首页    期刊浏览 2025年05月11日 星期日
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:All knowledge is not created equal: knowledge effects and the 2012 presidential debates.
  • 作者:Gottfried, Jeffrey A. ; Hardy, Bruce W. ; Winneg, Kenneth M.
  • 期刊名称:Presidential Studies Quarterly
  • 印刷版ISSN:0360-4918
  • 出版年度:2014
  • 期号:August
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:Center for the Study of the Presidency
  • 关键词:Campaign debates;Information literacy;Presidential candidates

All knowledge is not created equal: knowledge effects and the 2012 presidential debates.


Gottfried, Jeffrey A. ; Hardy, Bruce W. ; Winneg, Kenneth M. 等


Decades of research have confirmed that debates increase viewer knowledge about the issue stands of the candidates (e.g., Benoit, Hansen, and Verser 2003). However, the conditions under which viewers learn are less well understood. In this article, we examine how differences in the context of information in presidential debates affect both who learns from such debates and what they learn. We ask three research questions regarding learning from exposure to the 2012 general election presidential debates: (1) Did these debates increase knowledge of the content discussed in them? (2) Were viewers more likely to learn about issues and matters relevant to the 2012 presidential election when one candidate challenged the other's view or when the candidate's statement was not contested? (3) Does learning occur at the same rate regardless of viewer political predispositions, or did confirmation bias determine whether and which individuals learned from the debates?

Issue Knowledge and "Correct" Voting

For a representative democracy to work, citizens must be able to cast a "correct vote" (Lau and Redlawsk 1997), one in line with their policy preferences. In a world in which presidential campaigns do at times mislead (see Jackson and Jamieson 2007), citizens may vote for a candidate on the supposition that their positions align when in fact they do not (see Waldman and Jamieson 2006). In 2008, for example, the Obama campaign used targeted radio ads to deceptively cast Senator John McCain as opposed to federal funding for stem-cell research (see Kenski, Hardy, and Jamieson 2010). If a stem-cell issue voter voted against McCain based on this deceptive message, then an "incorrect vote" was cast.

Because most of us do not personally know national political actors, we rely on media and campaign events to provide the necessary information to cast a "correct vote." One important function of presidential campaigns, then, is to inform voters of candidate policy stands and of other factual information that is relevant to the election. In our system of government, this function is central because, as Gans (2003) wrote, "The country's democracy may belong directly or indirectly to its citizens, but the democratic process can only be truly meaningful if these citizens are informed" (1). Debates offer a unique opportunity for voters to gain such information.

Debates and Learning

Since incumbent president and Republican nominee Gerald R. Ford challenged Democrat Jimmy Carter to a series of nationally televised debates in 1976, broadcast debates have become standard campaign events in U.S. politics. These quadrennial events demand an unmatched level of accountability of those seeking public office. Research on debate viewing effects has focused primarily on political learning and shifts in vote intentions (e.g., Benoit and Hansen 2004; Chaffee 1978; Jamieson and Adasiewicz 2000). We focus on the former.

Past studies have consistently linked debates viewing with higher levels of political knowledge. A meta-analysis of 18 studies on debates from 1976 to 2000 (total N--7,202) found a mean weighted correlation coefficient of 0.256 between debates and political knowledge, suggesting that on average, debate viewing has a substantial impact on viewer learning of the candidates' policy positions (Benoit, Hansen, and Verser 2003). Some research suggests that debates increase issue salience and have an agenda-setting effect (Becker et al. 1978; Benoit and Hansen 2004). Benoit and Hansen (2004) examined the 1976, 1984, 1996, and 2000 data in the American National Election Studies (ANES) and found that debate watchers cited more issues in their evaluations of the candidates than did nondebate watchers. Consistent with these findings, we hypothesize,

Hypothesis 1: Exposure to the 2012 presidential debates will increase accurate knowledge of the issues and matters discussed in the debates themselves.

Debate Learning by Type of Information

This study adds to the existing literature by examining whether and how the context in which information is discussed within a presidential debate affects rates of learning. In debates, information can be divided into two categories: that which is contested and uncontested. Contested information is presented by one candidate and challenged by the other. Uncontested information is presented without challenge. We will treat these information structures as two different informational contexts.

The structure of campaign information can influence the motivation and the ability of individuals to process it (Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida 1994). For example, a candidate speech is a simple structure that is often person-centered and invites easy candidate evaluation. Debates, on the other hand, are more complex, presenting information on a number of different facets from two or more candidates in an alternating format. Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida (1994) characterize debates as a dimension-center format and conclude that the more person-centered information contexts, such as the candidate speeches, require less cognitive effort to process, whereas in the more dimension-centered contexts, such as debates, the complex presentation of the information may interfere with the ability to make sense of the information (Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida 1994). This finding is consistent with Just, Crigler, and Wallach's (1990) finding that political ads are more likely to increase candidate knowledge than debates.

Like the candidate speech, uncontested information provides a single viewpoint or dimension. By contrast, contested information creates a more complex context. Among other things, the presence of two or more sides to an argument can lead to confusion. Thus, we further hypothesize,

Hypothesis 2: Information introduced by one candidate and not rebutted by the other-uncontested information--is learned at the greater rate than information that is presented by one candidate but challenged by the opponent--contested information.

Similar to Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida (1994), we hypothesize that motivation and ability to process information in these two different contexts will vary based on viewers' political sophistication. Because political sophisticates have greater "cognitive dexterity, they should be less constrained by the structure of the information imposed on them" (Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida 1994, 195). Likewise, since processing contested information requires greater cognitive effort than uncontested information (see Hypothesis 2) and since political sophisticates have greater cognitive dexterity, they should be less hobbled by complex information contexts than political nonsophisticates. Thus,

Hypothesis 3: The rate of learning in general and learning from contested exchanges in particular will be greater among political sophisticates than political nonsophisticates.

Previous studies have found that one's political beliefs will influence how one interacts with and processes political information because of selective attention and exposure (Graf and Aday 2008; Stroud 2008, 2011). But when viewers are exposed to a source that presents information that is both consistent and inconsistent with their political beliefs, viewers will likely rely on heuristics, such as candidate preference, in determining which side is accurate. The confirmation bias hypothesis suggests that people bolster "hypotheses or beliefs whose truth is in question," create "one-sided case building processes]," and interpret "evidence in ways that are partial to existing beliefs, expectations, or a hypothesis in hand" (Nickerson 1998, 175). When exposed to information in a debate, particularly information that is contested, confirmation bias should be actively and forcefully at play. The same may not be true of uncontested information because the accuracy of this information is not in question. Specifically, viewers may respond to uncontested claims using a simple heuristic that assumes if the statement was not accurate, the side disadvantaged by the information would flag the inaccuracy. Therefore, we hypothesize,

Hypothesis 4: When information in a debate is contested, viewers will be more likely to accept the interpretation offered by their preferred candidate, but uncontested information will be accepted regardless of candidate preference.

