Improving the life chances of disadvantaged children.
Ludwig, Jens
Improving the schooling outcomes for disadvantaged children is
central to efforts to reduce overall inequality and for increasing
economic growth. Around 78 percent of white high school students
graduate within four years, compared to 58 percent of Hispanics and 55
percent of blacks. (1) In the federal government's 2007 National
Assessment of Educational Progress, only 16 percent of fourth-grade
students who were eligible for free lunch scored at proficient levels in
reading, compared with 44 percent of those with higher family incomes.
(2) These large disparities understandably have intensified concern
about how to improve our system of public schools.
The possibility that some of the most effective ways to improve
school outcomes might not have anything to do with elementary or
secondary schools first was raised in a landmark 1966 study named after
its lead investigator, the distinguished sociologist James S. Coleman.
(3) The "Coleman Report" made several remarkable claims,
including: the black-white gap in school "inputs" was much
smaller than generally perceived; school inputs were only weakly
correlated with student test scores; among the strongest correlates of
test scores were family background and the socio-economic composition of
the child's school; and, disparities in test scores open up very
early in life, so that for example the black-white test score gap was
already 1.5 standard deviations by first grade. Subsequent studies have
shown that these disparities are evident in the pre-school years, in
part because of disparities in early learning environments. By age
three, children in professional families have larger vocabularies than
the parents of children in families on welfare. (4)
My research and that of other NBER family members suggests that
segregation, poverty, and other aspects of the out-of-school
environment, particularly early in life, indeed seem to matter for
children, but apparently more so for behavioral outcomes like schooling
attainment and criminal behavior than for achievement test scores.
Social Context
Since at least the 1920s, social scientists have thought that child
development may be heavily influenced by the child's social
context, including the interactions with peers that shape the returns to
different behaviors, the information that local adult role models convey
about the value of schooling and formal labor market involvement, and
the quality of local institutions such as schools and police. These
beliefs are consistent with the substantial cross-sectional variation
observed in children's learning and other outcomes across schools
and neighborhoods of differing socio-economic and racial compositions.
Yet in practice, isolating the causal effects of social context on
children's life chances has been quite difficult because of the
endogenous sorting of families across schools and neighborhoods.
To identify and estimate the causal effects of neighborhoods on
children and families, the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban
Development (HUD) sponsored the Moving to Opportunity (MTO) residential
mobility experiment. Started in 1994 in five cities (Baltimore, Boston,
Chicago, Los Angeles, and New York), MTO enrolled a sample of 4600
public housing families with children and via random lottery offered
some families the chance to use a housing voucher to move into a less
distressed neighborhood. Random assignment in MTO generated very large
changes in neighborhood conditions among otherwise comparable groups of
families. For example, families with MTO vouchers moved into census
tracts with average poverty rates of just 12 percent in the year 2000,
much lower than the average baseline tract's poverty rate of 50
percent.
Data collected on MTO families about five years after a baseline
revealed no detectable differences in average achievement test scores
across randomly assigned MTO mobility groups. However, my study with
Jeffrey Kling and Lawrence Katz shows that arrest rates for violent
crime among youth who relocated through MTO were around 40 percent lower
than those for youth in the control group. (5) MTO also reduced arrest
rates for other types of crimes among young females, but it seems to
have increased property-crime arrests for young males. Other studies
using data from randomized public-school choice lotteries also have
found that moving to a higher-quality or less segregated school has more
pronounced effects on behavioral outcomes, like crime, than on
achievement test scores. However, the school choice studies do not find
signs of adverse effects on property offending or other criminal
behaviors of male youth. (6)
These various studies of randomized housing-voucher or
school-choice lotteries identify partial-equilibrium effects by focusing
on those who move to a new social context. To learn more about the
general-equilibrium effects on crime from large-scale government efforts
to re-sort people across social contexts, David Weiner, Byron Lutz, and
I study the largest and arguably most important policy initiative in
this area: court-ordered school desegregation, which has been of
increasing interest to economists in recent years. (7) Most of the
nation's largest urban districts were forced to desegregate by
local federal court order; differences across these districts in the
timing of the court orders provide our source of identifying variation.
Our analysis suggests that re-sorting children across social settings is
not just a zero-sum game. Court-ordered school desegregation seems to
generate substantial declines in homicide victimization and offending
among black youth and, interestingly, seems to generate beneficial
spillovers to other groups as well (such as whites and black adults), at
least in the short term.
