Rhetorical convergence and issue knowledge in the 2000 presidential election.
Waldman, Paul ; Jamieson, Kathleen Hall
Both of us use similar language to reach an exactly opposite
outcome.
--Al Gore (presidential debate, October 17, 2000)
Citizens often say that all politicians sound the same. While
occasionally used as an excuse for apathy, this complaint captures, at
least in part, a critical characteristic of contemporary political
discourse. This was seldom more true than in the presidential campaign
of 2000. Although the candidates' actual policy positions differed
substantially on dozens of issues, at times it seemed to many that they
were making similar arguments, using similar terms to describe their
policies, and in general converging toward a single set of goals. This
convergence influenced what voters knew and thought about George W. Bush
and Al Gore. It also represented a successful rhetorical strategy on
Bush's part and a failure by the Gore campaign.
The subject of rhetorical ambiguity as a campaign strategy has been
addressed before, with mixed results. While Downs (1957) argued that
ambiguity was advantageous to candidates, a view supported by Page
(1976), Shepsle (1972) attempted to demonstrate that theoretical
conditions exist under which ambiguity results in fewer votes, a view
supported by Alvarez (1998). Campbell (1983) showed that ambiguity is
most effective when the candidate's position is at odds with
majority opinion and when opinion is widely dispersed. Unsurprisingly,
candidates have the greatest incentive to be ambiguous when specificity
would alienate significant numbers of voters.
When candidates are vague, one must surmise their true positions.
Often, voters simply "project" their own views onto their
candidates of choice; instead of looking at their positions and deciding
whom they like, they decide whom they like and then infer that the
candidate's positions mirror their own. The stronger and more
apparent a candidate's ideology, the less likely it is that voters
will project their own views on to that candidate (Conover and Feldman
1989). Unlike some other information shortcuts, such as the use of party
identification to infer issue positions, projection is not inherently
likely to be accurate, although in practice the accuracy of projection
is related to an individual's political sophistication (Krosnick
1990).
But ambiguity as explored by these scholars is not precisely the
same as the rhetorical convergence that could be observed in the
campaign of 2000. Bush and Gore did not simply decline to state where
they stood or issue vague pronouncements potentially compatible with any
policy position. Nor did they adopt a few selected positions associated
with the other party to cloud perceptions of their general ideologies, a
strategy Norpoth and Buchanan (1992) suggest may not be effective.
Rather, they pledged support to a common set of ends--maintaining a
strong military, providing prescription drug coverage, strengthening
social security, and increasing accountability in education--but
differed only on the means by which these ends could be achieved.
Nonetheless, the most common interpretation of the 2000
presidential election is not that of common issue positioning. Instead,
the election is seen as one in which one candidate had an advantage on
issues, while the other had an advantage on personality. Consequently,
Bush's victory may be explained by, as The Baltimore Sun put it,
"negative attitudes toward [Gore] personally, which seem to be
negating his perceived advantage on issues" (West 2000, A1).
Although there were many factors contributing to the election's
final outcome, the premise of the explanation--that Al Gore had the
advantage on issues--has not been challenged. In contrast, we will argue
that Gore's "advantage" on issues was substantially
mitigated by the public's lack of knowledge and misperception, to
the point of being no advantage at all. First, many and in some cases
most voters were not aware of where the candidates stood. Second, partly
because Gore's positions were in fact more popular, when people
made mistakes in attributing issue positions to the candidates, the
results helped Bush and hurt Gore. The unnoticed factor in the 2000
election was thus not uninformed opinion but misinformed opinion. As
others have noted, the two are conceptually distinct and have differing
kinds of consequences, although both can be substantial (Dalager 1996;
Gilens 2001; Kuklinski et al. 2000).
The rhetoric of the two candidates did not serve to reduce voter
confusion. One explanation for candidates' rhetorical choices can
be found in the spatial theory of voting originated by Downs (1957),
which in its simplest form states that each voter selects the candidate
closest to his or her favored policy positions, and therefore the
rational decision for the candidate (depending on what the opponent
does) is to place himself or herself at the median point of voter
opinion, thereby maximizing the number of votes he or she will receive.
