The effects of a manualized group-psychotherapy intervention on client God images and attachment to God: a pilot study.
Thomas, Michael J. ; Moriarty, Glendon L. ; Davis, Edward B. 等
The goal of this pilot study was to examine the effects of an
8-week, manualized, outpatient group-psychotherapy intervention on
client god images and attachment to God. Participants were 26 adults who
reported a Christian religious affiliation and who sought religiously
based, group-psychotherapy treatment for difficulties in their emotional
experience of God (i.e., negative god images). The treatment protocol
reflected a psychotherapy-integrationist approach to treating god-image
difficulties. Treatment chiefly included psychoeducational,
dynamic-interpersonal, and cognitive interventions, although it also
included allegorical-hibliotherapy and art/music interventions. The pre-
and post-test questionnaire included the Attachment to God Inventory (R.
Beck & McDonald, 2004) and a brief God adjective-checklist, along
with several open-ended questions. Participants reported experiencing
adaptive shifts in their god images and attachment to God. Specifically,
when pre- and post-questionnaire ratings were compared, they reported
experiencing God emotionally as more accepting, intimate, and supportive
and as less disapproving, distant, and harsh. In addition, they reported
experiencing significantly both less attachment anxiety with God and
less attachment avoidance with God. Furthermore, they reported
experiencing more congruence between their emotional experience of God
(god images) and their theological beliefs about God (god concepts). The
interventions that were deemed the most therapeutically effective were
the allegorical-bibliotherapy and the cognitive-restructuring
interventions. Clinical implications and limitations are discussed.
As Hathaway (2003) has highlighted, within the broad field of
mental health, the past 20 years have marked a significant increase in
the clinical attention that is devoted to religious/spiritual issues.
For example, the American Psychological Association's (2002) Ethics
Code now includes religion among the domains of diversity that
mental-health professionals must address in a respectful and culturally
responsive manner (Hays, 2007; Richards & Bergin, 2000). Another
such development is the inclusion of the Religious or Spiritual Problem
diagnostic category (V62.89) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition-Text Revision (American Psychiatric
Association, 1994; see Turner, Lukoff, Barnhouse, & Lu, 1995). As
its name implies, this category is meant to be used "when the focus
of clinical attention is a religious or spiritual problem"
(American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 685). For religiously/
spiritually oriented clients, god-image difficulties are a common
Religious or Spiritual Problem that motivates them to seek psychotherapy
(Allmond, 2009). In particular, such clients often experience a
clinically significant discrepancy between their god concepts and their
god images (Yarborough, 2009)-that is, between their head knowledge of a
divine attachment figure (DAF; e.g., God, Allah, Jesus, Buddha, Krishna,
etc.) and their heart knowledge of that same figure, respectively
(Davis, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, in press).
In the current article, we report findings from a pilot study of an
8-week, manualized, outpatient group-psychotherapy intervention
(entitled "Discovering God") that was designed to treat
god-image difficulties, with a specific view toward improving client god
images and attachment to God. Our treatment protocol reflected a
psychotherapy-integrationist approach (Norcross & Golfried, 2005;
cf. Moriarty & Davis, in press). It chiefly included
psychoeducational, dynamic-interpersonal, and cognitive interventions,
although it also included allegorical-bibliotherapy and art/music
interventions. In the section that follows, we will briefly describe our
psychodynamic-cognitive conceptualization of god images, on which both
our treatment manual and our clinical interventions were based.
DEFINITIONS, DEVELOPMENT, AND DYNAMICS
Definitions
As Davis (2010) has described, god images are the affect-laden
mental representations that underlie a person's embodied, emotional
experiences in relationship with a DAF, such as God, Allah, Jesus,
Buddha, or Krishna. They are primarily comprised of implicit relational
knowledge (i.e., "gut-level procedural knowledge of how to perceive
and be in relationship with a general or specific relational
partner," Moriarty & Davis, in press; cf. Hall, 2004;
Kihlstrom, 2008; Lyons-Ruth et al., 1998). Basically, god images
"guide and integrate how a person experiences [the DAF] at an
emotional, physiological, largely nonverbal, and usually implicit level
(i.e., outside of conscious awareness; Davis, 2010; cf. Fogel, 2009;
Noffke & Hall, 2007)" (Moriarty & Davis, in press). Hall
(2007) has thus described god images as a type of attachment filter (p.
24), through which embodied, emotional experiences with a DAF are
mediated.
In contrast, god concepts are the belief-laden mental
representations that underlie a person's conscious, linear
knowledge about a DAF. They are chiefly comprised of semantic memory
(i.e., "general knowledge about the world, including words and
concepts, their properties and interrelations," Smith &
Kosslyn, 2007, p. 541; cf. Davis, 2010; Siegel, 2010), Essentially, they
guide and integrate how a person thinks and talks about a DAF at an
abstract, theological, conceptual, and usually explicit level (i.e.,
inside of conscious awareness; Davis, 2010). As such, god concepts may
be thought of as a type of doctrinal filter, through which theological,
abstract thoughts and discussions about a DAF are mediated (cf. Hall,
2007).
Development and Dynamics
God-image development and dynamics. It is beyond the scope of this
article to comprehensively describe the development and dynamics of god
images and god concepts (see Davis, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, in
press, for such reviews). However, we will provide a brief summary here,
highlighting the points that group-participants read about in our
treatment manual (see Thomas, 2009, Appendix G, for a copy of this
manual).
As Davis (2010) has detailed, our conceptualization of god-image
development and dynamics is based on Hall's (2004)
relational-spirituality theory (cf. Hail, 2007; Hall, Fujikawa, Halcrow,
Hill, & Delaney, 2009). Consequently, it is undergirded by the
central organizing principles of that theory, as enumerated in Hall
(2004):
Central Organizing Principle #1. People are fundamentally motivated
by, and develop in the context of emotionally significant relationships.
(p. 68)
Central Organizing Principle #2. There are multiple codes of
emotional information processing which provide a theoretical framework
for understanding the way in which close relationships are processed and
internalized, thereby shaping the patterns of our relationships with
God, self and others. (p. 69)
Central Organizing Principle #3. Implicit relational
representations are repetitions of relational experiences, sharing a
common affective core, that are conceptually encoded in the mind as
non-propositional meaning structures. They are the memory basis for
implicit relational knowledge; that is, our "gut level" sense
of how significant relationships work. (p. 71)
Central Organizing Principle #4. Implicit relational
representations, formed particularly from experiences [in] early
relationships with caregivers, shape the emotional appraisal of meaning
and subsequent patterns of relationship. (p. 72)
Central Organizing Principle #5. Implicit relational
representations and knowledge form the foundation of our knowledge of
self and others because they are processed automatically, and are not
under the direct control of knowledge in the form of words that [are]
processed in a linear manner. (pp. 73-74)
Our treatment manual reflected a psychotherapy-integrationist
approach (Norcross & Goldfried, 2005; cf. Moriarty & Davis, in
press). As such, our conceptualization of god-image development and
dynamics highlighted ideas from several theoretical traditions. For
example, from the classical-psychoanalytic tradition (Freud, 1913/1950,
1930/1962; Jones, 1991, 2007), we stressed the pervasive and
long-lasting influence of early-childhood experiences in relationship
with one's caregivers. From the object relations tradition
(Rizzuto, 1979; Winnicott, 1951/1975, 1971), we emphasized the processes
of internalization (Davis, 2010; Moriarty, 2006; Schafer, 1968) and of
separation-individuation (Mahler, Pine, & Bergman, 1975), the former
via the caregiver-child relationship and the latter via the use of God
as a transitional object (Moriarty, 2006; Winnicott, 1951/1975, 1971).
