Happiness economics.
Blanchflower, David G.
Despite Thomas Carlyle's claim, when he was arguing that
slavery was morally superior to the market, economics is no longer the
dismal science. (1) A growing body of literature in the economics of
happiness and mental well-being has emerged. It is now fashionable to
try to understand the pursuit of happiness, and, after a long delay, the
ideas promoted originally by Richard Easterlin are attracting worldwide
attention. (2) There is even a World Database of Happiness.
Anyway, I came to the topics of happiness and well-being as a labor
economist who had mostly worked on wages, and who early on was struck by
the stability of the Mincerian earnings function across time and space.
The basic structure of a log earnings equation, no matter what dataset
was used and what country it is estimated for, has a similar structure.
It turns out that there are patterns in the well-being data. I am struck
by the fact that there is a great deal of stability in happiness and
life-satisfaction equations, no matter what country we look at, what
dataset or time period, whether the question relates to life
satisfaction or happiness, and how the responses are coded whether in
three, four, five or even as many as ten categories.
In general, economists have focused on modeling three fairly simple
questions on life satisfaction and happiness, and that is what I have
done mostly in my research, primarily with Andrew Oswald at the
University of Warwick, but also with David Bell at the University of
Stirling; Chris Shadforth at the Bank of England, and Richard Freeman
from Harvard.
Typical questions are: 1) Happiness--for example from the General
Social Survey, Taken all together, how would you say things are these
days--would you say that you are very happy, pretty happy, or not too
happy? 2) Life satisfaction--for example from the European Eurobarometer
Surveys, On the whole, are you very satisfied, fairly satisfied, not
very satisfied, or not at all satisfied with the life you lead? 3)
Psychological health and mental strain--for example from the British
Household Panel Survey, Such as the GHQ score, which amalgamates answers
to questions about how well people have been sleeping, their level of
confidence, feelings of depression, among others. (3)
The micro data on happiness are easily obtainable from most data
archives including ICPSR for the GSS and the Eurobarometers, the Data
Archive at the University of Essex and ZACAT in Germany for the
Eurobarometers, ISSP, European Social Survey, BHPS, GSOEP, European
Quality of Life Survey, European Social Surveys and so on. Life
satisfaction data are also now available annually from the
Latinobarometers, while happiness data are also available annually in
the Asianbarometers. Several of the data series extend back at least to
the early 1970s. Some are panels (BHPS, GSOEP).
Economists have had longstanding reservations about the reliability
of interpersonal comparisons of well-being. Psychologists, however, view
it as natural that a concept such as happiness should be studied in part
by asking people how they feel. One definition of happiness is the
degree to which an individual judges the overall quality of his or her
life as favorable. As a validation of the answers to recorded happiness
levels, it turns out that answers to happiness and life satisfaction
questions are correlated with: 1) objective characteristics such as
unemployment; 2) assessments of the person's happiness by friends
and family members; 3) assessments of the person's happiness by his
or her spouse; 4) heart rate and blood-pressure measures of response to
stress; 5) the risk of coronary heart disease; 6) duration of authentic
or so-called Duchenne smiles (a Duchenne smile occurs when both the
zygomatic major and obicularus orus facial muscles fire, and human
beings identify these as "genuine" smiles); 7) skin-resistance
measures of response to stress; 8) electroencephelogram measures of
prefrontal brain activity.
It is apparent that most people are happy (or, more precisely, mark
themselves fairly high up on a scale). This finding has subsequently
been replicated in many datasets over many time periods and for numerous
countries. For example, in the United States in 2006 only 13 percent of
people in the GSS said they were not very happy, 56 percent were pretty
happy, and 31 percent very happy. In Eurobarometer 67.2 for April-May
2007 (ICPSR#21160) for the European Union (EU) 15 in 2007, for example,
3 percent said they were not at all satisfied, while 12 percent were not
very satisfied, 60 percent fairly satisfied, and 24 percent very
satisfied. In the 2005 Latinobarometers, which also asked the same
4-step life satisfaction question in eighteen Latin American countries,
4.6 percent said they were not at all satisfied, 25.4 percent were not
very satisfied, 39.7 percent were fairly satisfied, and 30.3 percent
very satisfied.