Bill Clinton's "triangulation" strategy for the 1996
election, in which he positioned himself between his own party and the
Republicans, is a near-perfect example of the spatial theory in action
(King 1997).
Although the spatial theory and associated rational choice analyses
have been extensively critiqued in recent years, particularly for their
failure to make accurate empirical predictions (see Green and Shapiro
1994), in 2000 Al Gore and George W. Bush appeared to conduct
themselves--rhetorically if not in policy--as though they were following
the prescriptions of the spatial theory. In many cases, they placed
themselves on the popular side of the issue in question; in others, they
cloaked discussion of unpopular positions in terms that might have
convinced many paying less than perfect attention that their positions
were something they were not. This strategy was particularly attractive
to George W. Bush, who appeared to implement it successfully.
Rhetorical choices make it possible for candidates to cloud
understanding of unpopular positions through the use of what Jacobs and
Shapiro (2000) call "crafted talk." They argue that a key
element of policy making is the effort by political actors to move
public opinion by attaching popular words and symbols to policies that
may begin with little popular support, resulting in a politics that
appears far more responsive to public opinion than it actually is.
Although Jacobs and Shapiro are concerned more with policy debates than
with the positioning of candidates during campaigns, their analysis goes
a substantial way toward explaining both the campaign rhetoric of 2000
and the shape of voter awareness of Bush and Gore.
Most political professionals believe that each presidential
campaign revolves around no more than three issues. If one candidate can
shift the agenda to issues on which his position or record is more
popular than that of his opponent, he stands a good chance of winning
the election. Changing people's minds on an issue is difficult, as
political actors are well aware (Jacobs and Shapiro 2000); it is far
easier to shift public attention and prime a certain set of issues
(Iyengar and Kinder 1987). Campaigns thus attempt to heighten the
salience of some issues and reduce that of others. For example, the
famous 1992 Clinton campaign mantra, "It's the economy,
stupid," was an instruction for the candidate and his aides to keep
the focus on an area in which voters felt he would do a better job than
his opponent. The Bush campaign in turn attempted to heighten the
salience of candidate character, on which they felt they had an
advantage.
Although candidates spend much of their time competing to set the
agenda, many citizens believe that politicians adopt positions that they
believe are popular but that do not reflect conviction. Bald dishonesty of this sort is actually uncommon. More often, candidates simply shift
attention or change the terms they use to describe their positions. The
complicating factor is that because they poll relentlessly, both
campaigns know the public's priorities. As Jacobs and Shapiro
(2000) put it, "The irony of contemporary politics is that
politicians both slavishly track public opinion and, contrary to the
myth of `pandering,' studiously avoid simply conforming policy to
what the public wants" (pp. 7-8).
In 2000, as in years past, the candidates denied that polls
mattered to them. When asked about the use of polls, Gore said that
"I don't think they have all that much meaning." Bush
concurred, "I don't base my decisions on what polls and focus
groups say" (NBC Nightly News, October 6, 2000). Of course, this
professed lack of faith in polls did not stop either candidate from
mounting an extensive polling operation.
In 2000, to a large extent the campaigns did not compete to
determine the issue agenda but instead simply took their cues from what
their polls told them were the issues of greatest concern to the public.
In the 2000 National Annenberg Election Survey (NAES) (1)--the study on
which the data in this article are based--as well as those conducted by
other organizations such as Gallup, education consistently topped the
list of issues Americans found most pressing. While social security and
health care ranked somewhat lower, they were of intense concern to the
age group with the highest voter turnout: senior citizens. These issues
became the primary focus of both candidates.
A look at the candidates' advertising shows the extent to
which they had nearly identical agendas. To analyze these agendas, we
divided each ad into a series of claims and then recorded the issue
being discussed in each. Education, prescription drugs for seniors, and
social security made up three of the top four issues for each candidate.
These three accounted for more than 40 percent of the claims in
Gore's ads and more than 50 percent in Bush's.
Had the candidates offered clear distinctions between their goals
and plans on these issues, the rhetorical convergence in their
advertising might have been beneficial to voters. Unfortunately, most of
the ads were not specific enough to allow viewers to make comparative
judgments. Witness the following two ads about education. If one simply
reads the transcripts, it is difficult to tell which candidate is
speaking:
Candidate A: Strengthening education begins with us, not with
government. Parents simply have to get involved and take responsibility
to make sure that their children study and learn. But government has to
take responsibility for what it can do. Fix failing schools, reduce the
class size, set high standards for students and for teachers. We are in
an information age. We have a responsibility to make sure that our
schools are the very best in the entire world.