From the cognitive tradition (A. T. Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979;
DeRubeis, Tang, & A. T. Beck, 2001), we underscored the concepts of
core beliefs (Moriarty, 2006), automatic thoughts (A. T. Beck et al.,
1979), self-schemas (Markus, 1977; Markus & Kunda, 1986), and
cognitive distortions (DeRubeis et al., 2001). Last, from the attachment
tradition (Bowlby, 1973; Collins, Guichard, Ford, & Feeney, 2004),
we underlined the interconnections among internal working models of
self, God, and others, particularly the reliable similarities between
one's global (i.e., generalized) internal working models of self
and one's global internal working models of God (Benson &
Spilka, 1973; Buri & Mueller, 1993; Yarborough, 2009; cf. Davis,
2010; Hall et al., 2009; Moriarty & Davis, in press).
In this latter regard, our conceptualization of god-image
development and dynamics affirmed the implicit-relational-knowledge
correspondence hypothesis (Hall, 2004, 2007; Hall et al., 2009), which
posits that individuals' experiences in relationship with human
attachment figures (e.g., caregivers, peers, romantic partners, and
coreligionists) undergirds and parallels their experiences in embodied,
emotional relationship with God. Importantly, according to this
hypothesis, people's implicit relational knowledge of how to
perceive and be in relationship with God is reflected in their implicit
religious/spiritual functioning (i.e., "their felt
religious/spiritual experience at a relational, motivational, emotional,
and physiological level," Moriarty & Davis, in press) but not
necessarily in their explicit religious/spiritual functioning (i.e.,
"their religious/spiritual behaviors and consciously articulated
beliefs; e.g., religious/spiritual commitment, church attendance, and
theological beliefs," Moriarty & Davis, in press; see Davis,
2010; Hall et al., 2009, for reviews).
There is preliminary research support for the
implicit-relational-knowledge hypothesis (Davis, 2010; Hall et al.,
2009). For example, Hall et al. (2009) found that adult participants
from the four main attachment groups (secure, anxious, avoidant, and
fearful) differed significantly on three of four implicit
religious/spiritual functioning indicators (interpersonal unforgiveness,
spiritual community, and attachment anxiety with God) but did not
exhibit significant differences on the explicit religious/spiritual
functioning indicator (explicit religious/spiritual commitment).
God-concept development and dynamics. In contrast to their god
images, people's god concepts are highly related to their explicit
religious/spiritual history and functioning. Hoffman (2005) has
described god concepts as mainly deriving from informal and formal
learning, via such avenues as religious/spiritual texts and the teaching
and modeling of parents and religious/spiritual leaders (Davis, 2010;
Grimes, 2007).
In the case of religious/spiritual persons who report having a
personal relationship with a DAF, it is likely that their god concepts
are relatively adaptive and "accurate," from the standpoint of
their primary religious/spiritual texts (e.g., the Bible, Book of
Mormon, Qur'an; Davis & Moriarty, 2008). However,
religious/spiritual individuals often experience disparities between the
DAF they believe in explicitly (god concepts) and the DAF they
experience implicitly (god images; see Davis, 2010; Lawrence, 1997;
Moriarty, 2006; Moriarty & Davis, in press). The degree and
qualitative nature of such disparities will vary from person to person.
Significant disparities may reflect the operation of what Bucci (1997)
has termed desymbolization or dissociation, whereby "the
connections between the subsymbolic and the symbolic components of the
schemas are cut" (Bucci, 1997, p. 202; cf. Davis, 2010; Davis &
Moriarty, 2008) or what interpersonal neurobiologists (e.g., Badenoch,
2008; Siegel, 2010) have referred to as a lack of integration, where
integration is defined as the "linkage of differentiated parts of a
system [e.g., the mind/bran-1]" (Siegel, 2010, p. 262; see Davis,
2010, for a review).
RESEARCH ON PROMOTING GOD-IMAGE CHANGE THROUGH PSYCHOTHERAPY
Research on promoting god-image change through psychotherapy is
admittedly sparse. At the time of writing this article (February 2011),
there were only two published, peer-reviewed studies in this area:
Tisdale et al. (1997) and Cheston, Piedmont, Eanes, and Lavin (2003).
Tisdale et al. (1997)
First, Tisdale et al. (1997) examined god-image change through
religiously based, object-relations-oriented, multimodal
inpatient-psychiatric treatment. All participants were self-identified
evangelical Christians and most of them were Caucasian and of middle or
upper-middle socioeconomic status. The mean age was 37 (range: 17-65
years old). Most participants had a primary diagnosis of Major
Depressive Disorder and had never before been psychiatrically
hospitalized. The average length of hospitalization was 20.2 days
(range: 10-43 days).
The following self-report measures were administered at admission
(N = 99), at discharge (N= 99), at 6 months following discharge (n =
67), and at 12 months following discharge (n = 30): (a) the Bell Object
Relations Reality Testing Inventory (Bell, 1991; measuring object
relations maturity and reality testing); (b) the Personal Self subscale
of the Tennessee Self-Concept Scale (Fitts, 1965; measuring overall
self-worth); (c) the Presence, Challenge, and Acceptance subscales of
the God Image Scales (Lawrence, 1997; measuring God's felt
presence, desire for one's growth, and valuation of one's
boy-ability, respectively); and (d) the Religious Experience
Questionnaire (Edwards, 1976; measuring the overall experience of God
emotionally as loving and close). Psychiatric inpatient treatment
included the following interventions: (a) pharmacotherapy (as needed),
(b) 30 minutes of individual psychotherapy (5 days per week), (c) 90
minutes of group psychotherapy (7 days per week), (d) 60 minutes of
religiously based psychoeducation (7 days per week), and (e) milieu
treatment (e.g., occupational therapy, community meetings, and
vocational counseling; as needed; Tisdale et al., 1997).
Overall, participants reported experiencing adaptive changes in
their god images, when their admission and discharge scores were
compared. Specifically, at the end of their psychiatric-inpatient
treatment, they reported experiencing God emotionally as more close,
loving, accepting, and present, compared with when they entered
treatment. These therapeutic gains were maintained over the course of
the 12-month follow-up, although without further improvements. It is
important to note that adaptive changes in participants' god images
were consistently associated with adaptive changes in their self-images.
In fact, these adaptive shifts in self-images were dramatic in
comparison to the adaptive shifts in god images, suggesting that
god-image change is associated with (and perhaps evenmediated by)
improvements in self-images. Improvements in self-worth were also
retained across the 12-month follow-up period (Tisdale et al., 1997; cf.
Davis, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, in press).
