e.g., Leonard, Cook,
Boyatzis, Kimball, & Flanagan, 2012; Putney & Middleton, 1961;
Rice, 1999; Wilson & Sherkat, 1994). Within the current study, it is
possible that students who were frequently exposed to religious
practices during their high school years (i.e., high parent religious
emphasis) but who displayed few forms of public religiosity in college
were demonstrating a need to go against parents'
conventions, thereby adopting a higher degree of alcohol use. Further
supporting this claim was the observation that for those who did not
grow up with parents who emphasized religiosity in the home, alcohol use
remained stagnant despite changes in levels of public religiosity.
However, it is worthy of note that such acts of rebellion were not found
in students who attended private, religiously affiliated schools, with
mean levels of alcohol use approximately equal to those of students who
did not attended public middle and high schools. To attempt to identify
such a rebellion effect longitudinally, it would be beneficial for
future studies to measure religiosity over the course of high school and
college years.
Implications and Directions for Future
Research Parent religious emphasis may lead to higher use in
students who stop attending religious services or who stop showing other
forms of religious involvement after leaving home for college. On this
point, it would be beneficial for future studies to investigate this
trend longitudinally from high school into the first several years of
college, sampling both from public and private, religiously affiliated
high schools. Furthermore, it is possible that there is an impact of
participant religious affiliation, which was not analyzed within this
study beyond as a demographic variable. Within certain affiliations it
is sometimes the case that illicit substances are seen as a way of
enhancing religious experience in either public or private settings.
Because customs within affiliations may vary, additional studies may be
interested in developing these questions to be specific to each to best
capture diverse forms of public and private religiosity as well as the
perceived norms of alcohol and substance use within each affiliation.
The impact of peer alcohol use is an important predictor of
drinking behaviors regardless of public or private religiosity,
stressing the importance of college prevention programs targeting not
religious or familial domains but instead use among friends and peer
groups. Because of the high degree of external validity of this sample
compared to the gender, racial, and ethnic characteristics of the wider
university, we feel that we can safely extend these findings to large,
public universities comprising similar proportions of student
demographics as well as a comparable distribution of parental income.
However, generalizability is limited such that all participants were
students in an introductory psychology course for which they received
research credit upon questionnaire completion. Future studies may wish
to expand external validity across universities or through
college-oriented survey websites.
While this initial study has indicated the importance of
distinguishing between private and public religiosity in the study of
underage alcohol use, these findings may serve as a basis for future
work in considering how other factors also associated with the college
transition (e.g., social groups, educational pursuits, religious
beliefs) might together impact drinking behaviors. Furthermore, it may
prove valuable to assess the impact of fundamentalism and students'
views of God or other deities (e.g., as forgiving or condemning) on
illicit substance use (Cook & Hillman, 2006; Cook, Larson, &
Boivin, 2003). A similar possible direction for future research would be
to capture peer closeness as measured through communication and bonding
as well as the degree to which respondents consider their peers to be
good role models to determine whether participants' affinities with
their peers impact the influence of peer alcohol use on drinking
tendencies as a function of religiosity. Similarly, it would be
beneficial to evaluate perceived alcohol use norms within the wider
university to better understand if student alcohol use stems from a
desire to fit in with their close peers or to fit in with the larger
student body. In this way, it would not be peer pressure to drink that
would dictate alcohol use but instead individuals' beliefs of
college student drinking norms regardless of whether or not those
students--be they friends, classmates, acquaintances, or
strangers--pressure them to engage in such behaviors.
Because our findings held significant differences stemming from two
distinct measures of religiosity, this study represents the importance
of separating public and private religiosity when studying alcohol use
and possibly other substance related behaviors in college students,
where outwardly visible forms of religious devotion are not necessarily
indicative of internal beliefs, religious importance, and dedication to
prayer. Future studies would benefit from separating the historically
single construct of religiosity into its distinct domains to better
assess the protective its nature against other negative factors
including depression, anxiety, and violent behavior, among others.
This study was one of the first to empirically evaluate both social
and distinct public and private religious influences operating on
alcohol use, focusing on religiosity as a topic of key interest. It is
our hope that these findings and those of future research will further
our understanding of the predictive and protective factors of alcohol
use within the emerging adult population, thereby enhancing college
prevention and intervention strategies to more efficiently reduce
student drinking behavior and its consequences on society.