Candidate B: Our parents and our schools and our citizens ought to
have high expectations for every child.
Announcer: [Candidate B]'s education plan is hailed as
remarkable for its bold conservatism.
Candidate B: The best way to have the best school system is to have
programs emanate from the local level.
Announcer: [Candidate B]'s plan calls for teaching character
in the classroom. His plan holds schools accountable, blows the whistle
on failure, gives parents more choices, and focuses on teaching all
children to read.
Candidate B: Every parent wants their child to get the very best.
Candidate A asserts that strengthening education does not begin
with government, while Candidate B wants programs that emanate from the
local level. Candidate A wants high standards, while Candidate B favors
accountability. Candidate A wants to fix failing schools, while
Candidate B wants to blow the whistle on failure. Although the use of
the phrase "bold conservatism" should tip off the reader that
the first candidate is Al Gore and the second George Bush, it is
difficult for a viewer to see precisely what one would do differently
than the other.
One might view the fact that the candidates settled on a common
agenda as a positive development: while they might have advanced their
own agendas, the candidates addressed the public agenda. If they had
been able to do so in a way that allowed most voters to understand the
distinctions between them, the case for that conclusion might be
compelling. But by November 7, many voters remained unsure of precisely
how George Bush's education or Medicare plans differed from Al
Gore's. Although Bush favored school vouchers, the word hardly ever
passed his lips, perhaps because the word may be less popular than the
idea. Nearly half of those we interviewed immediately after the campaign
were unaware that Bush supported vouchers. The previous Bush education
ad says that he favors "giving parents more choices," but this
could forecast any number of policies, some looked on more favorably by
voters than others. Indeed, it could mean support for choice within the
public school system, a position favored by Gore.
In the final debate, moderator Jim Lehrer asked Bush directly,
"Governor, what is your position on vouchers?" Bush responded,
First of all, vouchers are up to states. If you want to do a
voucher program in Missouri, fine. See, I strongly believe in
local control of schools.... When we find children trapped in
schools that will not change and will not teach, instead of saying,
"Oh, this is okay in America--just to shuffle poor kids through
schools"--there has to be a consequence. And the consequence is
that federal portion of federal money will go to the parent so the
parent can go to a tutoring program or another public school or
another private--or a private school. (October 17, 2000)
Bush says that "vouchers are up to states" and then
describes a federal voucher program he would support. This was also one
of the only times during the campaign that Bush mentioned his support of
vouchers for private schools. His Web site articulated the position this
way:
Failing schools will be given a finite period to change; if they
fail to do so, children of low-income parents will have the option
of transferring to another public school or using their share of
federal funds to pay for another option (tutoring, charter school,
etc.).
The "etc." refers to private schools, which lie at the
heart of the voucher controversy. (2)
Senior Citizens: Target 1
Although senior citizens are often courted in campaigns, Bush and
Gore pleaded for their votes to an unusual degree for a presidential
campaign. In their advertising, one-fifth of the claims made by Bush and
one-quarter of the claims made by Gore concerned prescription drug
coverage for seniors, Medicare, or social security. One Bush ad that
excerpted his convention speech could easily have been offered by Hubert
Humphrey or Walter Mondale:
Bush: We will strengthen social security and Medicare for the
greatest generation and for generations to come. I believe great
decisions are made with care, made with conviction. We will make
prescription drugs available and affordable for every senior who needs
them. You earned your benefits. You made your plans. And President
George W. Bush will keep the promise of social security, no changes, no
reductions, no way.
Health care was an area in which the candidates' differences
were particularly hard to discern. For instance, one of Bush's ads
stated that "while Washington deadlocked, he delivered a
patient's bill of rights that's a model for America."