Method

We analyze data from the Annenberg Public Policy Center's (APPC) Institutions of Democracy 2012 Political Knowledge Survey, a six-wave national cross-sectional telephone survey of U.S. adults, 18 years or older, conducted during and after the 2012 presidential election. In this article, we use waves 3 and 4 since they were conducted immediately following the second and third presidential debates. Wave 3 was fielded between October 17 and October 23, 2012, and wave 4 between October 24 and October 29, 2012. Throughout the analyses in this article, wave 3 is used to test the hypotheses only for the second presidential debate, and wave 4 is used to test the hypothesis only for the third presidential debate, since each wave was fielded immediately following each respective debate.

Under contract to APPC, Social Science Research Solutions (SSRS) completed a total of 1,233 interviews for wave 3 and 1,248 for wave 4 with randomly selected adults contacted through random-digit dialing (RDD) of cell phones (wave 3: N = 422; wave 4: N = 430) and landline telephones (wave 3: N = 811; wave 4: N = 818). (1) We oversampled households in battleground states; 53.0% of the sample (N = 653) in wave 3 and 49.1% of the sample (N = 613) in wave 4 is residents in a battleground state. (2)

Dependent Variables

Total Knowledge of the Second and Third Presidential Debates. Our focus in this article is not on recall of what was said in the debates but on the accuracy of the knowledge gained from exposure. Consequently, we rely on a series of knowledge questions that asked respondents about content discussed in the second and third presidential debates. In wave 3, respondents were asked 16 items about the second presidential debate, and in wave 4, they were asked six items about the third presidential debate. All six items asked about the third presidential debate in wave 4 were also asked in wave 3. (3) Table 1 presents the wave 3 items, their correct answers, and the percent of the sample that answered each item correctly; (4) Table 2 presents the same for the items in wave 4. Each item was first coded to indicate whether a respondent answered it correctly or not. (5) Scales were then created to indicate the percent of total items answered correctly about the second presidential debate from wave 3 (mean = 55.24%; SD = 18.62) and the third presidential debate from wave 4 (mean = 51.38%; SD = 21.58).

Contested and Uncontested Debate Knowledge. The knowledge items were categorized into two groupings: knowledge based on information that was contested and knowledge based on accurate information that was not contested in either debate. The categorization of each item is dependent only on whether the information was contested or uncontested in the second presidential debate for the wave 3 items and in the third presidential debate for the wave 4 items, and not whether it was contested or uncontested in any other form of communication, including other debates. A question based on contested information asked, "Has oil production on federal lands increased, decreased, or stayed the same under President Obama?" In the debate, Obama stated, "So here's what I've done since I've been president. We have increased oil production to the highest levels in 16 years." Romney followed up this claim by arguing, "As a matter of fact, oil production is down 14 percent this year on federal land, and gas production is down 9 percent. Why? Because the president cut in half the number of licenses and permits for drilling on federal lands and in federal waters." A question drawing on accurate uncontested information from the second debate is "Which candidate proposes raising federal income taxes on households earning 250 thousand dollars or more per year? Barack Obama, Mitt Romney, Both, or Neither?" In this debate, Obama clearly says that he plans to raise federal income taxes on households earning $250,000 a year, and the statement was not challenged by Romney:

PRESIDENT OBAMA: [I]n addition to some tough spending cuts, we've also got to make sure that the wealthy do a little bit more. So what I've said is your first $250,000 worth of income, no change. And that means 98 percent of American families, 97 percent of small businesses, they will not see a tax increase. I'm ready to sign that bill right now.... But what I've also said is for above 250,000 (dollars), we can go back to the tax rates we had when Bill Clinton was president."

For the second presidential debate (wave 3), four items were categorized as based on information that was contested and 11 were based on accurate information that was uncontested. One item was categorized as neither based on contested nor on uncontested accurate information and thus not included in either scale. For the third presidential debate (wave 4), three items were classed as based on information that was contested in the debate and three on accurate information that was uncontested. The categorization of each item is also presented in Tables 1 and 2. Scales were created in both waves that indicate the percent of items based on contested information answered correctly (wave 3: mean = 39-52%, SD = 22.97; wave 4: mean = 39-21%, SD = 26.72) and based on accurate uncontested information answered correctly (wave 3: mean = 63-21%, SD = 23-61; wave 4: mean = 63.62%, SD = 31.98).

Independent Variables

Second and Third Presidential Debate Viewership. Respondents were asked in wave 3 how much of the second presidential debate they watched, and respondents in wave 4 were asked how much of the third presidential debate they watched. These two variables are on a four-point scale ranging from 0 (none) to 3 (all) (wave 3: mean = 1.73, SD = 1.25; wave 4: mean = 1.48; SD = 1.27).

Political Sophistication. In the same vein as Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida (1994), we created a composite index from several different measures for our measure of political sophistication. We standardized and took the mean of three measures: total news media consumption, how closely respondents were following the election, and basic campaign knowledge. Total news media consumption was measured by taking the mean of five items that indicate the days/week that individuals watch local news, broadcast national nightly news, their most frequently watched cable news channel, listened to talk radio, or read a newspaper. The scale ranges from 0 (very low news media consumption) to 7 (very high news media consumption) (wave 3: mean = 3.14, SD = 1.68; wave 4: mean = 2.97, SD = 1.69). Closely following was measured with a single item that asked respondents "How closely are you following the 2012 presidential campaign?" ranging from 1 (not closely at all) to 4 (very closely) (wave 3: mean = 3.31, SD = 0.89; wave 4: mean = 3.26, SD = 0.96). Basic campaign knowledge was measured with two items that asked respondents to name the current vice president of the United States and the Republican vice presidential candidate. A three-point scale ranging from 0 to 2 correct answers was created (wave 3: mean = 1.45, SD = 0.80; wave 4: mean = 1.47, SD = 0.76). The standardized scale from these three items was split at the median to determine whether respondents were political sophisticates or nonsophisticates. The measure varied somewhat from Rahn, Aldrich, and Borgida's (1994); notably, we did not include a measure of participation in political activities.