References
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing
authoritarianism. San Francisco, CA US: Jossey-Bass.
Baer, J. S. (1994). Effects of college residence on perceived norms
for alcohol consumption: An Examination of the first year in college.
Rsychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8, 43-50.
doi:10.1037/0893-164X.8.1.43
Bahr, S. J., Maughan, S. L., Marcos, A. C., & Li, B. (1998).
Family, religiosity, and the risk of adolescent drug use. Journal of
Marriage & the Family, 60, 979-992.
Barry, A. E. (2007). Using theory-based constructs to explore the
impact of Greek membership on alcohol-related beliefs and behaviors: A
Systematic literature review. Journal Of American College Health, 56(3),
307-315.
Bartholow. B. D., Sher. K. J., & Krull, J. L. (2003). Changes
in heavy drinking over the third decade of life as a function of
collegiate fraternity and sorority involvement: A Prospective,
multilevel analysis. Health Psychology, 22, 616-626.
doi:10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.616
Bert, S. C. (2011). The Influence of religiosity and spirituality
on adolescent mothers and their teenage children. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence, 40, 72-84. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9506-9
Blobaum, E. M., & Anderson, J. E (2006). The Impact of exposure
and perceived disapproval of underage drinking. Criminal Justice
Studies: A Critical Journal of Crime, Law & Society, 19, 171-192.
doi:10.1080/14786010600764559
Borders, T. F., Curran, G. M., Mattox, R., & Booth, B. M.
(2010). Religiousness among at-risk drinkers: Is it prospectively
associated with the development or maintenance of an alcohol-use
disorder? Journal of Studies on Alcohol & Drugs, 71, 136-142.
Brechting, E. H., Brown, T. L., Salsman, J. M., Sauer, S. E..
Holman, V. T., & Carlson, C. R. (2010). The role of religious
beliefs and behaviors in predicting underage alcohol use. Journal of
Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 19, 324-334.
doi:10.1080/1067828X.2010.502494
Brown, T. L., Salsman, J. M., Brechting, E. H., & Carlson. C.
R. (2007). Religiousness, spirituality, and social support: How are they
related to underage drinking among college students? Journal of Child
& Adolescent Substance Abuse, 17, 15-39.
Brownfield, D. & Sorenson, A. M. (1991). Religion and drug use
among adolescents: A Social support conceptualization and
interpretation. Deviant Behavior, 12, 259-276.
Burkett, S. R. (1993). Perceived parent's religiosity.
friend's drinking, and hellfire: A Panel study of adolescent
drinking. Review of Religious Research, 35. 134-154.
Capone, C., Wood, M. D., Borsari, B., & Laird, R. D. (2007).
Fraternity and sorority involvement, social influences, and alcohol use
among college students: A prospective examination. Psychology Of
Addictive Behaviors, 21(3), 316-327. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.21.3.316
Carey, K., & DcMartini, K. (2010). The motivational context for
mandated alcohol interventions for college students by gender and family
history. Addictive Behaviors, 35(3), 218-223.
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.10.011
Chassin. L., Hussong, A., & Beltran, I. (2009). Adolescent
substance use. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of
adolescent psychology. Vol I: Individual bases of adolescent development
(3rd ed.) (pp. 723-763). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Chitwood, D. D., Weiss, M. L., & Leukefeld, C. G. (2008). A
Systematic review of recent literature on religiosity and substance use.
Journal of Drug Issues, 38, 653-688.
Cook, K. V. & Hillman, E. (2006). Christian views of self and
God: Context matters. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 34(2),
133-141.
Cook, K. V., Larson, D. C., & Boivin, M. D. (2003). Moral
voices of women and men in the Christian liberal arts college: Links
between views of self and views of God. Journal of Moral Education,
32(1). 77-89.
Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Church attendance or expressions of
religiosity: Predictors of adolescent drug use. Marriage 6- Family
Review, 5(1), 79-91.
Edlund. M. J., Harris, K. M., Koenig, H. G., Han, X., Sullivan. G.,
Mattox, R., & Tang, L. (2010). Religiosity and decreased risk of
substance use disorders: Is the effect mediated by social support or
mental health status? Social Psychiatry e- Psychiatric Epidemiology. 45.