There are a number of common patterns in the determinants of
happiness, which have been replicated in a number of other papers.
Happiness and life satisfaction tends to be higher among a) women, b)
people with lots of friends, c) the young and the old, d) married and
cohabiting people, e) the highly educated, f) the healthy, g) those with
high income, h) the self-employed, i) people with low blood pressure, j)
those who have sex at least once a week with the same partner, k)
right-wing voters, l) the religious, m) members of non-church
organizations, n) volunteers, o) those who take exercise, and p) those
who live in western countries. (4) The self-employed especially value
their independence. (5) It turns out that the main findings from
responses on both happiness and life satisfaction are also broadly
replicated in data on unhappiness, hypertension, stress, depression,
anxiety, and pain from a considerable number of cross-country data
sources. (6) Happy people are less likely to commit suicide. (7)
There is also evidence of adaptation. Good and bad life events wear
off, at least partially, as people get used to them. Oswald and
Powdthavee provide longitudinal evidence that people who become disabled
go on to exhibit considerable recovery in mental well-being. (8) In
fixed-effects equations they estimate the degree of hedonic adaptation
at--depending on the severity of the disability--approximately 30
percent to 50 percent.
I recently compared the results using data on life satisfaction and
happiness with those from evaluated time use (9) and in particular the
U-index (for "unpleasant" or "undesirable") as
propounded by Krueger et al for use in National Time Accounting. (10)
The U-index is designed to measure the proportion of time an individual
spends in an unpleasant state. Encouragingly, there are many
similarities in the findings. For example, both the U-index and
conventional measures show higher levels of well-being among wealthier,
higher educated and older individuals. One attraction of the evaluated
time use data, though, is that it can be used to understand why some
groups are happier than others.
In the United States happiness has trended downward over time, but
the picture is rather more mixed in Europe: using the Eurobarometer life
satisfaction questions, when an ordered logit is estimated with controls
for education, age, gender, schooling, marital status, and labor market
status, nine countries have positive time trends (Denmark; Finland;
France; Italy: Luxembourg; Netherlands; Spain; Sweden; and the United
Kingdom.) Austria and Ireland have no significant time trend while
Belgium, Germany, Greece, and Portugal have significant downward trends.
There is evidence from developing countries of a steeper upward trend in
happiness. This is especially apparent in Latin America and among the
eight Eastern European countries that joined the European Union in 2004.
(11)
There is evidence though of a downward trend in happiness for women
and an upward trend for men in the United States (12). Betsey Stevenson
and Justin Wolfers confirmed this finding for the United States and also
found that women's declining relative well-being is found in
multiple countries, datasets, and measures of subjective well-being, and
is pervasive across demographic groups. (13) Relative declines in female
happiness have eroded a gender gap in happiness in which women in the
1970s typically reported higher subjective well-being than did men.
These declines have continued and a new gender gap may be emerging, one
with higher subjective well-being for men.
A small literature, begun by Rafael Di Tella and his co-authors,
has found that both unemployment and inflation lower happiness. (14)
This is true even after controlling for country fixed effects and year
effects. My paper extends the literature by looking at more countries
over a longer time period. It also considers the impacts on happiness of
GDP per capita and interest rates. (15) I find, conventionally, that
both higher unemployment and higher inflation lower happiness. Interest
rates are also found to enter happiness equations negatively. Changes in
GDP per capita have little impact on more economically developed
countries, but do have a positive impact in the poorest
countries--consistent with the Easterlin hypothesis. I find that
unemployment depresses well-being more than inflation. The least
educated and the old are more concerned about unemployment than
inflation. Conversely, the young and the most educated are more
concerned about inflation. An individual's experience of high
inflation over their adult lifetime lowers their current happiness over
and above the effects from current inflation and current unemployment.
Unemployment appears to be more costly than inflation in terms of its
impact on wellbeing.
Oswald and I found that the well-being of the young had risen both
in the United States and Europe. (16) Explaining why was difficult. This
is still the case: in the GSS the time trend is positive for those aged
under 30 and flat for those aged 30 and higher. In Europe using pooled
Eurobarometers there is a significant upward time trend for both but
much stronger for the young. We ruled out that it was explained by a
decline in the chance of war with the Eastern bloc, falling
discrimination, changing education and work, or the rise in
youth-oriented consumer goods. The paper demonstrated that most of the
increase is to be found in the group who were unmarried.