Cheston et al. (2003)
In a similar study, Cheston et al. (2003) examined god-image change
through general (i.e., not necessarily religiously based) outpatient
individual-psychotherapy. Participants included a nonrandomized
convenience sample of 98 adults-30 in the treatment group (23 women, 7
men; mean age: 39.3 years) and 68 in the control group (53 women, 15
men; mean age: 50.1 years). All participants completed two self-report
measures--the God Adjective Check List (Gough & Heilbrum, 1983;
measuring god-image characteristics) and the Brief Symptom Inventory
(Derogatis, 1993; measuring psychological symptoms). The 30 individuals
in the treatment group completed these measures within 4 weeks of
beginning psychotherapy (time 1) and then either at termination or at
the end of 6 months (time 2), whichever came later. The 68 individuals
in the control group completed these measures at point of first contact
(time 1) and then from 2 to 6 months later (mean intertest interval: 4
months). For the individuals in the treatment group, the mean intertest
interval was 6.7 months (range: 6-19 months), and the mean number of
sessions was 20 (range: 2-52 sessions). Of note, at time 1 and time 2,
each psychotherapy-client's therapist completed the Derogatis
Psychiatric Rating Scale (Derogatis, 1978; a clinician-rated measure of
psychological symptoms) on the respective client. Also, at time 2, the
therapist rated the client's overall emotional growth (single-item
measure) and overall spiritual growth (single-item measure).
When self-ratings at time 1 and time 2 were compared, psychotherapy
participants reported experiencing significantly decreased psychological
symptoms over time, whereas control participants tended to reliably
remain asymptomatic. Similarly, psychotherapy participants reported
experiencing adaptive changes in their god images over time, whereas
control participants tended to reliably experience God in an emotionally
healthy manner. More specifically, psychotherapy participants reported
experiencing God emotionally as lower in trait neuroticism and as higher
in trait agreeableness; however, this treatment effect was only
evidenced among those clients who showed high emotional growth over
time, as per clinician-rating. Moreover, the clients who evidenced high
spiritual growth over time tended to show a concomitant shift in
experiencing God emotionally as more loving, caring, and compassionate
(Cheston et al., 2003). As Moriarty and Davis (in press) have concluded
Cheston et al.'s study suggests the possibility that the
common factors of psychotherapy (e.g., empathy, positive regard) may
facilitate god-image change indirectly, via other forms of emotional
change, such as improvements in self-image and adaptive shifts in
implicit relational knowledge.
Furthermore, as Cheston et al. (2003) indicated: "The
psychological and spiritual selves of clients may be integrally
connected; thus, when something shifts for one part of the self, it
shifts for another part of the self as well" (Cheston et al., 2003,
p. 106).
Current State of the Research Evidence
Taken together, Tisdale et al. (1997) and Cheston et al. (2003)
have offered research evidence that adaptive god-image change can be
effected through (a) religiously based, object-relations-oriented,
multi-modal inpatient-psychiatric treatment (Tisdale et al., 1997) and
(b) general outpatient individual-psychotherapy (Cheston et al., 2003).
However, each of these studies suffered from notable methodological
limitations. For example, for ethical reasons (e.g., psychiatric
inpatients ethically cannot be put onto a waiting list), Tisdale et
al.'s study did not have a control group, thereby limiting internal
validity. In addition, Tisdale et al. did not evaluate the relative
effects of the different inpatient-treatment components, thereby
limiting treatment specificity (i.e., conclusions regarding the active
ingredients of patient change; Chambless & Hollon, 1998; Chambless
& 011endick, 2001).
Cheston et al.'s (2003) study was more methodologically
rigorous. Even so, there were noteworthy limitations in that study also.
For instance, the treatment group was small (N = 30), thus limiting
power, and females were over-represented (n = 23), thus limiting
external validity. Moreover, Cheston et al. did not randomly assign
treatment and control participants, limiting the study's internal
validity. Furthermore, they did not evaluate the degree to which
religious/spiritual components were explicitly addressed in
psychotherapy, limiting an understanding of the treatment specificity
(Chambless & Hollon, 1998; Chambless & Ollendick, 2001). In sum,
the research evidence on promoting god-image change through
psychotherapy participation is still preliminary.
The Current Study
In our pilot study, we sought to build on this research by (a)
studying a manualized treatment intervention and (b) studying the
specific impact of religiously based, psychotherapy integrationist,
outpatient group-psychotherapy. Building on the above-mentioned research
findings, we predicted the following, with regards to
group-participants' answers on the pre- and post-questionnaires:
a. Hypothesis 1. We predicted that group participants would report
experiencing less attachment anxiety with God.
b. Hypothesis 2. We predicted that group participants would report
experiencing less attachment avoidance with God.
c. Hypothesis 3. We predicted that group participants would report
experiencing God emotionally as more accepting, intimate, and
supportive.
d. Hypothesis 4. We predicted that group participants would report
experiencing God emotionally as less disapproving, distant, and harsh.
e. Hypothesis 5. We predicted that group participants would report
experiencing more congruence between their emotional experience of God
(god images) and their theological beliefs about God (god concepts).
METHOD
Participants
Participants were adults who sought group-psychotherapy treatment
for difficulties in their emotional experience of God (i.e., negative
god images). As such, each group member had a primary diagnosis of
Religious or Spiritual Problem (V62.89; American Psychiatric
Association, 1994). Several members had a secondary diagnosis of Major
Depressive Disorder or Anxiety Disorder Not Otherwise Specified.
Notably, 10 group members were concurrently receiving religiously based,
outpatient individual-psychotherapy and/or pharmacotherapy.
Screening and attrition. Thirty-seven individuals participated in
the study's screening session. At that point, four participants
decided not to participate in the study, and one person was screened out
because other clinical difficulties were more prominent and necessitated
more in-depth treatment than our manualized group-psychotherapy
intervention was able to provide. Thirty-two participants began the
group-psychotherapy process, but six individuals dropped out, for
various reasons (e.g., one individual moved out of the area, two decided
to solely participate in individual-psychotherapy, and three did not
provide a reason).
Pilot-study sample. In the end, 26 adults (11 men, 15 women, Mage =
29.6, SDage = 6.56, age range: 1946 years old) completed the 8-week
group-psychotherapy intervention. Most were Caucasian graduate students
(from various disciplines), of middle to upper-middle socioeconomic
status. Twenty-five participants reported a Protestant-Christian
affiliation; one, a Catholic-Christian affiliation. A total of seven
psychotherapy-groups were conducted, each using the same 8-week
manualized treatment protocol. The mean number of group participants was
3.7, and all groups included both male and female participants.
Group Psychotherapists
Each group was facilitated by two doctoral-level students in
clinical psychology--one male and one female. In total, there were seven
group psychotherapists who facilitated these groups. Each
psychotherapist participated in a 4-hour training that was aimed at
preparing them to effectively co-facilitate their respective group, in
faithful adherence to the manualized treatment protocol. Partly to
ensure adequate client care and partly to ensure protocol adherence,
this article's second author (GLM) met with the psychotherapists
weekly for group supervision.