Candidate Favorability. This variable was constructed by subtracting a feeling thermometer ranging from 0 to 10 for Mitt Romney from that of Barack Obama, creating a scale ranging from -10 (very high favorability of Romney over Obama) to 10 (very high favorability of Obama over Romney) (wave 3: mean = 0.58, SD = 7.16; wave 4: mean = 0.54, SD = 7.07).

Analyses

First, for the test of Hypothesis 1, multiple linear regression models predicting total debate knowledge from debate viewing were created. Next, for the tests of Hypothesis 2, linear regression models were conducted to predict the scales for knowledge based on information that was contested in the debates and knowledge based on accurate information that was uncontested in the debates. For the test of Hypothesis 3, multiple linear regression models were constructed to predict each of the three knowledge scales from debate viewing, political sophistication, and the interaction between debate viewing and political sophistication. Finally, for the test of Hypothesis 4, two logistic regression models were conducted for each individual knowledge item to test whether the effect of debate viewing varies by candidate favorability; one model predicted correct knowledge of each item from debate viewership, and a second included candidate favorability and an interaction between debate viewership and candidate favorability. In addition to the independent variables already discussed, each model controls for total news media consumption, basic campaign knowledge, (6) residence in a battleground state, political party identification, political ideology, years of education, race, gender, and age. (7)

Results

Table 3 presents the models that predict each of the three knowledge scales of the second presidential debate (wave 3)--total knowledge, knowledge based on information that was contested, and knowledge based on accurate information that was uncontested. Table 4 presents the same for the third presidential debate (wave 4). The first model in both Tables 3 and 4 tests Hypothesis 1, which predicts that debate exposure will increase total knowledge of content discussed in the debate. Consistent with the hypothesis, we find that reported debate viewership increased total knowledge in both debates; reporting watching all of the second debate increased the percent of questions answered correctly by 11.9% compared to not watching any of the debate (t = 9-88, p < .001), and reported viewing all of the third debate increased the number of questions answered correctly by 15.2% compared to not watching any of that debate (t = 10.53, p < .001).(8)

It should first be noted that respondents answered more of the questions based on accurate uncontested information correctly than those based on contested information; 63.2% (wave 3) and 63.6% (wave 4) of the items based on accurate uncontested information were answered correctly compared to 39.5% (wave 3) and 39.2% (wave 4) of items based on information that was contested.

The second and third models in Tables 3 and 4 test Hypothesis 2, which predicts that debate exposure will increase knowledge based on information that was uncontested in the debates more so than knowledge based on information that was contested. First, we find strong evidence from both waves that viewing the debates increased accurate information that was uncontested. Watching all of the second debate increased knowledge of the accurate uncontested information by 16.0% (t= 10.53, p< .001) compared to not watching any of that debate, and watching all of the third debate increased accurate uncontested knowledge by 22.2% (t = 10.12, p < .001) compared to not watching any of that debate. Additionally, we find evidence from wave 4 that debate viewing increased knowledge based on information that was contested. Watching all of the third presidential debate increased contested knowledge by 8.4% (t = 4.35, p < .001) compared to not watching that debate at all. While the coefficient for the effect of watching the second presidential debate on knowledge based on contested information was also positive in wave 3, it does not meet conventional standards of statistical significance.

Further, consistent with the hypothesis, the coefficient of debate viewership significantly differs between the two knowledge scales for both debates; the effect of debate viewership was greater on knowledge based on accurate uncontested information than on knowledge based on contested information (wave 3: F = 36.95, p < .001; wave 4: F = 21.76, p < .001). The predicted values for the effects of debate watching on the two knowledge scales for the second presidential debate are presented in Figure 1, and for the third presidential debate, they are presented in Figure 2. The slope of the lines for both knowledge based on accurate uncontested information and knowledge based on contested information increases as the amount of debate viewing does (though not significantly for knowledge based on contested information for the second presidential debate), but at a greater rate for knowledge based on accurate uncontested information.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

Tables 5 and 6 present the models for the test of Hypothesis 3, which predict that political sophisticates will learn from the debates at a greater rate than nonsophisticates, particularly for the knowledge based on contested information. First, we find that political sophisticates were significantly more knowledgeable than nonsophisticates for total knowledge and knowledge based on accurate uncontested information in both waves. While also in the expected positive direction, the coefficients for being a political sophisticate in the models that predict knowledge based on contested information are not significant in either wave. Additionally, there is no evidence in any of the models that the effect of debate viewership on total knowledge, knowledge based on contested information, or knowledge based on accurate uncontested information varied between political sophisticates and nonsophisticates, as seen with the nonsignificant interaction terms. Thus, we find no evidence for Hypothesis 3.

To begin understanding why there are the differences in the rates of learning of each type of knowledge, as found in the test of Hypothesis 2, we turn to the logistic regression models in Table 7 that predict each knowledge item individually. There are two separate models for each item; the first column for each item is a model that predicts having answered the item correctly from debate viewing, and the second column includes candidate favorability and the interaction between debate viewing and candidate favorability.

[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

From the models in the first column for each item, we find that two of the seven (28.6%) items based on contested information across the two debates (zero of four items from the second debate and two of the three from the third debate) were significantly and positively predicted by debate viewing. Comparatively, 11 of the 14 (78.6%) items based on accurate uncontested information (eight of 11 items from the second debate and all three from the third debate) were significantly predicted by debate viewing. Information complexity, then, provides one explanation for why the rate of learning contested information is lower than uncontested accurate information. When accurate information is rebutted during a debate, complexity increases, and audiences are less likely to learn.(9)

These differences in the rates of learning based on contested compared to uncontested information also presumably occur because viewers assume that arguments advanced by their candidate are accurate regardless of whether or not they are, as stated in Hypothesis 4, which predicts that viewers would be more likely to learn the interpretation offered by their preferred candidate among the information that is contested in the debates. The models for the test of this hypothesis are presented in the second column for each item in Table 7. Starting with the knowledge based on accurate uncontested information, there is only evidence for four of the 14 (28.6%) items that learning varied by respondents' preference for one candidate over the other. Thus, for the items based on information that was accurate and uncontested in the debates, learning from debate viewing occurred for a vast majority of these items and most often occurred at the same rate regardless of which candidate the respondents supported.