In a 2004 paper, Oswald and I estimated the dollar values of events
like unemployment and divorce. (17) They are large. For the typical
individual, a doubling of salary makes a lot less difference than life
events like marriage or unemployment. A lasting marriage (compared to
widowhood as a "natural" experiment), for example, is
estimated to be worth $100,000 a year. Further calculation suggests that
to "compensate" men exactly for unemployment would take a rise
in income of $60,000 per annum, and to "compensate" for being
black would take $30,000 extra per annum. Oswald and Powdthavee find
that using GHQ mental distress as the measure of well-being, the hedonic
compensation annual amount in the first year for the death of a child
might be of the order of 100,000 [pounds sterling] ($200,000). (18)
These are all large sums, and in a sense reflect the low (happiness)
value of extra income. Money, it seems, does not buy more sexual
partners or more sex. (19)
The data show that richer people are happier and healthier, but can
we be sure about causality? As in other areas of economics, this is both
of central importance and difficult to establish beyond all doubt. One
attempt, by Jonathan Gardner and Oswald, found that Britons who receive
lottery wins of between 1,000 [pounds sterling] and 120,000 [pounds
sterling] go on, compared to those who receive tiny wins, to exhibit
better psychological health. (20) But Oswald and I found that
individuals in the United States were found to be less happy if their
incomes are far above those of the poorest people. However, people do
appear to compare themselves more with well-off families, so that
perhaps they get happier the closer their income comes to that of rich
people around them. Relative income certainly appears to matter. Erzo
Luttmer finds, for the United States, that higher earnings of neighbors
are associated with lower levels of self-reported happiness, controlling
for an individual's own income. (21) Alberto Alesina and his
co-authors find, using a sample of individuals across the United States
(1981-96) and Europe (1975-92) that individuals have a lower tendency to
report themselves as happy when inequality is high, even controlling for
individual income. The effect is stronger in Europe than in the United
States. (22)
Life satisfaction scores predict reasonably well the scale of the
flow of workers coming to the United Kingdom from the eight Accession
countries of the Czech Republic, Estonia, Hungary, Latvia, Lithuania,
Poland, Slovakia, and Slovenia. These countries joined the European
Union in 2004 and only the United Kingdom and Ireland gave them the
right to work. In these countries the levels of happiness are low. (23)
The propensity to migrate is more highly correlated with life
satisfaction scores than it is with GDP per capita, the unemployment
rate, or the employment rate. (24)
Consistent with other research, Oswald and I recently documented,
using evidence on over two million people, that psychological well-being
is U-shaped through life. (25) A difficulty with research on this issue
is that there are likely to be omitted cohort effects (earlier
generations may have been born in, say, particularly good or bad times).
First, using data on 500,000 randomly sampled Americans and West
Europeans, the paper designs a test that can control for cohort effects.
Holding other factors constant, we show that a typical individual's
happiness reaches its minimum on both sides of the Atlantic and for both
males and females in middle age. Second, evidence is provided for the
existence of a similar U-shape through the life-course in East European,
Latin American, and Asian nations. Third, a U-shape in age is found in
separate well-being regression equations in 72 developed and developing
nations. Fourth, using measures that are closer to psychiatric scores,
we document a comparable well-being curve across the life cycle in two
other datasets: 1) in GHQ-N6 mental health levels among a sample of
16,000 Europeans, and 2) in reported depression-and-anxiety levels among
one million UK citizens. Fifth, we discuss some apparent exceptions,
particularly in developing nations, to the U-shape. Sixth, we note that
American male birth-cohorts seem to have become progressively less
content with their lives. More, truly longitudinal research on this
topic would be valuable.
In surveys of well-being, countries such as Denmark and the
Netherlands emerge as particularly happy while nations like Germany and
Italy report lower levels of happiness. But are these kinds of findings
credible? Oswald and I provide some evidence suggesting that the answer
is yes. (26) Using data on 16 countries, we show that happier nations
report systematically lower levels of hypertension. As well as
potentially validating the differences in measured happiness across
nations, this suggests that blood-pressure readings might be valuable as
part of a national well-being index.