Procedures
Recruitment. This study was conducted at a mid-Atlantic, religious
university. Participants were primarily recruited from the student body
of that institution, via on-campus publicity efforts (e.g., mails,
flyers, word-of-mouth). A few other participants were recruited from the
surrounding community, via local churches and a local community mental
health center. The recruitment materials emphasized how the group was
dedicated to helping people develop increased congruence between their
head and heart knowledge of God. The flyers were labeled
"Discovering God: A Group Therapy Experience" and featured a
large wardrobe with a young girl peeking inside, thereby alluding to C.
S. Lewis's (19501956/2001) classic book The Lion, the Witch, and
the Wardrobe.
Manualized treatment protocol. Prior to the screening session, all
prospective participants read and signed an informed consent form and
then completed a demographic form. Of the pilot-study sample (N = 26),
all participants completed the pre-test questionnaire following session
1 and the post-test questionnaire following session 8. They also
received a copy of the study's debriefing form, following session
8. The entire manualized treatment protocol consisted of eight,
90-minute group-psychotherapy sessions. Sec Table 1 for an overview of
the manualized treatment protocol (see Thomas, 2009, Appendix C, for a
copy).
MATERIALS
The pre- and post-test questionnaire included the Attachment to God
Inventory (R. Beck & McDonald, 2004) and a brief God
adjective-checklist, along with several open-ended questions. The
Attachment to God Inventory (AGI) is a 28-item self-report measure of
adult attachment tendencies in emotional relationship with God. It is a
self-report, survey-based measure of implicit relational spirituality
(Hall et al., 2009) and thereby of god images. The AGI consists of two
subscales--the Anxiety subscale (measuring anxiety about abandonment;
e.g., "I worry a lot about my relationship with God") and the
Avoidance subscale (measuring avoidance of intimacy; e.g., "I am
uncomfortable being emotional in my communication with God"). Each
subscale contains 14 items, some of which are reverse-scored and all of
which involve rating along a seven-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 =
disagree strongly to 4 = neutral/mixed to 7 = agree strongly; R. Beck
& McDonald, 2004).
Our brief God adjective-checklist was developed for this study. It
consisted of three positively valenced trait adjectives--accepting,
intimate, and supportive--and three negatively valenced trait
adjectives--disapproving, distant, and harsh. Respondents rated the
degree to which the trait adjective tended to describe how they
experienced
TABLE 1
An Overview of the Manualized Treatment Protocol
Session Context Intervention description
In-session Welcome and introductions
1 Group overview, ground rules, and basic
rationale
Completion of the Draw-A-God figure-drawing
(Moriarty, 2006)
Psychoeducation on god images and god concepts
Discussion of god-image/god-concept
discrepancies
Post-session Completion of the pre-test questionnaire
Homework Parent/God-Image Grids exercise examining the
relative impact of early caregiver-child
relationships on god images (Moriarty, 2006)
2 In-session Review of homework
Psychoeducation on and discussion of the
connections between early-childhood experiences
(e.g., wounds) and subsequent god-image
difficulties
Dynamic-interpersonal exercise in which members
play the role of "the Real God," speaking to
another member's wounds
Psychoeducation on god-image development
(psychodynamic view)
Homework Select a meaningful song/hymn and read its
lyrics twice daily
3 In-session Review of homework
Psychoeducation on an orthodox Christian
theology of god images, followed by an
introduction to a cognitive-theory view of god
images
Discussion and film clip of the
thoughts-feelings connection
Write a spiritual-journal letter to oneself from
the Real God
Homework Continue to read the meaningful song/hymn twice
daily
4 In-session Further psychoeducation on a cognitive-theory
view of god images
Psychoeducation on the God Image Automatic
Thought Record (GIATR;
Moriarty, 2006), followed by verbal practice
completing one
Homework Complete the GIATR in writing, at least 3 times
during the week
Attend a 30-minute individual-psychotherapy
session with a group cofacilitator, to discuss
treatment progress and to complete a GIATR
Begin to sing the meaningful song/hymn twice
daily
5 In-session Psychoeducation on cognitive distortions,
interspersed with applied discussion
Homework Complete the GIATR in writing, at least 3 more
times during the week
Start to read the C. S. Lewis (1950-1956/2001)
book The Horse and His Boy
Continue to sing the meaningful song/hymn twice
daily, and add another song/hymn
6 In-session Psychoeducation on schemas and god images,
interspersed with applied discussion
Homework Continue to complete GIATRs and to read The
Horse and His Boy
Continue singing the songs/hymns twice daily but
may change them at any time
7 In-session Psychoeducation on
allegorical-bibliotherapy and its use in
changing god images
Discussion of reactions to The Horse and His
Boy, focusing on experiences of Asian
Exercise of watching and discussing reactions to
a film clip depicting Asian
Homework Continue to complete GIATRs
Continue singing the songs/hymns twice daily but
may change them at any time
Write down a word of encouragement for each
group member, to share next session
8 In-session Group overview, reflections, and celebration
Dynamic-interpersonal exercise in which members
encourage and thank one another for their
respective growth-promoting contributions to the
group
Post-session Completion of the post-test questionnaire
God emotionally, using a five-point Likert scale that ranged from 1
(very accurate) to 3 (neutral) to 5 (very inaccurate).
On both the pre- and post-test questionnaires, participants were
asked to rate how similar their emotional experience of God (god images)
was to their theological beliefs about God (god concepts), using a
ten-point Likert scale in which lower scores indicated lower congruence
and higher scores indicated higher congruence. Last, on the post-test
questionnaire only, participants were asked three exploratory,
open-ended questions. Specifically, they were asked to identify (a) the
psychological intervention that most influenced their emotional
experience of God, (b) the treatment-manual component that most
influenced their emotional experience of God, and (c) the primary
mechanism by which interactions with other group members influenced
their emotional experience with God.
RESULTS
Hypotheses 1 and 2
We predicted that group participants would report experiencing less
attachment anxiety with God (Hypothesis 1) and less attachment avoidance
with God (Hypothesis 2). Paired-samples t tests yielded support for each
of these hypotheses (see Table 2). Only 20 pilot-study participants
completed the AGI, because it was not administered to the first
psychotherapy group (n = 4), and two other group members did not
complete all the AGI items, for unknown reasons.
Hypotheses 3 and 4
We predicted that group participants would report experiencing God
emotionally as more accepting, intimate, and supportive (Hypothesis 3)
and as less disapproving, distant, and harsh (Hypothesis 4). Paired
samples t tests yielded support for each of these hypotheses (see Table
3). All 26 pilot-study participants completed the brief God
adjective-checklist.
TABLE 2
Contrast of Pre- and Post-Test Scores on the Attachment to
God Inventory (N = 20)
Pre-test Post-test
Variable M (SD) M (SD) t( 19)
Attachment anxiety with God 69.90 (17.5) 56.50 (20.5) 4.25
Attachment avoidance with God 50.60 (178) 43.70 (14.5) 3.57
Variable P
Attachment anxiety with God .001
Attachment avoidance with God .002
Note. For each subscale of the Attachment to God Inventory,
higherscores indicate higher levels of the assessed attachment
tendency, and total subscale scores can range from 7 to 98.
