The economics of education.
Levitt, Steven D.
In recent years, I have written a number of papers related to the
economics of education. This research agenda has three distinct strands.
One set of papers analyzes the impact of school choice on student
outcomes. A second line of research investigates teacher and
administrator cheating on standardized tests, and explores how such
behavior responds to the introduction of high-stakes testing. Third, I
have examined Black-White test score differentials and the role that the
educational system may play in contributing to those differences. I
discuss these three sets of papers in turn.
The Impact of Public School Choice on Student Outcomes
In recent years, school choice has become an increasingly prominent
feature of primary and secondary school education. With the passage of
new federal legislation (No Child Left Behind), there is little doubt
that the trend will continue. School choice comes in a variety of
flavors. Vouchers and charter schools are two types of school choice
which have received a great deal of both academic and media attention. A
third type of school choice, open enrollment, is actually far more
prevalent than either vouchers or charter schools. Under open
enrollment, students within a public school district are able to attend
schools other than their neighborhood school, including specially
designated magnet schools. As of 1996, open enrollment was available in
more than one in every seven school districts nationally, and in more
than a third of large districts. Moreover, No Child Left Behind mandates
that students in underperforming schools be provided the option to
attend other schools in the district.
Along with co-authors Julie B. Cullen and Brian Jacob, I have
written two papers that analyze the impact of open enrollment policies
on student outcomes in the Chicago Public Schools (ChiPS). ChiPS
represents an excellent laboratory for studying the impact of open
enrollment. Chicago has been among the most aggressive cities in
implementing this form of school choice, with more than half of the
students in the system presently opting out of their neighborhood
schools. Thus it may provide a window into what the future holds for
other districts that are moving in the same direction. The Chicago data
are also exceptionally rich, including not only detailed administrative
records on attainment and test scores, but also attitudinal surveys
administered periodically to students.
The first of these papers (1) starts with the observation that
students who opt out of their local school to take advantage of open
enrollment are 7.6 percentage points more likely to graduate from high
school than peers who are observationally equivalent in eighth
grade--off of a baseline graduation rate of 50 percent. This increment to graduation is the same order of magnitude as the gap between students
at Catholic and non-Catholic schools in previous studies.
There are several competing explanations for why students who opt
out of their assigned school outperform those who stay behind. Higher
graduation rates among those who opt out may be the result of these
students attending better schools or finding a school that better
matches their preferences. In either of these cases, the increased
graduation rates represent the true benefits of open enrollment. There
are, however, scenarios in which the students who take advantage of
school choice outperform students who do not, but the differences in
outcomes do not actually reflect real benefits of open enrollment.
Higher graduation rates among those who opt out may be spurious if those
who opt out are better on unobserved dimensions (for example, student
motivation, parental involvement). In other words, the students who opt
out may have systematically done better than other students, even if
they had not left their assigned schools. Also, it is possible that the
graduation gap is attributable not to the students who opt out doing
better, but rather to the students who remain behind doing worse, since
they have less able and motivated peers.
Our results suggest that, with the exception of career academies
(that is, vocational schools that focus on practical skills), the
benefits of school choice to students who opt out are illusory. There
are three primary pieces of evidence supporting this claim. First, in a
survey administered in eighth grade that asks students a wide range of
questions about their expectations for the future, past educational
record, and parental involvement, the responses are strongly correlated with both the likelihood of graduation and with the decision to opt out.
This suggests that students who opt out would be expected to do better,
even if they had to remain in their local school. The second piece of
evidence is that students who live in areas with many nearby schools on
average should derive the greatest benefit from the availability, of
school choice, because distance to nearby schools is a strong predictor
of the likelihood that a student will opt out of the assigned school.
Empirically, we find that easy access to a career academy is associated
with substantial increases in graduation likelihood, but the same is not
true for other types of schools, including high-achieving schools.
Finally, when we compare student outcomes within a given school (in most
schools in ChiPS some students are assigned and some opt in), we find
that those opting in do the same as those assigned at career academies,
but do much better at other schools. Since all students at a school
experience similar peers and teacher quality, the fact that those opting
in far outperform those assigned to the school reinforces the idea that
those who opt in are systematically better than observationally similar
students who make other schooling choices and would outperform them
regardless, except at career academies.
Our second paper on this topic (2) exploits the fact that school
choice causes desirable schools in ChiPS to be oversubscribed, and many
of these schools use randomized lotteries to determine which students
gain admission. We analyze data from 194 separate lotteries held to gain
access to high school. One drawback of the data is that we only observe
student outcomes if they enroll in ChiPS. To the extent that there is
selective attrition, the inferences drawn from a simple comparison of
outcomes of lottery winners and losers will be misleading. Relative to
past studies (for example, the Milwaukee voucher experiment), however,
attrition rates are low, with over 90 percent of the students remaining
in ChiPS.