In a pair of articles Oswald and I studied well-being in Australia
along with a published response by Andrew Leigh and Justin Wolfers. (27)
According to the HDI, Australia now ranks third in the world. That
places the country above all the other English-speaking nations. This
article raises questions about that assessment. It reviews the new work
on happiness economics, considers implications for policymakers, and
examines where Australia lies in international subjective well-being
rankings. Using new ISSP data on approximately 50,000 randomly sampled
individuals from 35 nations, the article shows that Australia lies close
to the bottom of an international ranking of job satisfaction levels.
Among a sub-sample of English-speaking nations, where a common language
should help subjective well-being measures to be reliable, Australia
performs fairly poorly on a range of happiness indicators. Moreover,
Australia has the highest overall suicide rate. This appears to be a
paradox.
The literature on the economics of well-being is currently growing
at a remarkable rate. If one takes the view that human happiness is
ultimately the most important topic in social science, perhaps--after
decades where economists lagged behind other social scientists--this
should not be surprising. However, we still have a great deal to learn.
Is the Easterlin Paradox--that happiness at a national level does not
increase with wealth once basic needs are fulfilled--correct or only an
approximation to the truth? Are relative-wage effect comparisons always
harmful to people? How should we think about the connections between
mental health and what economists have traditionally called utility?
What are the deep links between money and happiness ? The next decade is
likely to see a great deal of work on these important topics.
(1) "... I should say, like some we have heard of, no, a
dreary, desolate, and indeed, quite abject and distressing one, what we
might call, by way of eminence, the dismal science" Thomas Carlyle,
1849.
(2) R.A. Easterlin, "Does Economic Growth Improve The Human
Lot? Some Empirical Evidence" in Nations and Households in Economic
Growth: Essays in Honor of Moses Abramowitz, P. A. David and M. W.
Reder, eds. New York: Academic Press (1974), and R.A. Easterlin,
"Will Raising the Incomes Of All Increase the Happiness of
All?" Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27 (June
1995), pp.35-48.
(3) For economists who want a readable way into the literature,
introductions to it can be found in sources such as A.J. Oswald,
"Happiness And Economic Performance, Economic Journal, 107 November
1997, pp. 1815-31; B.S. Frey and A. Stutzer, Happiness and Economics,
Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press, (2002); and A.E.
Clark, P Frijters, and M.A. Shields, "Relative Income, Happiness,
and Utility: An Explanation for the Easterlin Paradox And Other
Puzzles", Journal of Economic Literature, 46(1), (2008), pp.
95-144.
(4) D.G. Blanch flower and A.J. Oswald, "Well-Being Over Time
in Britain and the United States, Journal of Public Economics, Volume
88, Issues 7-8, July 2004, pp. 1359-86; and P. Dolan, T. Peasgood, and
M. White, "Do We Really Know What Makes Us Happy? A Review of the
Economic Literature on the Factors Associated with Subjective
Well-Being", Journal of Economic Psychology, 29, (2008), pp.
94-122.
(5) D.G. Blanch flower and A.J. Oswald, "What Makes An
Entrepreneur?" Journal of Labor Economics, January 1998, 16(1),pp.
26-60; D.G. Blanch flower, "Self-Employment: More May Not Be
Better", Swedish Economic Policy Review, 11 (2), Fall 2004, pp.
15-74; and D.G. Blanchflower and C. Shadforth, "Entrepreneurship in
the UK", Foundations and Trends in Entrepreneurship, 3(4), (2007),
pp. 1-108.
(6) D.N.F. Bell and D.G. Blanchflower, "The Scots May Be Brave
But They Are Neither Healthy Nor Happy", Scottish Journal of
Political Economy, 54(2), pp. 151-307, May 2007; and D.G. Blanchflower,
"International Evidence on Well-Being", presented to an NBER
conference, forthcoming in National Time Accounting and Subjective
Well-Being, A.B. Krueger, ed. University of Chicago Press.
(7) H.H. Koivuma, R. Honkanen, H. Viinamaeki, K. Heikkalae, J.