TABLE 3
Contrast of Pre- and Post-Test Scores on the Brief God
Adjective-Checklist (N = 26)
Pre-test Post-test
Variable M (SD) M (SD) t(25)
Positively valenced trait adjectives
Accepting 3.08 (1.3) 1.80 (0.7) 5.94
Intimate 3.23 (1.4) 185 (10) 5.72
Supportive 2.73 (13) L85 (0.9) 3.00
Negatively vaIenced trait adjectives
Disapproving 2.15 (12) 3.62 (0.9) -6.54
Distant 165 (0.9) 3.58 (13) -6.68
Harsh 3.00 (14) 4.04 (0.9) -4.24
Variable p
Positively valenced trait adjectives
Accepting .001
Intimate .001
Supportive .006
Negatively vaIenced trait adjectives
Disapproving .001
Distant .001
Harsh .001
Note. For each trait adjective, lower scores indicate
higher levels of the degree to which the trait adjective
self-reportedly describes one's emotional experience of
God, with scores ranging from I (very accurate) to 3
(neutral) to 5 (very inaccurate).
Hypothesis 5
We predicted that group participants would report experiencing more
congruence between their emotional experience of God (god images) and
their theological beliefs about God (god concepts) at the end of
treatment. As stated previously, on the pre- and post-test
questionnaires, participants were asked to rate how similar their
emotional experience of God was to their theological beliefs about God,
using a ten-point Likert scale in which lower scores indicated lower
congruence and higher scores indicated higher congruence. (All 26
pilot-study participants completed this item.) To quantify this
god-image/god-concept discrepancy, we categorized respondents based on
their item-rating: (a) Severe Discrepancy (ratings of 1 to 3), (b)
Moderate Discrepancy (4 to .5), (c) Mild Discrepancy (6 to 7), and (d)
Minimal Discrepancy (8 to 10). On the pre-test questionnaire, the
frequency breakdown was as follows: (a) Severe Discrepancy (n = 10), (b)
Moderate Discrepancy (n = 9), (c) Mild Discrepancy (n = 5), and (d)
Minimal Discrepancy (n = 2). In contrast, on the post-test
questionnaire, the frequency breakdown was as follows: (a) Severe
Discrepancy (n = 1), (b) Moderate Discrepancy (n = 4), (c) Mild
Discrepancy (n = 12), and (d) Minimal Discrepancy (n = 9). To
statistically examine this quasi-interval data, we conducted a
paired-samples t test, which revealed support for Hypothesis 5 as well,
t(25) = -5.84, p [less than] .001.
TABLE 4
Most Commonly Cited Answers to Open-Ended Questions on
the Post-Test Questionnaire (n = 23)
Commonly cited answer n
Most influential intervention
The Horse and His Boy (allegorical-bibliotherapy) 9
God Image Automatic Thought Record (cognitive 7
restructuring)
Psychoeducation on connections between 4
early-childhood experiences and subsequent
god-image difficulties
Most influential treatment-manual component
God Image Automatic Thought Record (cognitive 9
restructuring)
Parent/God-Image Grids exercise examining the 9
relative impact of early caregiver-child
relationships on god images (Moriarty, 2006)
Psychoeducation on connections between 4
early-childhood experiences and subsequent
god-image difficulties
Primary mechanism of group-member influence
Open sharing and insightful comments from other 13
group members (interpersonal input and output)
Feeling as if they were not alone and as if they 10
were experiencing similar difficulties to other
group members (universality)
Safe environment allowed for authentic interaction 6
among group members (cohesiveness and catharsis)
Commonly cited answer [percentage] of
n
Most influential intervention
The Horse and His Boy (allegorical-bibliotherapy) 39[percentage]
God Image Automatic Thought Record (cognitive 30[percentage]
restructuring)
Psychoeducation on connections between 17[percentage]
early-childhood experiences and subsequent
god-image difficulties
Most influential treatment-manual component
God Image Automatic Thought Record (cognitive 39[percentage]
restructuring)
Parent/God-Image Grids exercise examining the 39[percentage]
relative impact of early caregiver-child
relationships on god images (Moriarty, 2006)
Psychoeducation on connections between 17[percentage]
early-childhood experiences and subsequent
god-image difficulties
Primary mechanism of group-member influence
Open sharing and insightful comments from other 57[percentage]
group members (interpersonal input and output)
Feeling as if they were not alone and as if they 43[percentage]
were experiencing similar difficulties to other
group members (universality)
Safe environment allowed for authentic interaction 26[percentage]
among group members (cohesiveness and catharsis)
Exploratory Analyses
As stated above, on the post-test questionnaire, participants were
asked to identify (a) the psychological intervention that most
influenced their emotional experience of God, (b) the treatment-manual
component that most influenced their emotional experience of God, and
(c) the primary mechanism by which interactions with other group members
influenced their emotional experience with God. See Table 4 for the most
commonly cited answers to each of these three open-ended questions. Only
23 participants answered this portion of the questionnaire.
DISCUSSION
Summary of Findings
In sum, on average, participants in our 8-week, manualized,
outpatient group-psychotherapy intervention reported experiencing
adaptive shifts in their god images and attachment to God. Specifically,
when pre- and post-questionnaire ratings were compared, they reported
experiencing God emotionally as more accepting, intimate, and supportive
and as less disapproving, distant, and harsh. In addition, they reported
experiencing significantly both less attachment anxiety with God and
less attachment avoidance with God. Furthermore, they reported
experiencing more congruence between their emotional experience of God
(god images) and their theological beliefs about God (god concepts).
Last, the interventions that were deemed the most therapeutically
effective were the allegorical-bibliotherapy and the
cognitive-restructuring interventions.
Clinical Implications
Our pilot study has several implications for clinicians who are
addressing god-image difficulties in clinical practice. Perhaps most
substantively, our findings support Rizzuto's (1974) claim that god
images are indeed changeable. Our findings provide further support that
psychotherapeutic treatment can lead to adaptive changes in god images,
as Tisdale et al. (1997) and Chcston et al. (2003) have previously
shown.
Our results also provide preliminary support for the possibility
that group psychotherapy is an effective treatment format for treating
negative god images. This finding is consistent with the findings of
O'Hare (2003), whose small-scale dissertation study (N = 6) also
revealed evidence that group psychotherapy can lead to adaptive
god-image change.
As stated above, our participants were asked to identify the
primary mechanism by which interactions with other group members
influenced their emotional experience with God. Here, the most commonly
cited answers were (a) open sharing and insightful comments from other
group members (interpersonal input and output), (h) feeling as if they
were not alone and as if they were experiencing similar difficulties to
other group members (universality), and (c) that the safe environment
allowed for authentic interaction among group members (cohesiveness and
catharsis). Speaking of god-image treatment in a group-psychotherapy
format, Moriarty and Davis (in press) have highlighted the potential for
the relationships among group members to provide yet another change
mechanism, in addition to actual psychological interventions. Our
findings support this possibility. Indeed, group members consistently
reported that their interactions with other group members had a
substantive positive impact on their god images, particularly the open
sharing and insightful comments from other group members (interpersonal
input and output). In addition, many participants initially felt as if
they were alone in their god-image struggle, but through meeting other
people who had similar problems (universality), they were able to
connect with each other and to grow toward health--individually and
collectively. In short, our results are consistent with the reliable
research finding that interpersonal input, interpersonal output, and
universality are among the therapeutic factors that outpatient
group-psychotherapy participants frequently cite as powerful change
mechanisms (see Yalom & Leszcz, 2005, p. 88). Our participants also
felt as if the safe environment of the group allowed for authentic
interaction among group members, and this cited change mechanism seems
to relate to the therapeutic factors of cohesiveness (related to the
experience of safety) and catharsis (related to the experience of
authentic interaction; Yalom & Leszcz, 2005, p. 88).