While learning often did not vary by candidate preference among the knowledge items based on information that was accurate and uncontested in the debates, learning often did vary by candidate preference among the knowledge items based on information that was contested in the debates. Among four of the seven (57.1%) knowledge items based on contested information--two of the four from the second presidential debate and two of three from the third presidential debate--the rate of learning varied with candidate preference. A positive interaction indicates that Obama supporters learned that specific piece of information at a greater rate than Romney supporters, while a negative interaction indicates that Romney supporters learned at a greater rate than Obama supporters. Nonsignificant interaction terms indicate that the rate of learning, if any, does not differ by candidate preference. Of the two contested items that are significantly predicted by debate viewing, both vary by respondent candidate preference, suggesting that when learning occurred overall, it occurred at a depressed rate. Further, among two of the contested items in which learning overall did not occur, the effect of debate viewing varied by respondent disposition toward the candidates. In other words, for these two items, learning of correct and incorrect answers both occurred, but the overall effects of debate viewing on knowledge was cancelled out by partisan preference. Because both of these items were from the second presidential debate, this explains why there was no significant effect of debate viewing on knowledge of information based on contested information in the second debate. For the remaining three contested knowledge items, there is no evidence of learning of any kind. Overall, these results provide evidence for confirmation bias in that learning information that is contested is largely affected by candidate preference.

Further, the directions of the interaction terms suggest that those who preferred Obama over Romney gained accurate knowledge about the contested items in which the correct answer was favorable to the Obama candidacy, and those who preferred Romney over Obama gained accurate knowledge about the contested items in which the correct answer was favorable to the Romney candidacy. For example, for the item that asks the accuracy of the statement, "While in foreign countries, Barack Obama has repeatedly apologized for America" (third debate, wave 4), the positive interaction suggests that as favorability for Obama over Romney increased, so did the likelihood of reporting the correct answer from viewing the third debate. This relationship can be seen in Figure 3.10 Conversely, for the statement that asks about the accuracy of the statement, "Governor Romney wanted to take Detroit auto manufacturers into bankruptcy without providing any form of federal aid" (third debate, wave 4), the negative interaction suggests that as support for Romney over Obama increased, the rate of reporting the correct answer increased.(11) This relationship can be seen in Figure 4.

[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]

Discussion

This study explores how the context of information in debates influences rates of learning and who learns. First, consistent with previous literature, we report that watching the debates increased knowledge of campaign issues and related matters discussed in the debates. We also found that knowledge based on accurate information that was uncontested in the debate was gained at a greater rate than knowledge based on information that was presented by one candidate but contested by the other. Thus, when accurate information is presented in a debate, the context of the exchange influences the rate of learning.

[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]

We did not find differences in the amount of knowledge political sophisticates and nonsophisticates gained from debates. Debates may provide a venue in which learning the issues and matters relevant to a specific presidential campaign occurs regardless of how politically sophisticated viewers are. We do realize, though, that there may be limitation in our measure of sophistication.

Consistent with confirmation bias, learning based on information that was contested in the debate was influenced by viewers' candidate preferences more often than not. Yet, learning largely occurred at the same rate for knowledge that was based on accurate information that was uncontested in the debate. These findings suggest that in a debate format, confirmation bias often, but does not always, influence whether viewers will gain accurate knowledge when information is contested. Additionally, even though there was uncontested information about both candidates, in most cases if the information was presented without rebuttal, it was learned equally among both Romney and Obama supporters. This result confirms a civic function of debates. Specifically, in some contexts they can increase viewer knowledge about both candidates regardless of candidate preference.

Some notable limitations exist in this research. First, the analyses are based on observational survey data, whose internal validity is less robust than experimental designs. Replicating these results through an experimental design would be the next step. Specifically, due to the nature of observational survey data, the reverse relationship between debate viewership and knowledge is possible in that those who are more knowledgeable about campaign content are more likely to have viewed the debates. Given the specificity of the questions asked about the debate content, the extensive list of controls, and the consistency of the findings with previous research, though, we suspect that the direction of the relationship of exposure increasing knowledge is more plausible. Next, we recognize the research that suggests that people overreport their media and presidential debate consumption (e.g., Prior 2009, 2012). However, since we examine the effect of debate viewership on knowledge, overreported debate viewership would understate effects because those who falsely reported viewing the debates were analyzed as viewing them, diminishing the actual knowledge effects of those who accurately reported viewing the debates.

Overall, this study is important for three reasons. First, it confirms that debates remain important events that foster learning about the candidates and their issue stands. Second, it details the influence of context (specifically whether information is or is not contested) on learning in absorption of debate content. Finally, it underscores the finding that confirmation bias is strongly at play among debate viewers but plays a lesser role in the face of uncontested than contested information.

References

Becker, Lee B., Idowu A. Sobowale, Robin E. Cobbey, and Chaim H. Eyal. 1978. "Debates' Effects on Voters' Understanding of Candidates and Issues." In The Presidential Debates: Media, Electoral, and Policy Perspectives, eds. G. F. Bishop, R. G. Meadow, and M. Jackson-Beeck. New York: Praeger, 126-39

Benoit, William L., and Glenn J. Hansen. 2004. "Presidential Debate Watching, Issue Knowledge, Character Evaluation, and Vote Choice." Human Communication Research 30 (1): 121-44.

Benoit, William L., Glenn J. Hansen, and R. M. Verser. 2003. "A Meta-analysis of the Effects of Viewing U.S. Presidential Debates." Communication Monographs 70 (4): 335-50.

Chaffee, Steven. 1978. "Presidential Debates: Are They Helpful to Voters?" Communication Monographs 45 (4): 330-46.

Gans, Herbert J. 2003. Democracy and the News. New York: Oxford University Press.

Graf, Joseph, and Sean Aday. 2008. "Selective Attention to Online Political Information." Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 52 (1): 86-100.

Jackson, Brooks, and Kathleen Hall Jamieson. 2007. Hnspun: Finding Facts in a World of Disinformation. New York: Random House.

Jamieson, Kathleen Hall, and Christopher Adasiewicz. 2000. "What Can Voters Learn from Election Debates?" In Televised Election Debates: International Perspectives, ed. Stephen Coleman. New York: St. Martin's Press, 25-42.

Just, Marion, Ann Crigler, and Lori Wallach. 1990. "Thirty Seconds or Thirty Minutes: What Viewers Learn from Spot Advertisements and Candidate Debates ."Journal of Communication 40 (3): 120-33.

Kenski, Kate, Bruce W. Hardy, and Kathleen Hall Jamieson. 2010. The Obama 'Victory: How Media, Money, and Message Shaped the 2008 Election. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lau, Richard R., and David P. Redlawsk. 1997. "Voting Correctly." American Political Science Review 91 (3): 585-98.