Although it took credit for a bill Bush had actually opposed, he made
the claim in the first debate as well. Sounding a similar note, Gore
said he was "leading the fight for a patient's bill of
rights." Whereas Gore in his ads promised to "give control
back to patients and doctors, not HMO bureaucrats," Bush accused
Gore of forcing seniors into a "government HMO" (i.e.,
Medicare). Both said they favored a patient's bill of rights; both
criticized HMOs. The fact that their plans differed radically was
virtually impossible to discern from their advertising. By the
race's end, the voters had a slightly better understanding of
Gore's position than Bush's: about half of those we
interviewed knew that Gore favored a Medicare benefit, while around a
third correctly identified Bush's position. Only slightly more than
a quarter of Americans correctly identified both positions, meaning that
three-quarters were somewhat confused about the candidates'
prescription drug proposals. A Pew Research Center (2000) poll in
October showed that 59 percent of the public thought there were
important differences between Bush's and Gore's prescription
drug plans. According to the NAES, however, most people did not quite
know what those differences were.
Although Bush may have engaged in the greater amount of crafted
talk, at times Al Gore mimicked the Texas governor's language as
well. For instance, anyone reading the following excerpt from an October
24 speech would conclude that it must have been Bush talking:
I'm opposed to big government.... I'm for a smaller, smarter
government, one that serves people better, but offers real change
and gives more choices to our families.... I don't believe there's
a government solution to every problem. I don't believe any
government program can replace the responsibility of parents, the
hard work of families or the innovation of industry.
In fact, the speaker was Al Gore.
Since Democrats tend to be more supportive of government and
Republicans less so, the language they use to advocate their candidacies
usually differs. One analysis of a half-century's worth of
presidential campaign speeches showed that as a general rule, Democrats
talk about policy while Republicans talk about principles (Jarvis 1999).
Like many of his Republican predecessors, George W. Bush spent a good
deal of time criticizing the federal government in general terms,
despite his plans for expanded spending on a number of programs. As the
above example illustrates, when the Gore campaign surmised that
Bush's mantra, "He trusts government, I trust the
people," was beginning to stick, Gore distanced himself from
"big government" without dropping any of his proposals for
government spending.
As the campaign neared its end, the question of who was more
opposed to government became more and more muddled. In their first
debate, Gore adopted Bush's formulation on the issue of abortion:
"He trusts the government to order a woman to do what it thinks she
ought to do. I trust women to make the decisions that affect their
lives, their destinies, and their bodies." In their final debate,
Gore said, "During the last five years, Texas' government has
gone up in size. The federal government has gone down. Texas'
government has gone up. Now, my plan for the future, I see a time when
we have smaller, smarter government." Gore's general
philosophy about the size and scope of government was roughly the same
as most in his party, but his rhetoric might have led some to believe
that he was in favor of substantial reductions in the role of
government. The voter was left with two competing claims: Bush said Gore
loves government; Gore said Bush loves government. Instead of a debate
on the appropriate size of government, voters were offered competing
accusations. Each candidate claimed that the other was insufficiently
devoted to a goal that was lauded by both.
Despite its paradoxical (some might say hypocritical) character,
the stance that both candidates adopted--being against government but
for government programs--mirrored the contradiction in American public
opinion. For many years, surveys have found that if asked whether
government should be bigger or smaller, large majorities will respond
"smaller." But when asked about specific programs, similarly
large majorities will say that the government should spend more money,
whether on the environment, the military, health care, or any of a wide
array of programs. The exception is programs that offer benefits to poor
Americans, where majorities generally say the government should spend
less (Cantril and Cantril 1999). Consequently, railing against big
government while offering to spend more government money is an effective
strategy. Although Bush spent more time criticizing Washington and Gore
spent more time talking about government programs, both candidates
embraced the contradiction to a certain extent.
This is not to say that the candidates never added specific details
to their plans-they did, in position papers and speeches. But the
specifics never reached the majority of American voters. When a
candidate gives a thirty-minute speech, complete with policy details,
reporters select one or two quotations to feature in their stories. The
quotations selected are seldom those that provide the most lucid
explanation of a policy position. Instead, they are likely to be the
harshest attacks the candidate offered that day, regardless of whether
criticism of his opponent composed 99 percent or 1 percent of what he
had to say. Our research on the past twenty years of elections has shown
that the news consistently overrepresents the amount of attack present
in candidate discourse, whether the campaign is predominantly made up of
attack, contrast, or advocacy. In addition, the dominant motif of
strategy or "horse race" news involves the truncation of issue
argumentation in ways that make issue distinctions more difficult for
voters to discern (Jamieson and Waldman 1997).