827-836. doi:10.1007/s00127-009-0124-3
Ellison. C. G., Bradshaw, M., Rote, S., Scorch, J., & Trevino,
M. (2008). Religion and alcohol use among college students: Exploring
the role of domain-specific religious salience. Journal of Drug Issues,
38, 821-846.
Good, M., Willoughby, T., & Busseri, M. A. (2010). Stability
and change in adolescent spirituality/religiosity: A Person-centered
approach. Developmental Psychology, 1-13. doi:10.1037/a0021270
Grunbaum, J. A., Tortolero, S., Weller, N., & Gingiss, P.
(2000). Cultural, social, and interpersonal factors associated with
substance use among alternative high school students. Addictive
Behaviors, 25, 145-151.
Hayes, A. and Matthews, J. (2009). Computational procedures for
probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 924-936.
Hodge, D. R., Andereck. K., & Montoya, H. (2007). The
Protective influence of spiritual-religious lifestyle profiles on
tobacco use, alcohol use, and gambling. Social Work Research, 31,
211-219.
Iwamoto, D., Cheng, A., Lee, C. S., Takamatsu, S., & Gordon, D.
(2011). "Man-ing" up and getting drunk: The role of masculine
norms, alcohol intoxication and alcohol-related problems among college
men. Addictive Behaviors, 36(9), 906-911.
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.04.005
Jamison, J. J., & Myers, L. B. (2008). Peer-group and price
influence students drinking along with planned behaviour. Alcohol and
Alcoholism, 43, 492-497. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agn033
Jessor, R., Costa, F. M., & Turbin, M. S. (2003). SPSD2: Survey
of Personal and Social Development; University of Colorado.
Johnston, L. a, O'Malley, P. M., & Bachman, J. G. (1996).
National survey results on drug use from the Monitoring the Future
Study, 1975-1995: Vol. I. Secondary school students (NIH Publication No.
96-4139). Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.
Jones, J. W. (1982). Preliminary test manual: The Children of
Alcoholics Screening Test. Chicago: Family Recovery Press, Distributed
through Camelot Unlimited.
Leonard, K. C., Cook, K. V., Boyarzis, C. J., Kimball, C. N., &
Flanagan, K. S. (2012). Parent-child dynamics and emerging adult
religiosity: Attachment, parental beliefs, and faith support. Psychology
of Religion and Spirituality, 1-14. doi:10.1037/a0029404
Lubbers, M., Jaspers. E., & Ultee, W. (2009). Primary and
secondary socialization impacts on support for same-sex marriage after
legalization in the Netherlands. Journal of Family Issues, 30,
1714-1745.
Menagi, F. S., Harrell, Z. A. T., & June, L. N. (2008).
Religiousness and college student alcohol use: Examining the role of
social support. Journal of Religion & Health, 47, 217-226.
doi:10.1007/s10943-008-9164-3
Neumann. C., Leffingwell, T., Wagner. E., Mignogna, J., &
Mignogna, M. (2009). Self-esteem and gender influence the response to
risk information among alcohol using college students. Journal Of
Substance Use, 14(6), 353-363. doi:10.3109/14659890802654540
Office of the University Registrar. (2010.June). University of
North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Historical Enrollment Statistics:
Frequently Requested Reports. Available from
http://regweb.unc.edu/stats/freq_reports.php.
Ozorak, E. (1986, August 22). The Development of religious beliefs
and commitment in adolescence. Paper presented at the 94th Annual
Convention of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Park, A., Sher, K. J., Wood, P. K., & Krull, J. L. (2009). Dual
mechanisms underlying accentuation of risky drinking via
fraternity/sorority affiliation: The role of personality, peer norms,
and alcohol availability. Journal Of Abnormal Psychology, 118(2),
241-255. doi:10.1037/a0015126
Park, A., Sher. K. J., & Krull, J. L. (2008). Risky drinking in
college changes as fraternity/sorority affiliation changes: A
person-environment perspective. Psychology Of Addictive Behaviors,
22(2), 219-229. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.22.2.219
Perkins, H. W. (1987). Parental religion and alcohol use problems
as intergenerational predictors of problem drinking among college youth.