Empirically, we find that those students who win the lotteries
attend what appear to be substantially better high schools--for example,
schools with higher achievement levels and graduation rates and lower
levels of poverty. Nonetheless, consistent with our first paper
discussed earlier, we find little evidence that attending these
sought-after programs provides any benefit on a wide variety of
traditional achievement measures, including standardized test scores,
attendance rates, course-taking patterns, credit accumulation, or
grades. We do, however, find evidence that attendance at such schools
may improve non-traditional outcome measures, such as self-reported
enjoyment of school, availability of computers, expectations for college
attendance, and arrest rates. This suggests that schools may be
influencing children in a variety of ways not generally captured by test
scores. To the extent that these non-traditional measures help to
predict life outcomes such as college attendance, labor market attachment, wages, and criminal involvement, an exclusive focus on test
scores will be misleading.
An important caveat to interpreting the results of both of these
papers is that we are only able to evaluate how access to a particular
school affects educational outcomes for a student, holding constant the
existence of a school choice program. We cannot estimate the overall
impact of introducing a system of school choice, which might induce
changes in residential location choice or in overall school quality due
to increased competition.
Teacher Cheating
High-stakes testing, like school choice, has become an increasingly
prominent feature of the educational landscape. Every state in the
country, except Iowa, currently administers state wide assessment tests
to students in elementary and secondary school. Federal legislation
requires states to test students annually in third through eighth grade
and to judge the performance of schools based on student achievement
scores.
The debate over high-stakes testing traditionally has pitted
proponents arguing that such tests increase incentives for learning and
hold schools accountable for their students' performance against
opponents who argue that the emphasis on testing will lead teachers to
substitute away
from teaching other skills or topics not directly tested on the exam.
Along with Brian Jacob, I have written two papers that explore a very
different concern regarding high-stakes testing--cheating on the part of
teachers and administrators. As incentives for high test scores
increase, unscrupulous teachers may be more likely to engage in a range
of illicit activities, such as changing student responses on answer
sheets, or filling in the blanks when a student falls to complete a
section. Our work in this area represents the first systematic attempt
to identify empirically the overall prevalence of teacher cheating and
to analyze the factors that predict cheating.
To address these questions, we once again turn to data from the
Chicago Public Schools, for which we have the question-by-question
answers given by every student in grades 3-7 taking the Iowa Test of
Basic Skills (ITBS) over an eight year period. In the first paper, (3)
we develop and test an algorithm for detecting cheating. Our approach
uses two types of cheating indicators: unexpected test score
fluctuations and unusual patterns of answers for students within a
classroom. Teacher cheating increases the likelihood that students in a
classroom will experience large, unexpected increases in test scores one
year, followed by very small test score gains (or even declines) the
following year. Teacher cheating, especially if done in an
unsophisticated manner, is also likely to leave tell-tale signs in the
form of blocks of identical answers, unusual patterns of correlations
across student answers within the classroom, or unusual response
patterns within a student's exam (for example, a student who
answers a number of very difficult questions correctly while missing
many simple questions).
Empirically, we find evidence of cheating in approximately 4 to 5
percent of the classes in our sample. For two reasons, this estimate is
likely to be a lower bound on the true incidence of cheating. First, we
focus only on the most egregious type of cheating, where teachers
systematically alter student test forms. There are other more subtle
ways in which teachers can cheat, such as providing extra time to
students, that our algorithm is unlikely to detect. Second, even when
test forms are altered, our approach is only partially successful in
detecting illicit behavior. We then demonstrate that the prevalence of
cheating responds to relatively minor changes in teacher incentives. The
importance of standardized tests in the ChiPS increased substantially
with a change in leadership in 1996. Schools that scored low on reading
tests were placed on probation and faced the threat of reconstitution.
Following the introduction of this policy, the prevalence of cheating
rose sharply in classrooms with large numbers of low-achieving students.
In contrast, schools with average or higher-achieving students, which
were at low risk for probation, showed no increase in cheating.
Our second paper on this topic (4) reports on the results of an
unusual policy implementation of our cheating detection tools. We were
invited by ChiPS to design and implement auditing and retesting
procedures implementing our methods. Using that cheating detection
algorithm, we selected roughly 120 classrooms to be retested on the
Spring 2002 ITBS. The classrooms retested include not only cases
suspected of cheating, but also classrooms that had achieved large gains
but were not suspected of cheating, as well as a randomly selected
control group. As a consequence, the implementation also allowed a
prospective test of the validity of the tools we developed in our first
paper on the subject.