Even when a reporter makes a good faith effort to find quotations
that accurately represent a candidate's speech, he or she will
select statements that synopsize the candidate's arguments. In
2000, the candidates took many identical positions on the broad goals of
policy. The distinctions, though substantial, lay in the details.
Synopsizing quotations tend to describe the broader goals. When the
details failed to pass through the news filter, voters learned only that
both candidates wanted to protect social security, offer prescription
drug coverage to seniors, or support a patient's bill of rights.
The means by which they intended to accomplish these goals differed
radically--but it was difficult for voters to understand how.
As a consequence, despite the closeness of the 2000 race, the
distinctions were less clear than they had been in previous elections.
In 1996, we conducted a survey in which people were asked about Bill
Clinton's and Bob Dole's positions on various issues. A
comparison of those data with data from 2000 shows that despite the
common belief that 1996 was a "boring" election to which
voters paid little attention, distinctions between Clinton and Dole on
many issues were substantially clearer than distinctions between Gore
and Bush.
For instance, on the issue of abortion, immediately following the
1996 debates fully 80 percent of those we surveyed accurately judged
that Bob Dole favored making it harder for women to obtain abortions,
while less than 14 percent thought that Bill Clinton favored such
restrictions. At a similar time in 2000, only 64.1 percent knew that
George W. Bush favored restricting abortion, while 25.5 percent
erroneously thought that Al Gore agreed. (3)
One area in which the distinctions were clearer was the death
penalty. While a similarly small number were aware of Clinton and
Gore's support for the death penalty-36 percent and 41.5 percent,
respectively-many more were aware of Bush's support in 2000 (78.3
percent) than Dole's in 1996 (53 percent). Bush, of course, is a
strong advocate of the death penalty whose state executed more prisoners
than under any governor since the death penalty was reinstated in 1976.
During the campaign, many news stories were written about the case of
Gary Graham, an inmate for whom Bush refused to grant a stay of
execution despite questions about his guilt.
When exploring the effects of candidate rhetoric on public
impressions, one must keep in mind that there is not a single entity
called "the public" whose members receive an identical body of
information. A small number of people are familiar with the entire scope
of the candidates' records and issue positions, but most pick up
their information in drips and drabs as the campaign progresses. For
many, the simplified messages of ads and sound bites are the only
exposure they get. In addition, many of the issue positions that
consistently distinguish the two major parties--and that elites take for
granted--are not understood by a significant proportion of the
electorate. Even the relatively simple question asked by the National
Election Studies (NES), "Would you say that either one of the
parties is more conservative than the other at the national level? Which
party is more conservative?" has never been answered correctly by
more than 60 percent of respondents. Ideally, presidential campaigns
should provide a quadrennial instruction in these important issue
distinctions.
Most voters are exposed to the visible rhetoric--not the policy
papers on the candidates' Web sites or the text of their economic
plans but the content of their ads, what they say in debates, and the
mantras they repeat on the stump. In 2000, this rhetoric made it
particularly difficult to distinguish between the candidates.
Since 1952, the NES have asked voters, "Do you think there are
any important differences in what the Republicans and Democrats stand
for?" Prior to 2000, the years in which the most people, 63
percent, answered in the affirmative were 1984 and 1996, elections in
which a strong economy propelled an incumbent to a relatively easy
victory. The number seeing a difference between the parties has been
about 60 percent in presidential election years but falls below 50
percent in off-year congressional elections, indicating that for a
significant number of people, a perception of party differences is not a
consistent part of their understanding of politics but is rather
something that is clarified to a greater or lesser extent by
presidential campaigns. In 2000, the number of NES respondents answering
that there were important differences between Democrats and Republicans
reached a new high of 70 percent. Similarly, polling by the Pew Research
Center (2000) found that 61 percent of voters felt that "George W.
Bush and Al Gore take different positions on the issues." In an era
in which partisan conflict in Washington is both a reality and a
consistent storyline of news--and in the midst of a hard-fought
presidential campaign--the fact that most people would perceive a
difference between the parties is unsurprising. Less encouraging is the
degree to which they were able to discern the content of that
difference.