Our research provides preliminary support for the use of a
manualized group-therapy protocol in the treatment of god-image
difficulties, as well as for the use of a psychotherapy-integrationist
approach in such treatment (Norcross & Goldfried, 2005; cf. Moriarty
& Davis, in press). When we asked participants to identify the
treatment-manual components that were the most influential in promoting
adaptive change in their god images, the most frequently cited answers
were (a) the GIATR (cognitive restructuring), (b) the Parent/God-Image
Grids exercise (examining the relative impact of early caregiver-child
relationships on god images [Moriarty, 20061), and (c) psychoeducation
on connections between early-childhood experiences and subsequent
god-image difficulties. All these components relate to the change
mechanism of increases in self-understanding, which is another mechanism
that outpatient group-psychotherapy participants often cite as effective
(Yalom & Leszcz, 2005, p. 88).
Next, our results provide preliminary evidence that
allegorical-bibliotherapy and cognitive-restructuring interventions are
particularly effective mechanisms for promoting adaptive shifts in god
images and attachment to God. With regard to allegorical-bibliotherapy
interventions, Moriarty and Davis (in press) have discussed the use of
the Chronicles of Narnia series (Lewis, 1950-1956/2011):
Christian clients readily identify with the characters who interact
with Asian (i.e., God). Through identification with these characters,
clients learn to make sense of difficult personal situations and to
experience God emotionally as more affirming and experience-near,
particularly during trying times.
Indeed, in our study, thirty-nine percent of the participants
stated that reading C. S. Lewis's The Horse and His Boy was
influential in improving their god images. This book specifically
addresses the role of God in the pain and suffering of different
characters in the book. Although we did not ask our participants what
specific elements of the allegorical-bibliotherapy were impactful for
them, we did have a few notable observations. First, participants stated
they were able to empathize with many characters in the novel. Many
characters inappropriately perceived Asian (i.e., God) to be distant and
harsh when in fact he was intimate and intentional in how and when he
interacted with each character. Given that many participants initially
experienced God emotionally as distant, it was profoundly healing for
them to read a novel in which the God-figure (Asian) is convincingly and
poignantly portrayed as ever-present and experience-near. Thus, the
impactful theme that emerged through reflecting on the novel was that
although God may seem distant or harsh at times, God's actions are
perhaps more appropriately viewed as intimate, intentional, loving, and
caring, even when they do not feel like it in the moment. Remarkably, by
and large, group participants were able to internalize this message and
to generalize it to their own emotional experience of God.
Second, in their experience of the allegorical-bibliotherapy
intervention, participants reported that they experienced relief from
reading and reflecting on the words Asian spoke in the novel. In The
Horse and His Boy (1950-1956/2011), when Asian enters into the
allegorical-narrative, he often speaks words that encourage, affirm, and
empower the character(s) with whom he is interacting. Many participants
reported that these words from Aslan were encouraging, affirming, and
empowering for them as well, particularly as it related to promoting
their ability to experience God emotionally as more accepting, intimate,
and supportive.
With regard to the use of cognitive-restructuring interventions
(e.g., GIATRs), it is noteworthy to point out that our use of GIATRs was
quite incorporative of emotions (see Moriarty & Davis, in press, for
a description of this intervention and for a copy of the GIATR). Here,
we were mindful of the dangers Moriarty and Davis (in press) have
addressed elsewhere:
While treating god-image concerns with cognitive-behavioral
interventions, it is easy to focus solely on thoughts and behaviors,
thereby fostering surface-Ievel shifts in client god concepts instead of
promoting deeper-level changes in client god images. Fortunately,
cognitive-behavioral techniques can be used in ways that affirm and
involve both cognition and emotion. (cf. Cozolino, 2010)
Limitations
Lack of a control group. As stated above, both Tisdale et
al.'s (1997) and Cheston et al.'s (2003) studies had notable
limitations, and our pilot study was no exception. Perhaps most notably,
as with Tisdale et al.'s study, our pilot study did not have a
control group (and thus there was no random assignment), given the
exploratory nature of our research. Consequently, we are unable to offer
definitive conclusions regarding what caused the changes in
participants' god images and attachment to God.
Limits to treatment specificity. This threat to our study's
internal validity is also related to the fact that, during at least a
portion of our study, ten of our group-therapy participants were
concurrently in individual psychotherapy, most often for the treatment
of depression and/or anxiety. Therefore, it is difficult to specify the
degree to which our study's findings were due to our
group-psychotherapy intervention, compared with the relative influence
of other factors (e.g., individual psychotherapy, extratherapy factors).
Low sample size. Another limitation of our study was its low sample
size (N = 26). Having such a low number of participants limited our
study's power. Even so, the fact that all our hypotheses were
statistically supported with such low power provides promising evidence
that true differences in the examined variables exist.
Limits to external validity. One more limitation of our study was
the demographic makeup of our sample. Our population of interest was
adults who report having difficulties in their emotional experience of
God (i.e., negative god images). Even though our sample was generally
representative in terms of gender (11 men, 15 women), it was
over-representative of young, highly educated, Caucasian,
Protestant-Christian adults, of middle to upper-middle socioeconomic
status. Thus, study's findings are most appropriately generalized
to young adults who are demographically similar to our sample.
Use of self-report, survey-based measures. Another limitation of
our study was the sole use of self-report, survey-based measures. As
Gibson (2007) and Moriarty and Davis (in press) have explained, such
measures are the most popular way to clinically assess god images.
However, these measures may be quite limited in their ability to validly
assess respondents' heart knowledge of God (god images),
particularly given the embodied, emotional, implicit, nonverbal nature
of god-image functioning (see Davis, 2010; Moriarty & Davis, in
press). In assessing god images (which largely involve right-brain
mediated processes) via self-report, survey-based measures (which tend
to tap into largely left-brain mediated processes; Moriarty & Davis,
in press; cf. Gibson, 2007), there is often a danger that respondents
will deduce the "right" (i.e., researcher-expected) answer and
thus respond in a biased manner. Gibson (2007) has raised such concerns
and has offered some suggestions for using self-report, survey-based
measures in the most optimal manner. Following Gibson's
suggestions, we framed questions in ways that explicitly asked
respondents to discriminate between their head and heart knowledge of
God.