Nickerson, Raymond S. 1998. "Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises." Review of General Psychology 2 (2): 175-220.

Prior, Markus. 2009. "Improving Media Effects Research through Better Measurement of News Exposure. "Journal of Politics 71 (3): 893-908.

--.2012. "Who Watches Presidential Debates? Measurement Problems in Campaign Effects Research." Public Opinion Quarterly 76 (2): 350-63.

Rahn, Wendy M., John H. Aldrich, and Eugene Borgida. 1994. "Individual and Contextual Variations in Political Candidate Appraisal." American Political Science Review 88 (1): 193-99.

Stroud, Natlie Jomini. 2008. "Media Use and Political Predispositions: Revisiting the Concept of Selective Exposure." Political Behavior 30 (3): 341-66.

--.2011. Niche News: The Politics of News Choice. New York: Oxford University Press.

Waldman, Paul, and Kathleen Hall Jamieson. 2006. "Rhetorical Convergence and Issue Knowledge in the 2000 Presidential Election." Presidential Studies Quarterly 33 (1): 145-63.

JEFFREY A. GOTTFRIED

Pew Research Center

BRUCE W. HARDY

Annenberg Public Policy Center

University of Pennsylvania

KENNETH M. WINNEG

Annenberg Public Policy Center

University of Pennsylvania

KATHLEEN HALL JAMIESON

Annenberg Public Policy Center

University of Pennsylvania

(1.) All interviews were conducted by live interviewers using computer-assisted telephone interviewing (CATI) software. In households reached by landline, an adult was selected at random based on their age (or most recent birthday). The questionnaire was delivered in Spanish for those who selected that option. Nonresponsive phone numbers were contacted up to six times, and in cases where initial attempts were met with soft refusal (i.e., abrupt hang-ups), refusal-conversion attempts were made. Accounting for design effects produced by survey weights, the margin of error is 3.7 percent for wave 3 and 3.6 percent for wave 4. The response rate was 13% for wave 3 and 11% for wave 4. Response rate uses the standard AAPOR RR3 formula.

(2.) These percentages are not weighted; every other raw percent in this article is weighted by national population parameters. Battleground states were defined as Colorado, Florida, Iowa, Michigan, Nevada, New Hampshire, North Carolina, Pennsylvania, Ohio, Virginia, and Wisconsin. The total share of adults residing in these states is 28.9% based on the 2012 March Supplement of the U.S. Census Bureau's Current Population Survey (CPS) (http://www.census.gov/cps).

(3.) Questions were chosen for inclusion in the debate knowledge scales only if they were directly and explicitly discussed by either candidate in the second and third presidential debates. If a question was not directly and explicitly discussed in the debate, then it was not included in the scale for that debate. There were no questions that met this requirement that were omitted from the analysis. The difference in the number of questions used for the scales between the second and third debates is due to there being fewer questions asked that met this requirement for the third debate.

(4.) To determine whether a candidate's statement about his or his opponent's future plans were accurate in the debates, we relied on candidate statements in speeches and on the candidates' web site. Many of the issues and claims about past actions or the content of speeches asked about in our survey were discussed by the four major national fact-checking organizations (Washington Post's Fact Checker. Pulitijact, FactCheck.org, and the Associated Press). For those questions in which the issues and claims were discussed by these organizations, we adopted their conclusions to determine the correct answer to each of our questions.

(5.) Do not know responses were coded as not correct. Refusals were coded as missing.

(6.) The models for Hypotheses 3 do not include total news media consumption and basic campaign knowledge since the measure of political sophistication already includes these two measures.

(7.) The following is the demographic breakdown of wave 3: political party identification--25.8% Republican, 33-6% Democrat; political ideology (l=very libetal; 5 = very conservative)--mean = 3.15, SD = 1.26; years of education--mean--13.82; SD = 2.51; race--12.4% black, 12.5% Hispanic; gender--51.4% female; age--mean = 47.78, SD = 17.80. The following is the demographic breakdown of wave 4: political party identification--22.6% Republican, 34.5% Democrat; political ideology (l=very liberal; 5= very conservative)--mean = 3.27, SD = 1.19; years of education--mean = 13.84; SD = 2.50; race--13-4% black, 13.5% Hispanic; gender--51.7% female; age--mean = 47.14, SD = 17.87.

(8.) For the models in Tables 3 and 4, we did not control for closely following the election because while this variable may be a confounding variable, it also theoretically can serve as a mediating variable between debate viewership and knowledge. That said, we ran an additional set of models with closely following as a control. In these models the magnitude of the coefficient for debate viewership across models is slightly depressed, but in the same direction, and significant where they were significant in Tables 3 and 4.

(9.) For the models in Table 7, we did not control for closely following the election for reasons previously mentioned. That said, we ran additional models with closely following as a control. The only mentionable differences for the second debate items are that the main effect of debate viewing in the noninteraction models of items 5 and 9 falls below significance, and the interaction between candidate favorability and debate watching becomes significant for item 11. For the third presidential debate, the only mentionable difference is that the main effect for debate viewership in the noninteraction model for item 1 falls below significance.

(10.) Figures 3 and 4 were created by plotting the predicted values for these two knowledge items at three different intervals of candidate favorability: completely favorable of Obama over Romney (i.e., 10 on the candidate favorability scale), completely favorable of Romney over Obama (i.e., -10 on the candidate favorability scale), and not favorable of one candidate over the other (i.e., 0 on the candidate favorability scale). These three theoretical points were chosen specifically as examples of the extremes of candidate favorability and a midpoint of no preference. Thus, this graph should not be interpreted as three groups of individuals, but instead as three lines of the ranges of the rates of learning since the candidate favorability scale is a continuum from -10 to 10. Based on the logistic regression models, the baseline respondents could be interpreted as those who did not favor one candidate over the other and did not watch any of the debate.

(11.) It should be noted that based on the predicted values in Figure 4, the rate of learning among those who completely favored Obama over Romney (i.e., 10 on the candidate favorability scale) is virtually flat. This is consistent with the notion that those who favored Romney learned at a greater rate about contested knowledge that was favorable of Romney and that those who favored Obama would learn at a depressed rate, if at all.

Jeffrey A. Gottfried is a research associate for the Pew Research Center for the People & the Press and the Pew Research Journalism Project.

Bruce W. Hardy is a senior researcher in political communication at the Annenberg Public Policy Center of the University of Pennsylvania.