Early Childhood Education
Early disparities in children's outcomes and the possibility
that certain learning can take place only at specific times in a
child's development have generated considerable interest in early
childhood interventions. Because getting parents to behave in more
developmentally productive ways seems to be quite difficult in practice,
most of the policy attention has been devoted to center-based early
childhood education (ECE) programs. Intensive, small-scale model
programs from the 1960s and 1970s--such as Perry Preschool and Carolina
Abecedarian--have been shown to improve important adult economic and
other outcomes, despite some "fade out" in test score gains.
While these programs seem to generate benefits far in excess of their
costs, (8) there remains the important policy question of whether these
small-scale model programs can be taken to scale effectively.
Head Start is the main example of such a scaled-up program, and has
consistently generated debate about whether it produces lasting benefits
to program participants. Head Start was launched in 1965 by the Office
of Economic Opportunity (OEO) and provides low-income children aged 3-5
years, and their parents, with schooling, health, nutrition, and social
welfare services. The first study arguing that Head Start benefits to
children fade out rapidly was released in 1966, which meant there was a
very short honeymoon period. The main concern with that early study, and
many subsequent ones, is the possibility that relatively more
disadvantaged families may select (or be selected) into program
participation, so that naive regressions that simply compare
participants and non-participants may understate the benefits of the
program.
My work on Head Start with Douglas Miller tries to identify its
causal effects on children's life outcomes by taking advantage of a
discontinuity in program funding across counties that resulted from the
way the OEO initially implemented the program. (9) During the spring of
1965, OEO provided technical assistance to the 300 poorest counties in
the United States to develop Head Start funding proposals. We show that
program funding and participation rates are 50-100 percent higher in
counties with poverty rates just above OEO's cutoff (the
"treatment" group) than in those just below (the control
group). This funding difference, which is the key to our regression
discontinuity (RD) research design, appears to have persisted through
the late 1970s. The estimated discontinuity in other federal social
spending is small and not significant.
Our main finding is that this large "jump" in Head Start
funding at the OEO threshold is mirrored in a large "drop" in
mortality rates to children 5 to 9 years of age over the period 1973-83
from causes addressed as part of Head Start's health services. Our
estimates imply that a 50-100 percent increase in Head Start funding
reduces mortality rates from relevant causes by 33-50 percent of the
control mean, enough to drive mortality rates from these causes in the
treatment counties down to about the national average. There do not
appear to be drops for other causes-of-death or birth cohorts that
should not be affected by Head Start. We also find suggestive evidence
of a "jump" at the OEO threshold in educational attainment,
but no statistically significant discontinuities in achievement test
scores measured during middle school. (10)
Implications for Policy and Next Steps
The growing body of research about the beneficial effects on
disadvantaged minority children from reducing segregation of schools and
neighborhoods is relevant to ongoing policy and legal debates about
government efforts in this area. While there would be great value in
learning more about the general-equilibrium effects of large scale
resorting policies, the evidence we have to date suggests that helping
poor families move out of high-poverty high-rise public housing projects
may help to improve at least certain aspects of child well-being.
What else policy might do to reduce the segregation of low-income
minority children in schools or neighborhoods is not clear. While many
public housing families appear eager to move to less-distressed areas
when given the chance, some of my ongoing work with Brian Jacob suggests
that other low-income families who are already in the private housing
market are reluctant to move out of their old neighborhoods, even when
provided with large rental subsidies. Re-sorting children across schools
without changing residential patterns is difficult given how segregated
our cities are, and given past U.S. Supreme Court decisions that make it
extremely difficult to re-sort children across school district
boundaries. Consider, for example, that in the Chicago Public School
system, just 9 percent of students are white, and fully 86 percent of
students are eligible for free or reduced price lunches.
Whether local, state, or federal governments will increase
investments in early childhood education despite their current budget
difficulties remains to be seen. At least as important for public policy
is the question of whether Head Start is as beneficial for today's
poor children as it was in the past. In principle, the net effects of
Head Start may have changed over time, as the developmental quality of
the program and its alternatives have changed substantially.
The federal government recently sponsored a randomized experimental
study of Head Start that found impacts on test scores measured at the
end of the program year on the order of 0.1 to 0.2 standard deviations.
These results led to considerable criticism of Head Start for not doing
more to eliminate the test score gap between minority and white children
or between rich and poor. But Deborah Phillips and I note that these
initial impacts are about the same as what was found for previous
cohorts of children, for whom we observed lasting benefits into
adulthood. (11) More puzzling are the latest results from the
experiment's first-grade follow-up, which showed almost complete
"fade out" of these initial gains--a more rapid decline in
Head Start effects than what was observed for previous cohorts of
program participants.