The results of the retesting provided strong support for the
effectiveness of the cheating detection algorithm. Classrooms suspected
of cheating experienced large declines in test scores (on average about
one grade equivalent, although in some cases the fall in mean classroom
test scores was over three grade equivalents) when retested under
controlled conditions. In contrast, classrooms not suspected of cheating
a priori maintained virtually all of their gains on the retest. As a
consequence of these audits and subsequent investigations, disciplinary
action was brought against a substantial number of teachers, test
administrators, and principals.
Black-White Test Score Gaps Early in Life and the Contribution of
Schools
The Black-White test score gap is a robust empirical regularity. A
simple comparison of mean test scores typically finds Black students
scoring roughly one standard deviation below White students on
standardized tests. Even after controlling for a wide range of
covariates including family structure, socioeconomic status, measures of
school quality, and neighborhood characteristics, a substantial racial
gap in test scores persists.
In a paper joint with Roland Fryer, (5) I revisit this topic with a
newly collected data set, the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K). The survey covers a sample of more than
20,000 children entering kindergarten in the fall of 1998. The original
sample of students has subsequently been re-interviewed in the spring of
kindergarten and first grade.
The results we obtain using these new data are informative and in
some cases quite surprising. As in previous datasets, we observe
substantial racial differences in test scores in the raw data: Black
kindergartners score on average .64 standard deviations worse than
Whites. In stark contrast to earlier studies (including those looking at
kindergartners), however, after controlling for a small number of other
observable characteristics (children's age, child's birth
weight, a socio-economic status measure, WIC participation,
mother's age at first birth, and number of children's books in
the home), we essentially eliminate the Black-White test score gap in
math and reading for students entering kindergarten. While there are
numerous possible explanations for why our results differ so sharply
from earlier research, we conclude that real gains by recent cohorts of
Blacks are likely to he an important part of the explanation.
Despite the fact that we see no difference in initial test scores
for observationally equivalent Black and White children when they enter
kindergarten, their paths diverge once they are in school. Between the
beginning of kindergarten and the end of first grade, Black students
lose .20 standard deviations (approximately .10 standard deviation each
year) relative to White students with similar characteristics. The
leading explanation for the worse trajectory of Black students in our
sample Is that they attend lower quality schools. When we compare the
change in test scores over time for Blacks and Whites attending the same
school, Black students lose only a third as much ground as they do
relative to Whites in the overall sample. This result suggests that
differences in quality across schools attended by Whites and Blacks is
likely to be an important part of the story. Interestingly, along
"traditional" dimensions of school quality (class size,
teacher education, computer-to-student ratio, and so on), Blacks and
Whites attend schools that are similar. On a wide range of "non
standard" school inputs (for example, gang problems in school,
percent of students on free lunch, amount of loitering in front of
school by non-students, amount of litter around the school, whether or
not students need hall passes, and PTA funding), Blacks do appear to be
attending much worse schools. Other explanations for the divergence in
Black White test scores, such as a greater "summer setback"
for Blacks when school is not in session, or discrimination by teachers
against Blacks, find no support in our data.
(1) J. B. Cullen, B. Jacob, and S. D. Levitt, "The Impact of
School Choice on Student Outcomes: An Analysis of the Chicago Public
Schools," NBER Working Paper No. 7888, September 2000, forthcoming
in Journal of Public Economics.
(2) J. B. Cullen, B. Jamb, and S. D. Levitt, "The Effect of
School Choice on Student Outcomes: Evidence from Randomized
Lotteries," forthcoming as an NBER Working Paper.
(3) B. Jacob and S. D. Levitt, "Rotten Apples: An
Investigation of the Prevalence and Predictors of Teacher
Cheating," NBER Working Paper No. 9413, January 2003, and Quarterly
Journal of Economics, 117 (August 2003), pp. 843-77.
(4) B. Jacob and S. D. Levitt, "Catching Cheating Teachers:
The Results of an Unusual Experiment in Implementing Theory," NBER
Working Paper No. 9414, January, 2003, and Brookings-Wharton Papers on
Urban Affairs, 2003.
(5) R. Fryer and S. D. Levitt, "Understanding the Black-While
Test Score Gap in the First Two Years of School," NBER Working
Paper No. 8975, June 2002, forthcoming in Review of Economics and
Statistics.
Steven D. Levitt, Levitt is a Research Associate in the NBER's
Programs on Public Economics, law and Economics, Children, and
Education. He is also a Professor of Economics at the University of
Chicago.