In short, our study had many of the same limitations as Tisdale et
al.'s (1997); both studies may be classified as Type 3 studies,
according to the seminal classification system espoused in A Guide to
Treatments That Work (3rd ed.; Nathan & Gorman, 2007). Nathan and
Gorman (2007) have described:
[Type 3 studies] are clearly methodologically limited, Generally,
Type 3 studies are open treatment studies aiming at obtaining pilot
data. They are highly subject to observer bias and can usually do little
more than indicate if a treatment is worth pursuing in a more rigorous
design ... . Such studies can, of course, provide a great deal of
naturalistic information but are prone to all of the problems of
uncontrolled data collection and retrospective recall error. (pp.
vii-viii)
Notably, because of its higher degree of methodological rigor,
Cheston et al.'s (2003) study may be classified as a Type 2 study,
which Nathan and Gorman (2007) have described in the following manner:
[Type 2 studies] arc clinical trials in which an intervention is
made, but some aspects of the Type 1 study requirement are missing. For
example,-- a trial in which two treatments are compared but the
assignment is not randomized ... . Such studies clearly do not merit the
same consideration as Type 1 studies but often make important
contributions and generally should not be ignored. (p. vii)
Future Research on God Images
It is important to note that, as of yet, no specified
psychological-interventions have enough research evidence to be deemed
either "well-established" or "probably efficacious"
in treating negative god images (Chambless & Hollon, 1998; Chambless
& Ollendick, 2001). For the god-image research literature to
continue to grow and mature,
Type 1 studies are especially needed. Here, Nathan and Gorman
(2007) have explained: [Type 1 studies] are the most rigorous and
involve a randomized, prospective clinical trial. Such studies also must
involve comparison groups with random assignment, blinded assessments,
clear presentation of exclusion and inclusion criteria, state-of-the-art
diagnostic methods, adequate sample size to offer statistical power, and
clearly described statistical methods. (p. vii)
Future psychotherapeutic-outcome research on god images should also
be conducted with samples that are more representative of age,
race/ethnicity, religious/spiritual affiliation, and socioeconomic
status than was our pilot-study sample. Furthermore, future research
should utilize a multimodal assessment approach, ideally including some
combination of clinician-rated measures; implicit measures (e.g., a
reaction-time test; Yarborough, 2009); projective assessments; and
self-report, survey-based measures (see Gibson, 2007; Moriarty &
Davis, in press, for reviews). Moreover, future research should explore
the specific mechanisms of therapeutically mediated change in god
images, in both individual-therapy and group-therapy formats. Last,
future research should further examine the use of treatment manuals,
perhaps even exploring options of either using another therapeutic
modality (e.g., narrative-experiential; Moriarty & Davis, in press)
or meeting in a different counseling context (e.g., a church-based
setting).
REFERENCES
Allmond, J. A. (2009). God images in Christian psychology,
spiritual direction, and pastoral counseling (Doctoral dissertation).
Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses. (AAT 3370134).
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition. Washington, DC:
Author.
American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of
psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060-1073.
Badenoch, B. (2008). Being a brain-wise therapist: A practical
guide to interpersonal neurobiology. NewYork, NY: Norton.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. {1979).
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, R., & McDonald, A. (2004). Attachment to God: The
Attachment to God Inventory, tests of working model correspondence, and
an exploration of faith group differences. Journal of Psychology and
Theology, 32,92-103.
Bell, M. (1991). An introduction to the Bell Object Relations
Reality Testing Inventory. Unpublished manuscript.
Benson, P. & Spilka, B. (1973). God image as a function of
self-esteem and locus of control. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 12, 297-310.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
Bucci, W. (1997). Psychoanalysis & cognitive science: A
multiple code theory. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Burt, J. R., & Mueller, R. A. (1993). Psychoanalytic theory and
loving God concepts: Parent referencing versus self-referencing. The
Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 127, 17-27.
Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining empirically
supported therapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
66,7-18.
Chambless, D. L., & 011endick, T. H. (2001). Empirically
supported psychological interventions: Controversies and evidence.
Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 685-716.
Cheston, S. E., Piedmont, R. L., Eanes, B., & Patrice, L.
(2003). Changes in clients' images of God over the course of
outpatient psychotherapy. Counseling and Values, 47, 96-108.
Collins, N. L., Guichard, A. C., Ford, M. B., & Feeney, B. C.
(2004). Working models of attachment: New developments and emerging
themes. In W S. RhoJes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult attachment:
Theory, research, and clinical implications (pp. 196-239). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Cozolino, L. (2010). The neuroscience of psychotherapy: Healing the
social brain (2nd ed.), New York, NY: Norton.
Davis, E. B. (2010). Authenticity, inauthenticity, attachment, and
God-image tendencies among adult evangelical Protestant Christians
(Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from Pro-Quest Dissertations &
Theses. (AAT 3400639). Retrieved from
hup://eaps.net/communities/christian-psychoanalysts/cp-continued-learning.
Davis, E. B., & Moriarty, G. L. (2008, August). Depression and
God images. In W. H. Hathaway (Chair), Appropriate religious/spiritual
treatment goals: Converging trends and professional roles. Symposium
conducted at the 116th Annual Convention of the American Psychological
Association. Boston, MA.
Derogatis, L. (1978). The Derogatis Psychiatric Rating Scale.
Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Derogatis, L. (1993). Brief Symptom Inventory: Administration,
scoring, and procedures manual (3rd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: National
Computer Systems.
DeRubeis, R. J., Tang, T. Z., & Beck, A. T., (2001). Cognitive
Therapy. In K. S. Dobson (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive-behavioral
therapies (2nd ed., pp. 349-392). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Edwards, K. J. (1976). Sex role behavior and religious experience.
In W. Donaldson, Jr. (Ed.), Research in mental health and religious
behavior. Atlanta, GA: Psychological Studies Institute, Fitts, W. H.
(1965). Manual for Tennessee (Department of Mental Health) Self Concept
Scale. Nashville, TN: Counseling Recordings and Tests.
Fogel, A. (2009). The psychophysiology of self-awareness:
Rediscovering the lost art of body sense. New York, NY: Norton.
Freud, S. (1950). Totem and taboo 0. Strachey, Trans,), New York,
NY: Norton (Original work published 1913).
Freud, S. (1962). Civilization and its discontents (J. Strachey,
Trans.). New York, NY: Norton (Original work published 1930).
Gibson, N. J. S. (2007). Measurement issues in God image research
and practice. In G. L. Moriarty & L. Hoffman (Eds.), Cod image
handbook for spiritual counseling and psychotherapy: Research, theory,
and practice (pp. 227-246). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Gough, H. G., & Heilbrum, A. B. (1983). The adjective check
list manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Grimes, C. (2007). God image research: A literature review. In G.
Moriarty & L. Hoffman (Eds.), God image handbook for spiritual
counseling and psychotherapy: Research, theory, and practice ( pp.
11-32). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Hall, T. W. (2004). Christian spirituality and mental health: A
relational spirituality framework for empirical research. Journal of
Psychology and Christianity, 23, 66-81.
Hall, T. W (2007). Psychoanalysis, attachment, and spirituality
part I: The emergence of two relational traditions. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 35, 14-28.
Hall, T. W., Fujikawa, A., Halcrow, S. R., Hill, P. C., &
Delaney, H. (2009). Attachment to God and implicit spirituality:
Clarifying correspondence and compensation models. Journal of Psychology
and Theology, 37, 227-242.
Hathaway, W. L. (2003). Clinically significant religious
impairment. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 6, 113-129. Hays, P.
A. (2007). Addressing cultural complexities in practice (2nd ed.).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Hoffman, L. (2005). A development perspective on the God image. In
R. H. Cox, B. Ervin-Cox, & L. Hoffman (Eds.). Spirituality and
psychological health (pp.129-147). Colorado Springs, CO: Colorado School
of Professional Psychology Press.