Journal fir the Scientific Study of Religion, 26, 340-357.
Pew Research Center. (2008, February). U.S. Religious Landscape
Survey. Religious Affiliation: Diverse and Dynamic. Available from
http://religions.pewforum.org/pdf/report-religious-landscapc-study-full.pdf.
Putney, S., & Middleton, R. (1961). Rebellion, Conformity, and
Parental Religious Ideologies. Sociometry, 24(2), 125-135.
Regnerus, M. D. (2006). Adolescent delinquency. In H.R. Ebaugh
(Ed.), Handbook of Religion and Social Institutions (pp. 265-282). New
York: Springer Publishing Co.
Rhode Island Department of Health. (2000, June). The 1998 Rhode
Island Adolescent Substance Abuse Survey: Report if statewide results.
Available from http://www.health.ri.gov/chic/statistics/asasl.htm.
Rice, E. (1999). Religion and the adolescent: A psychodynamic perspective. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 16(1), 58-75.
doi:10.1037/0736-9735.16.1.58
Roberts, A. E., Koch, J. R., & Johnson, D. (2001). Religious
reference groups and the persistence of normative behavior: An Empirical
test. Sociological Spectrum, 21, 81-98. doi:10.1080/02732170150201829
Rohrbaugh, J. & Jessor, R. (1975). Religiosity in youth: A
Personal control against deviant behavior. Journal of Personality, 43,
136-155.
Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Nanda, S. (2001). Short- and
long-term effects of fraternity and sorority membership on heavy
drinking: A Social norms perspective. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,
15,42-51. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.15.1.42
Steele. C. M., Southwick, L. L., & Critchlow, B. (1981).
Dissonance and alcohol: Drinking your troubles away. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 41(5). 831-846.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.41.5.831
Strawser, M. S., Storch, E. A., Geffken, G. R., Killiany, E. M.,
& Baumeister, A. L. (2004). Religious Faith and Substance Problems
in Undergraduate College Students: A Replication. Pastoral Psychology,
53(2), 183-188. doi:10.1023/BPASP.0000046829.61450.4c
Welte, J. W., Barnes, G. M., Hoffman, J. H., & Dintcheff. B. A.
(1999). Trends in adolescent alcohol and other substance use:
Relationships to trends in peer, parent and school influences. Substance
Use & Misuse, 34, 1427-1-449. doi:10.3109/10826089909029391
Westmaas, J., Moeller, S., & Woicik, P. B. (2007). Validation
of a measure of college students' intoxicated behaviors:
Associations with alcohol outcome expectancies, drinking motives, and
personality. Journal of American college Health, 55, 227-237.
Wills, T. A., Yaeger, A. M., & Sandy, J. M. (2003). Buffering
effect of religiosity for adolescent substance use. Psychology of
Addictive Behaviors, 17, 24-31.
Wilson, J., & Sherkat, D. E. (1994). Returning to the fold.
Journal For The Scientific Study of Religion, 33(2), 148.
Wood. M. D., Read, J. P., Mitchell, R. E., & Brand, N. H.
(2004). Do parent still matter? Parent and peer influences on alcohol
involvement among recent high school graduates. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 18, 19-30. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.18.1.19
Worthington, E. L., Kurusu, T. A., McCollough, M. E., &
Sandage, S. J. (1996). Empirical research on religion and
psychotherapeutic processes and outcomes: A 10-year review and research
prospectus. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 448-487.
Jessica E. Bodford
Arizona State University
Andrea M. Hussong
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Author Information
BODFORD, JESSICA E. Address: Arizona State University, P.O. Box
871104, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104. Email: jessica@bodford.com. Currently:
Graduate Student (Social Psychology) Arizona State University. Degrees:
B.S. (Psychology), B.A. (Spanish) University of North Carolina at Chapel
Hill. Specializations: Cyberpsychology, culture, and decision-making.
HUSSONG. ANDREA M. Ph.D. Address: University of North Carolina at
Chapel Hill CB#3270. Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3270. Title: Director, Center
for Developmental Science, Professor of Psychology. University of North
Carolina at Chapel Hill. Specializations: Developmental risk of land
resilience to substance use.