The Gore campaign's pollsters asked a related question about
the candidates themselves. They asked a national sample the following
question: "Both Al Gore and George W. Bush proposed plans on
education, a patient's bill of rights, and prescription drugs for
seniors. On these, which [plans are better]?" Exactly half of the
respondents chose that either Gore's plans were better or
Bush's were better. The other half could not decide between them,
with 21 percent of the total sample admitting that "it is too
confusing to figure out whose proposals are better" (Jamieson and
Waldman 2001).
At any given point in the campaign, a similar number to that found
in Gore's polling--between 15 percent and 20 percent of the people
the NAES interviewed--placed the candidates at the same spot on an
ideological scale. About half of those rated both candidates as
"moderate," while a third of them rated both as
"conservative." Seeing candidates as the same has a strong
negative relationship with other variables associated with political
interest and knowledge. For instance, as the campaign came to a close,
23.5 percent of our respondents who did not finish high school placed
both candidates in the same ideological position, compared to just 9.5
percent of college graduates.
During the campaign, George W. Bush spent time and effort
addressing issues that in the past have been "owned" by the
Democrats, with two results. The first was that given the lack of depth
with which most issues were discussed, the candidates looked more
alike--both wanted to improve education, protect social security, and
preserve Medicare. The second effect was that Bush blunted what might
have been some of Gore's strongest advantages on issues. Polling
done by The New York Times and CBS late in the campaign showed that
while more people trusted Gore to do a better job than Bush on education
and social security, the gap was extremely small. Both campaigns were
well aware of the progress Bush had made on these issues. During the
Annenberg postelection debriefing, Gore pollster Stan Greenberg reported
the struggles the Gore campaign had:
On questions of social insurance, like HMO reform, prescription
drugs, we won those issues in the end, though we had to battle
them. We paid a high price for that, but not on the issue itself.
We beat George Bush on prescription drugs and on Social Security,
but Bush looked like he wanted to do something. He didn't look like
a man with bad intentions. It also put us on seniors' issues for a
much longer period than we wanted to be.... On education, we won by
four, which was clearly unacceptable as a Democrat. (Jamieson and
Waldman 2001, 95-96)
Bush advertising consultant Mark McKinnon agreed:
When you looked at the issue matrix on this election, all the
issues that people cared about were typically Democratic issues:
education, Social Security, health care. So, we knew that while we
probably couldn't win on those issues, we had to at least keep them
close. Fortunately we had a candidate who had been talking about
those issues, not just in this campaign, but for years as governor
in Texas. So there was a platform there, and a history. Our
strategy was to stay close on those issues. (Jamieson and Waldman
2001, 145-46)
The result was an election in which most voters perceived a
difference between the candidates and the parties they represented, but
when it came to issues, they were not sure exactly what those
differences entailed. Whereas in previous elections each candidate
carved out an area of specificity on his strongest issues and remained
vague on those where his opponent was strong, in 2000 both candidates
focused on the same agenda. When distinctions on the issues on that
agenda were blurred, means of contrast became harder for voters to find.
Lack of Knowledge Benefited Bush More than Gore
Many postelection journalistic analyses concluded that while Gore
had an advantage on the issues, voters found Bush to be the more
appealing personality. Although our data support the second half of this
conclusion, they also show that Gore's advantage on issues was
substantially undercut by voter misperception. Despite the fact that
Gore's positions on a variety of issues were more popular than
Bush's, many voters were not aware that they agreed with Gore.
The NAES asked respondents to indicate where the candidates stood
on a variety of policies not only to test their knowledge of the
candidates but also to discover how closely their perceptions matched
reality. By combining the answers to these questions with those we asked
about the respondents' own beliefs, we can assess not only how many
voters agreed with each candidate's actual positions but how many
voters agreed with what they believed to be each candidate's
positions. When we do so, we discover not only that many people
misunderstood where the candidates stood but that in most cases this
misperception worked to Bush's benefit and Gore's detriment.