Jones, J. (1991). Contemporary psychoanalysis and religion:
Transference and transcendence. Binghamton, NY: Vail-Ballou Press.
Jones, J. (2007). Psychodynamic theories of the evolution of the
God image. In G. Moriarty & L. Hoffman (Eds.), God image handbook
for spiritual counseling and psychotherapy: Research, theory, and
practice (pp. 33-55). Binghamton, NY: Haworth/Routledge Press.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2008). The psychological unconscious. In O. P.
John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality:
Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 583-602). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Lawrence, R. T. (1997). Measuring the image of God: The God Image
Inventory and the God Image Scales. Journal of Psychology and Theology,
25, 214-226.
Lewis, C. S. (2001). The chronicles of Narnia. New York, NY:
HarperCollins Publishers (Original works published 1950-1956).
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bruschweiler-Stern, N., Harrison, A. M., Morgan, A.
C., Nahum, J. P., Sander, L., ... Tronick, E. Z. (1998). Implicit
relational knowing: Its role in development and psychoanalytic
treatment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 282-289.
Mahler, M., Pine, E, & Bergman, A. (1975). Psychological birth
of the human infant: Symbiosis and individuation. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about
the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 63-78.
Markus, H., & Kunda, Z. (1986). Stability and malleability of
the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
51,858-866.
Moriarty, G. (2006). Pastoral care of depression: Helping clients
heal their relationship with God. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Moriarty, G. L., & Davis, E. B. (in press). Client God images:
Theory, research, and clinical practice. In K. O'Grady, E.
Worthington, Jr., & J. Aten (Eds.), The psychology of religion and
spirituality for clinicians: Using research in your practice. New York,
NY: Routledge.
Nathan, P. E., & Gorman, J. M. (2007). Preface. In P. E. Nathan
& J. M. Gorman (Eds.), A guide to treatments that work (3rd ed., pp.
vii-ix). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Noffke, J. L., & Hall, T. W. (2007).Attachment psychotherapy
and God image.In G. L. Moriarty & L. Hoffman (Eds.), God image
handbook for spiritual counseling and psychotherapy: Research, theory,
and practice (pp. 57-78). Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Norcross, J. C. & Goldfried, M. R. (Eds.). (2005). Handbook of
psychotherapy integration. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
O'Hare, C. A. (2003). Challenging god images: Implementing a
Christian component within a standard group therapy intervention
(Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations &
Theses. (AAT 3062350),
Richards, P. S., & Bergin, A. E. (2000). Handbook of
psychotherapy and religious diversity. Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Rizzuto, A. (1974). Object relations and the formation of the image
of God. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 47, 83-99.
Rizzuto, A. (1979). The birth of the living God. Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Schafer, R. (1968). Aspects of internalization. New York, NY:
International Universities Press.
Siegel, D. J. (2010). The mindful therapist: A clinician's
guide to mindsight and neural integration. New York, NY: Norton.
Smith, E. E., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2007). Cognitive psychology:
Mind and brain. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Thomas, M. J. (2009). The effect of a manualized group treatment
protocol on God image and attachment to God (Doctoral dissertation).
Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses. (AAT 3357122).
Tisdale, T. T., Key, T. L, Edwards, K. J., Brokaw, B. F.,
Kemper-man, S. R., Cloud, H., Okamoto, T. (1997). Impact of treatment on
God image and personal adjustment, and correlations of God image to
personal adjustment and object relations development. Journal of
Psychology and Theology, 25, 227-239.
Turner, R. P., Lukoff, D., Barnhouse, R. T., & Lu, F. G.
(1995). Religious or spiritual problem: A culturally sensitive
diagnostic category in the DSM-IV. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 183, 435-444,
Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing and reality. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Winnicott, D. W. (1975). Transitional objects and transitional
phenomena. In D. W. Winnicott (Ed.), Through paediatrics to
psycho-analysis (pp. 229-242). New York, NY: Basic Books (Original work
published 1951).
Yarborough, C. A. (2009). Depression and the emotional experience
of God {Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations
& Theses. (AAT 3392180).
Yalom, I. D., & Leszcz, M. (2005). The theory and practice of
group psychotherapy (5th cd.). New York, NY: Basic Books.
AUTHORS
THOMAS, MICHAEL. J. Address: 1300 Eagle Road, McInnis 222 St.
Davids, PA 19087. Title: Visiting Assistant Professor, Eastern
University. Michael also serves as a psychologist within the Brandywine
Hospital Eating Disorder Program in Coatesville, Pennsylvania. Degree:
Psy.D. Areas of Specialization: Clinical and research interests include
eating disorders, god images, religious and spiritual issues, and
positive psychology.
MORIARTY, GLENDON, L. Address: 1000 Regent University Drive,
Virginia Beach, VA 23456. Email Address: glenmor@regent.edu. Phone:
757.352.4341. Title: Associate Professor, Department of Psychology,
Regent University. Degree: Psy.D. Areas of Specialization: Psychodynamic
psychotherapy, emotional experience of God/God images, integration of
psychology and technology.
DAVIS, EDWARD. B. Address: 13800 Biola Avenue, Rose Hall 158 La
Mirada, CA 90639. Email: edward.davis@biola.edu. Title: Assistant
Professor, Rosemead School of Psychology (Biola University). Degree:
Psy.D. Areas of Specialization: His academic, clinical, and research
interests include personality psychology, psychological assessment,
interpersonal neurobiology, psychology of religion/spirituality, god
images, attachment, narrative identity, mindful awareness practices, and
supervision/mentoring.
ANDERSON, ELIZABETH, L. Address: LSU Student Health Center Mental
Health Services, Room 252 Baton Rouge, LA 70803. Email Address:
elizan2@regent.edu Title: Doctoral Psychology Intern, Southern Louisiana
Internship Consortium (SLIC). Psy.D. Doctoral Candidate in Clinical
Psychology, Regent University. Degree. M.A. Areas of specialization:
integration of psychology and spirituality; emotional experience of
God/God images; psychodynamic psychotherapy; positive psychology; and
interpersonal, Yalom style, dynamic group psychotherapy.
Portions of this article are reprinted from the primary
author's dissertation (1-The Effect of a Manualized Group Treatment
Protocol on God Image and Attachment to God,' Thomas, 2009) and
from one secondary author's dissertation "Authenticity.
[nailthenticity, Attachment, and God-Image Tendencies Among Adult
Evangelical Protestant Christians," Davis, 2010). The authors wish
to express thanks to Abrielle Conway, Seth Rainwater, Sherley
Saget-Menager, and Stephanie Nowacki-Butzen, for their group
cofacilitation, and to Nicholas J. S. Gibson, Mark Blagen, Stephanie
Nowacki-Butzen, for their instrumentation consultation. Correspondence
concerning this article should be addressed to Michael j. Thomas,
Psychology Department, Eastern University, 1300 Eagle Road, McInnis
Learning Center 222, St. Davids, PA 19087. E-mail: mthomall@eastern.edu.
Michael J. Thomas, Glendon L. Moriarty, Edward B. Davis, and
Elizabeth L. Anderson
Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology
Regent University