Kenneth Winneg is the managing director of Survey Research at the Annenberg Public Policy Center of the University of Pennsylvania.

Kathleen HallJamieson is the Elizabeth Ware Packard Professor at the Annenberg School for Communication and director of the Annenberg Public Policy Center of the University of Pennsylvania.
TABLE 1
Knowledge Items Asked of Second Presidential Debate (Wave 3)

              Question Wording             Correct Answer   % Correct

     Knowledge Based on Contested
     Information

1    Mitt Romney's tax plan will            Not Accurate     46.9%
     increase the deficit by over 4 and
     a half trillion dollars. How
     accurate do you think that
     statement is? Would you say it is
     very accurate, somewhat accurate,
     not too accurate, or not accurate
     at all?

2    Has oil production on federal lands    Increased        30.3%
     increased, decreased, or stayed the
     same under President Obama?

3    Governor Romney wanted to take         Not Accurate     24.2%
     Detroit auto manufacturers into
     bankruptcy without providing any
     form of federal aid. How accurate
     do you think that statement is?
     Would you say it is very accurate,
     somewhat accurate, not too
     accurate, or not accurate at all?

4    No new jobs have been created as a     Not Accurate     56.7%
     result of the stimulus which was
     passed in the first year of Obama's
     presidency. How accurate do you
     think that statement is? Would you
     say it is very accurate, somewhat
     accurate, not too accurate, or not
     accurate at all?

     Knowledge Based on Accurate
     Uncontested Information

5    Which candidate would push for         Barack Obama     49.2%
     passage of the assault weapons ban?
     Barack Obama, Mitt Romney, both, or
     neither?

6    Which candidate favors branding        Mitt Romney      58.0%
     China as a currency manipulator and
     imposing tariffs on Chinese goods?
     Barack Obama, Mitt Romney, Both, or
     Neither?

7    In early October, the Bureau of        Accurate         65.5%
     Labor Statistics announced that the
     national unemployment rate dropped
     below eight percent. How accurate
     do you think that statement is?
     Would you say it is very accurate,
     somewhat accurate, not too
     accurate, or not accurate at all?

8    Which candidate has promised to        Mitt Romney      58.1%
     increase military spending? Barack
     Obama, Mitt Romney, Both, or
     Neither?

9    Which candidate proposes raising       Barack Obama     57.9%
     federal income taxes on households
     earning 250 thousand dollars or
     more per year? Barack Obama, Mitt
     Romney, both, or neither?

10   Which candidate says he will keep      Mitt Romney      55.3%
     the Bush tax cuts in place
     permanently? Barack Obama, Mitt
     Romney, both, or neither?

11   Which candidate favors building the    Mitt Romney      65.5%
     Keystone pipe line without delay?
     Barack Obama, Mitt Romney, both, or
     neither?

12   Mitt Romney has investments in         Accurate         77.3%
     Chinese companies. How accurate do
     you think that statement is? Would
     you say it is very accurate,
     somewhat accurate, not too
     accurate, or not accurate at all?

13   Which candidate supports the Dream     Barack Obama     73.6%
     act which provides a path to
     permanent residence status for
     young undocumented immigrants who
     were brought to this country
     illegally as children. Barack
     Obama, Mitt Romney, both, or
     neither?

14   Which candidate favors eliminating     Mitt Romney      72.9%
     funding for the Public Broadcasting
     Service, also known as PBS? Barack
     Obama, Mitt Romney, both, or
     neither?

15   The Obama administration cut banks     Accurate         63.2%
     out as the middlemen in the student
     loan program and put the money
     saved back into student loans. How
     accurate do you think that
     statement is? Would you say it is
     very accurate, somewhat accurate,
     not too accurate, or not accurate
     at all?

     Based on Neither Contested nor
     Accurate Uncontested Information

16   While in foreign countries, Barack     Not Accurate     30.9%
     Obama has repeatedly apologized for
     America. How accurate do you think
     that statement is? Would you say it
     is very accurate, somewhat
     accurate, not too accurate, or not
     accurate at all?

TABLE 2
Knowledge Items Asked of Third Presidential Debate (Wave 4)

              Question Wording              Correct Answer   % Correct

    Knowledge Based on Contested
    Information

1   While in foreign countries, Barack      Not Accurate     36.7%
    Obama has repeatedly apologized for
    America. How accurate do you think
    that statement is? Would you say it
    is very accurate, somewhat accurate,
    not too accurate, or not accurate at
    all?

2   Mitt Romney's tax plan will increase    Not Accurate     45.1%
    the deficit by over 4 and a half
    trillion dollars. How accurate do
    you think that statement is? Would
    you say it is very accurate,
    somewhat accurate, not too accurate,
    or not accurate at all?

3   Governor Romney wanted to take          Not Accurate     36.0%
    Detroit auto manufacturers into
    bankruptcy without providing any
    form of federal aid. How accurate do
    you think that statement is? Would
    you say it is very accurate,
    somewhat accurate, not too accurate,
    or not accurate at all?

    Knowledge Based on Accurate
    Uncontested Information

4   Which candidate has promised to         Mitt Romney      58.7%
    increase military spending? Barack
    Obama, Mitt Romney, Both, or Neither

5   Which candidate favors branding         Mitt Romney      58.4%
    China as a currency manipulator and
    imposing tariffs on Chinese good?
    Barack Obama, Mitt Romney, Both, or
    Neither?

6   Mitt Romney has investments in          Accurate         73.8%
    Chinese companies. How accurate do
    you think that statement is? Would
    you say it is very accurate,
    somewhat accurate, not too accurate,
    or not accurate at all?