Kaprio, and M. Koskenvuo, "Life Satisfaction and Suicide: a 20-Year
Follow-Up Study", American Journal of Psychiatry, 1589(3), 2001,
pp. 433- 9.
(8) A.J. Oswald and N. Powdthavee, "Does Happiness Adapt? A
Longitudinal Study Of Disability with Implications for Economists and
Judges", Journal of Public Economics, 92, (2008), pp.1061-77.
(9) D.G. Blanchflower, "International Evidence on
Well-Being."
(10) A.B. Krueger, D. Kahneman, D. Schkade, N Schwarz, and A.A.
Stone, "National Time Accounting: The Currency of Life", in
National Time Accounting and Subjective Well-Being, A.B. Krueger, ed.
University of Chicago Press, forthcoming.
(11) D.G. Blanchflower and C. Shadforth, "Fear, Unemployment,
and Migration", NBER Working Paper No. 13506, September 2007; and
D.G. Blanchflower, "International Evidence on Well-Being."
(12) D. G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald "Well-Being Over Time
in Britain and the United States."
(13) B. Stevenson and J. Wolfers, "The Paradox Of Declining
Female Happiness", mimeo, University of Pennsylvania, 2007.
(14) R. Di Tella, R.J. MacCulloch, and A.J. Oswald,
"Preferences Over Inflation and Unemployment: Evidence from Surveys
of Happiness; American Economic Review, 91, 2001, pp. 33-41. See also J.
Wolfers, "Is Business Cycle Volatility Costly? Evidence from
Surveys of Subjective Wellbeing", International Finance, 6:1, 2003,
pp. 1-26.
(15) D.G. Blanchflower, "Is Unemployment More Costly Than
Inflation?" NBER Working Paper No. 13505, September 2007.
(16) D.G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald, "The Rising Well-Being
Of The Young", in Youth Employment and Joblessness in Advanced
Countries, D.G. Blanchflower and R. B. Freeman, eds., University of
Chicago Press, 2000.
(17) D. G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald "Well-Being Over Time
in Britain and the United States."
(18) A.J. Oswald and N Powdthavee, "Death, Happiness, and the
Calculation of Compensatory Damages", Journal of Legal Studies,
forthcoming.
(19) D.G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald, "Money, Sex and
Happiness", Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 106(3), 2004, pp
393-415.
(20) J. Gardner and A.J. Oswald, "Money And Mental Well-Being:
A Longitudinal Study of Medium Sized Lottery Wins", Journal of
Health Economics, 26, 2007, pp.49-60.
(21) E. Luttmer, "Neighbors As Negatives; Relative Earnings
and Well-Being", Quarterly Journal of Economics, August 2005,
120(3), pp. 963-1002.
(22) A. Alesina, R.Di Tella, and R.J. MacCulloch, "Inequality
and Happiness: Are Europeans and Americans Different?" Journal of
Public Economics, 88, 2004, pp. 2009-42.
(23) D.G. Blanchflower, "Unemployment, Well-Being and Wage
Curves in Eastern and Central Europe", Journal of Japanese and
International Economies, 15(4), December 2001, pp. 364-402.
(24) D.G. Blanchflower and C. Shadforth "Entrepreneurship in
the UK".
(25) D.G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald, "Is Well-Being
U-Shaped over the Life Cycle?" Social Science and Medicine, 66(8),
pp. 1733-1749, April 2008.
(26) D.G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald, "Happiness and
Hypertension Across Nations", Journal of Health Economics, 27(2),
March 2008, pp. 218-33.
(27) D.G. Blanchflower and A.J. Oswald, "Happiness and the
Human Development Index: the Paradox of Australia," The Australian
Economic Review, 38(3), September 2005, pp. 307-18; D.G. Blanchflower
and A.J. Oswald, "On Leigh-Wolfers and Well-Being in
Australia" The Australian Economic Review, 39(2), June 2006, pp.
185-6; and A. Leigh and J. Wolfers, "Happiness and the Human
Development Index: Australia is not a Paradox" Australian Economic
Review, 39(2), June 2006, pp. 176-84.
David G. Blanchflower *
* Blanchflower is a Research Associate in the NBER's Program
on Labor Studies and the Bruce V. Rauner Professor of Economics at
Dartmouth College. His Profile appears later in this issue.