The recent Head Start experiment highlights the great value for
social policy in learning more about the mapping between short- and
long-term ECE impacts. Ideally, we would be able to use short-term
effects from ECE studies in a manner analogous to what medical
researchers call "surrogate clinical endpoints" (for example,
using changes in blood cholesterol levels to understand effects on
long-term risk for cardiovascular disease). It would certainly be less
than ideal to have to wait 30 or 40 years to understand the long-term
effects of today's early childhood interventions.
(1) C.B. Swanson, Cities in Crisis, 2009--Closing the Graduation
Gap: Educational and Economic Conditions in America's Largest
Cities, Bethesda, MD: Editorial Projects in Education, 2009.
(2) The Nation's Report Card, Reading 2007: National
Assessment of Educational Progress at Grades 4 and 8, National Center
for Education Statistics 2007-496, Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Education, Institute for Educational Sciences, 2007.
(3) J.S. Coleman, E. Q. Campbell, C.J. Hobson, et al, Equality of
Educational Opportunity, Washington, DC: Office of Education, U.S.
Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1966.
(4) B. Hart and T. Risley, Meaningful Differences in the Everyday
Experience of Young American Children, Baltimore, MD: Paul Brooks, 1995.
(5) See L. Sanbonmatsu, J.R. Kling, G.J. Duncan, and J.
Brooks-Gunn, "Neighborhoods and Academic Achievement: Results from
the Moving to Opportunity Experiment, NBER Working Paper No. 11909,
January 2006, and Journal of Human Resources, XLI, (2006), pp. 649-91;
J.R. Kling, J. Ludwig, and L.F. Katz, "Neighborhood Effects on
Crime for Female and Male Youth: Evidence from a Randomized Housing
Voucher Experiment", NBER Working Paper No. 10777, September 2004,
and Quarterly Journal of Economics, 1200), (2005), pp. 87-130.
(6) See J.B. Cullen, B.A. Jacob, and S. Levitt, "The Effect of
School Choice on Student Outcomes: Evidence from Randomized
Lotteries", NBER Working Paper No. 10113, November 2003, and
Econometrica 74(5) (2006), pp.1191-1230; and D. Deming, "Better
Schools, Less Crime?" working paper, Carnegie-Mellon University,
2009.
(7) D.A. Weiner, B.F. Lutz, and J. Ludwig, "The Effects of
School Desegregation on Crime", NBER Working Paper No. 15380,
September 2009. See also, for example, J. Guryan, "Desegregation and Black Dropout Rates," NBER Working Paper No. 8345, June 2001,
and American Economic Review, 94(4) (2004), pp. 919-43.
(8) See, for example, C.R. Belfield, M. Nores, W.S. Barnett, and L.
Schweinhart, "The High/Scope Perry Preschool Program: Cost-Benefit
Analysis Using Data from the Age40 Follow-up," Journal of Human
Resources, XLIO), (2006), pp. 162-90; and W.S. Barnett and L.N. Masse,
"Comparative Benefit-Cost Analysis of the Abecedarian Program and
its Policy Implications", Economics of Education Review, 26,
(2007),pp. 113-25.
(9) J. Ludwig and D.L. Miller, "Does Head Start Improve
Children's Life Chances? Evidence from a Regression Discontinuity
Design", NBER Working Paper No. 11702, October 2005, and Quarterly
Journal of Economics, 122(1) (2007), pp. 159-208.
(10) See also J. Currie and D. Thomas, "Does Head Start Make a
Difference?", NBER Working Paper No. 4406, July 1003, and American
Economic Review, 85(3) (1995), pp. 341-64; E. Garces, D. Thomas, and J.
Currie, "Longer Term Effects of Head Start," NBER Working
Paper No. 8054, December 2000, and American Economic Review, 92(4)
(2002), pp. 999-1012; and D. Deming, "Early Childhood Intervention and Life-Cycle Skill Development: Evidence from Head Start,"
American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 1(3) (2009), pp. 111-34.
(11) J. Ludwig and D.A. Phillips, "The Benefits and Costs of
Head Start", NBER Working Paper No.12973, March 2007, and Society
for Research in Child Development Social Policy Report, 21(3) (2007),
pp. 3-18.
Jens Ludwig *
* Ludwig is a Research Associate in the NBER's Program on
Children and a professor at the University of Chicago. His Profile
appears later in this issue.