TABLE 3
Effect of Watching the Second Presidential Debate on Knowledge of
Issues and Matters Discussed in the Debate (Wave 3)

                                                         Knowledge
                                                         Based on
                                                         Contested
                                  Total Knowledge       Information

                                    b       std err     b     std err

Amount of Second Presidential    3.98 ***    0.40      0.75    0.61
  Debate Watched
Total News Media Consumption     0.17        0.30     -0.09    0.45
Basic Campaign Knowledge         7.41 ***    0.69      1.03    1.04
Lives in Battleground State      0.21        0.88     -2.18    1.32
Republican                      -1.55        1.15      1.03    1.73
Democrat                         2.69 *      1.11      1.14    1.66
Conservative                    -1.44 ***    0.41      0.33    0.61
Education (years)                0.94 ***    0.19      0.40    0.29
Hispanic                        -1.96        1.78      2.03    2.67
Black                           -1.07        1.53      0.23    2.29
Female                          -4.62 ***    0.89     -1.25    1.33
Age                             -0.08 **     0.03     -0.08    0.04
Constant                        35.04                 34.89
N                               1,108                 1,108
[R.sup.2]                        0.32                  0.02

                                  Knowledge Based
                                    on Accurate
                                    Uncontested
                                    Information

                                    b       std err

Amount of Second Presidential    5.32 ***    0.51
  Debate Watched
Total News Media Consumption     0.25        0.38
Basic Campaign Knowledge        10.33 ***    0.87
Lives in Battleground State      1.04        1.10
Republican                      -1.67        1.44
Democrat                         1.89        1.39
Conservative                    -1.13 *      0.51
Education (years)                1.07 ***    0.24
Hispanic                        -3.63        2.23
Black                           -0.98        1.91
Female                          -6.37 ***    1.11
Age                             -0.08 *      0.04
Constant                        35.00
N                               1,108
[R.sup.2]                        0.34

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Source: Annenberg Public Policy Center's Institutions of Democracy
2012 Political Knowledge Survey.

TABLE 4
Effect of Watching the Third Presidential Debate on Knowledge of
Issues and Matters Discussed in the Debate (Wave 4)

                                                  Knowledge Based on
                                                       Contested
                              Total Knowledge         Information

                                b       std err       b       std err

Amount of Third              5.07 ***    0.48      2 80 ***    0.64
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Total News Media             0.92 *      0.38      0.50        0.51
  Consumption
Basic Campaign Knowledge     5.73 ***    0.93      1.98        1.24
Lives in Battleground        0.10        1.08      0.46        1.45
  State
Republican                   3.22 *      1.43      8.16 ***    1.91
Democrat                    -2.40        1.37     -3.15        1.83
Conservative                 0.62        0.52      1.64 *      0.70
Education [years)            0.90 ***    0.25      1.07 ***    0.33
Hispanic                     0.59        1.94      6.15 *      2.59
Black                       -1.38        1.99     -0.07        2.65
Female                      -3.03 **     1.11     -2.72        1.49
Age                         -0.05        0.03     -0.02        0.05
Constant                    23.28                 11.42
N                           1,112                 1,112
[R.sup.2]                    0.25                  0.10

                            Knowledge Based on
                                 Accurate
                                Uncontested
                                Information

                                b       std err

Amount of Third             7.38 ***     0.73
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Total News Media             1.30 *      0.58
  Consumption
Basic Campaign Knowledge     9.60 ***    1.41
Lives in Battleground       -0.41        1.64
  State
Republican                  -1.79        2.17
Democrat                    -1.45        2.08
Conservative                -0.30        0.79
Education [years)            0.71        0.37
Hispanic                    -5.32        2.94
Black                       -2.79        3.01
Female                      -3.12        1.69
Age                         -0.08        0.05
Constant                    34.74
N                           1,111
[R.sup.2]                    0.22

* p< .05, ** p< .01, *** p < .001.

Source: Annenberg Public Policy Center's Institutions of Democracy
2012 Political Knowledge Survey.

TABLE 5
Models Predicting Knowledge of Issues and Matters Discussed in the
Second Presidential Debate by Debate Viewership and Political
Sophistication (Wave 3)

                                                      Knowledge Based
                                                       on Contested
                                 Total Knowledge        Information

                                   b       std err      b      std err

Amount of Second                4.13 ***    0.53      1.01      0.77
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Political Sophisticate          7.72 ***    2.06      4.56      3.00
Debate Watched *               -0.36        0.87     -1.28      1.27
  Sophisticate
Lives in Battleground State     0.47        0.90     -2.12      1.31
Republican                     -0.70        1.18      1.03      1.72
Democrat                        2.68 *      1.14      1.11      1.66
Conservative                   -1.47 ***    0.42      0.37      0.61
Education (years)               1.35 ***    0.90      0.41      0.28
Hispanic                       -3.36        1.83      1.75      2.67
Black                          -2.67        1.55     -0.08      2.26
Female                         -4.90 ***    0.92     -1.20      1.33
Age                            -0.11 ***    0.03     -0.09 *    0.04
Constant                       39.77                 35.51
N                              1,109                 1,109
[R.sup.2]                       0.28                  0.02

                               Knowledge Based on
                                    Accurate
                                  Uncontested
                                   Information

                                   b       std err

Amount of Second                5.51 ***    0.67
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Political Sophisticate          9.93 ***    2.60
Debate Watched *               -0.26        1.10
  Sophisticate
Lives in Battleground State     1.41        1.14
Republican                     -0.45        1.49
Democrat                        1.89        1.44
Conservative                   -1.19 *      0.53
Education (years)               1.66 ***    0.24
Hispanic                       -5.56 *      2.32
Black                          -3.20        1.97
Female                         -6.79 ***    1.16
Age                            -0.13 ***    0.04
Constant                       41.52
N                              1,109
[R.sup.2]                       0.29

* p< .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Source: Annenberg Public Policy Center's Institutions of Democracy
2012 Political Knowledge Survey.

TABLE 6
Models Predicting Knowledge of Issues and Matters Discussed in the
Third Presidential Debate by Debate Viewership and Political
Sophistication (Wave 4)

                                                Knowledge Based on
                                                     Contested
                            Total Knowledge         Information

                              b       std err       b       std err

Amount of Third            6.04 ***    0.64      3.41 ***    0.85
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Political Sophisticate     7.41 ***    2.21      1.67        2.93
Debate Watched *          -1.39        0.99     -0.44        1.31
  Sophisticate
Lives in Battleground      0.18        1.10      0.77        1.46
  State
Republican                 3.32 *      1.46      8.18 ***    1.93
Democrat                  -2.80 *      1.39     -3.33        1.84
Conservative               0.55        0.53      1.55 *      0.70
Education (years)          1.18 ***    0.24      1.15 ***    0.32
Hispanic                  -0.28        1.96      5.68 *      2.60
Black                     -1.94        1.98      0.25        2.63
Female                    -3.13 **     1.13     -3.00 *      1.49
Age                       -0.05        0.03     -0.01        0.04
Constant                  28.35                 13.91
N                         1,116                 1,116
[R.sup.2]                  0.23                  0.10

                          Knowledge Based on
                               Accurate
                              Uncontested
                              Information

                              b       std err

Amount of Third            8.78 ***    0.98
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Political Sophisticate    13.04 ***    3.35
Debate Watched *          -2.41        1.50
  Sophisticate
Lives in Battleground     -0.45        1.67
  State
Republican                -1.66        2.21
Democrat                  -2.22        2.11
Conservative              -0.40        0.80
Education (years)          1.20 ***    0.37
Hispanic                  -6.63 *      2.98
Black                     -4.29        3.01
Female                    -3.12        1.71
Age                       -0.08        0.05
Constant                  42.54
N                         1,115
[R.sup.2]                  0.19

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Source: Annenberg Public Policy Center's Institutions of Democracy
2012 Political Knowledge Survey.

TABLE 7
Logistic Regression Models Predicting Each Knowledge
Item from Debate Viewing and Candidate Favorability

                                        Debate 2 Items
                                      Based on Contested
                                         Information

                               ([dagger])              2

Amount of Second            0.03      0.00       0.12      0.06
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --      -0.14 ***    --        .08 **
Favorability * Debate        --      -0.02 *      --       0.02 *
  Watch
Constant                   -1.47     -0.44      -0.26     -0.91
N                           1,107     1,096      1,106     1,095
McFadden [R.sup.2]           .14       .24        .10       .16

                                         Debate 2 Items
                                       Based on Contested
                                           Information

                                   3                     4

Amount of Second           -0.05      -0.09       0.03       0.06
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --       -0.06 *      --         .11 ***
Favorability * Debate        --       -0.01        --        0.00
  Watch
Constant                   -0.34       0.28       -0.46     -1.22
N                           1,106      1,095       1,106     1,095
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .08         .10         .09       .14

                                          Debate 2 Items
                                         Based on Accurate
                                      Uncontested Information

                                   5                      6

Amount of Second           0.20 **     0.18 **    0.44 ***    0.47 ***
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --       -0.04        --         0.00
Favorability * Debate        --        0.02 *      --        -0.04 ***
  Watch
Constant                  -0.96       -0.83      -2.11       -1.79
N                          1,107       1,096      1,107       1,096
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .05         .05        .11         .14

                                          Debate 2 Items
                                         Based on Accurate
                                      Uncontested Information

                                   7                      8

Amount of Second            0.12      0.14 *     0 41 ***    0.40 ***
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --       0.11 ***    --         0.00
Favorability * Debate        --       0.00        --         0.01
  Watch
Constant                    0.86      0.18      -1.80       -1.90
N                           1,107     1,097      1,107       1,096
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .08        .11        .11         .11

                                         Debate 2 Items
                                        Based on Accurate
                                     Uncontested Information

                                   9                     10

Amount of Second           0.19 **     0.18 **    0.01       0.00
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --        0.05 *      --       -0.01
Favorability * Debate        --       -0.01        --        0.01
  Watch
Constant                  -1.93       -2.13      -1.23      -1.34
N                          1,107       1,096      1,106      1,095
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .10         .10        .07        .07

                                       Debate 2 Items
                                     Based on Accurate
                                  Uncontested Information

                                  11                     12

Amount of Second            .53 ***    0.54 ***    .46 ***     .48 ***
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --       -0.03        --         0.07 **
Favorability * Debate        --       -0.02        --        -0.01
  Watch
Constant                  -1.59       -1.23       0.87        0.57
N                          1,107       1,096      1,104       1,093
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .25         .26        .08         .09

                                         Debate 2 Items
                                        Based on Accurate
                                     Uncontested Information

                              13 ([dagger])             14

Amount of Second            0.03       0.04     .39 ***    0.37 ***
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --        0.02       --       0.08 **
Favorability * Debate        --        0.01       --      -0.02 *
  Watch
Constant                   -1.74      -1.91    -0.99      -1.08
N                           1,107      1,096    1,103      1,092
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .08         .08      .19        .19

                            Debate 2 Items
                          Based on Accurate
                         Uncontested Information

                                  15

Amount of Second          0.34 ***   0.38 ***
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability      --       0.07 **
Favorability * Debate       --       0.01
  Watch
Constant                  2.16       1.72
N                         1,105      1,094
McFadden [R.sup.2]         .10        .14

                                        Debate 3 Items
                                       Based on Contested
                                          Information

                                   1                   2

Amount of Third            0.16 *    0.10        0.06     0.04
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability      --       0.11 ***      --    -0.13 ***
Favorability * Debate       --       0.05 ***      --    -0.01
  Watch
Constant                  -1.50     -2.59       -2.43    -1.59
N                          1,109     1,103       1,112    1,106
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .25       .36         .18      .25

                             Debate 3 Items
                           Based on Contested
                              Information

                                   3

Amount of Third              .22 ***    0.16 *
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --        -0.09 ***
Favorability * Debate        --        -0.02 *
  Watch
Constant                   -1.91       -1.05
N                           1,109       1,103
McFadden [R.sup.2]           .14         .19

                                        Debate 3 Items
                                       Based on Accurate
                                     Uncontested Information

                                    4                      5

Amount of Third            0.38 ***    0.39 ***   0.42 ***    0 46 ** *
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability      --         0.02        --         0.01
Favorability * Debate       --        -0.01        --        -0.04 ***
  Watch
Constant                  -1.82       -1.78      -1.69       -1.40
N                          1,110       1,104      1,109       1,104
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .12         .12        .12         .12

                              Debate 3 Items
                             Based on Accurate
                          Uncontested Information

                                    6

Amount of Third            0.33 ***   0.37 ***
  Presidential Debate
  Watched
Candidate Favorability       --       0.04 *
Favorability * Debate        --       0.01
  Watch
Constant                   1.57       1.22
N                          1,105      1,099
McFadden [R.sup.2]          .06        .07

* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

Notes: All models control for total news media consumption, basic
campaign knowledge, lives in battleground state, party
identification, political ideology, education, race, gender, and
age.

([dagger)] Question wording for each item can be found in Table
1. The numbers correspond to each respective item in Table 1.

Source: Annenberg Public Policy Center's Institutions of
Democracy 2012 Political Knowledge Survey.

* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

Notes: All models control for total news media consumption, basic
campaign knowledge, lives in battleground state, party
identification, political ideology, education, race, gender, and
age.

([dagger]) Question wording for each item can be found in Tables
1 and 2. The numbers correspond to each respective item in Table
1 for the second presidential debate and 2 for the third
presidential debate.

Source: Annenberg Public Policy Center's Institutions of
Democracy 2012 Political Knowledge Survey.
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有