Figures 1 and 2 show first the percentage of voters who placed
themselves and the candidate on the same side of the issue in question,
regardless of whether they were correct about the candidate; this is
termed perceived agreement. Second, they display the percentage whose
opinion matched the actual position that the candidate took during the
campaign. To focus on what voters had learned by the end of the
campaign, these figures show only data from the final stage of the
campaign, after the last debate on October 17.
[FIGURES 1-2 OMITTED]
On five of the nine issues, the percentage of voters who believed
they agreed with Bush exceeded the percentage who actually agreed with
him. The discrepancy was more than thirty points on the question of
requiring a license to purchase a handgun and more than twenty-five
points on the issue of providing health insurance to all children. The
only issues where substantially more people actually agreed with Bush
than perceived they agreed with him were two of his more popular
positions, support for the death penalty and allowing workers to invest
a portion of their social security contributions in the stock market. In
both cases, however, the discrepancy did not exceed fifteen percentage
points.
For Gore, on the other hand, the picture is far different. On all
nine issues, more people actually agreed with Gore than knew or believed
they did. (4) In a few cases, the differences are substantial. While 73
percent of voters shared Gore's support for the death penalty, only
38 percent thought they agreed with him (and some of these were death
penalty opponents who thought Gore shared their view). Only 43 percent
of voters thought they agreed with Gore on the question of whether
citizens should have the ability to sue their HMOs, although 75 percent
did in fact agree with him. On these nine issues, perceived agreement
with Gore lagged behind actual agreement by an average of seventeen
percentage points.
The net result was that Gore's "advantage" on the
issues, if understood not as the popularity of his positions but as the
proportion of voters who believed they agreed with him, was virtually
nonexistent. On these nine issues, the popularity of Gore's
position exceeded the popularity of Bush's by an average of
twenty-four points (61 percent to 37 percent) during this final period
of the campaign. But on perceived agreement, the candidates were almost
equal, with an average of 42 percent of voters believing they agreed
with Bush on any given issue, compared to 44 percent who believed they
agreed with Gore. While in many of these cases an alternate question
wording might have generated slightly different results, the overall
pattern is unmistakable. The accepted interpretation of the
campaign--that Gore led on the issues but Bush was better liked
personally--turns out to be false. Although it was true that voters
found Bush's personality more appealing, Gore did not hold any
appreciable advantage on issues. These data demonstrate that the story
of political knowledge in the 2000 election was not so much about
uninformed opinion but about misinformed opinion.
The question is not so much why Americans did not have a better
understanding of Gore's positions but why they did not have a
better understanding of both candidates' positions. There were, of
course, some issues that were better understood than others. For
instance, 77 percent knew that Bush favored the death penalty. As we
noted, the application of the death penalty in Texas was the topic of
many news stories and late-night jokes during the campaign. In contrast,
only 37 percent knew where Bush stood on gun licenses, which was not a
frequent topic of discussion. Overall, the number of respondents who
answered that they did not know where Gore stood was identical to the
number who said the same about Bush: an average of 23.5 percent
responded that they did not know where Gore stood on a given issue,
compared to 23.4 percent for Bush. Furthermore, as many people gave the
wrong response when citing Bush's position (25.4 percent) as they
did when citing Gore's (23.5 percent). But when these errors were
made, they were more likely to result in an increase in perceived
agreement for Bush and a decrease in perceived agreement for Gore (see
Figure 3).
[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]
To assess the informational environment that could have produced
such errors, we performed a content analysis of every story about the
presidential campaign that appeared on the three broadcast network news
shows between July 1, 2000, and election day. Each story was divided
into a series of statements by or about the candidates, and among other
variables, we recorded whether the statement mentioned any issue. Fewer
than a third of the statements in television news mentioned an issue at
all. Included in this figure are any mentions of an issue, no matter how
brief. Although the networks did air occasional in-depth explorations of
campaign issues, these stories made up a small portion of the total news
about the campaign. More typical was coverage similar to the following,
from NBC Nightly News on October 9:
Tom Brokaw: NBC News in-depth tonight, presidential politics. And
with the election now just a month away, the Gore campaign is going on
the offensive, perhaps because of several days of attacks by the Bush
camp. The latest polls show this race tightening to a dead heat. The
MSNBC/Reuters daily tracking poll had Gore with a five-point lead over
George W. Bush less than a week ago. Today Gore's lead is down to
one point now. That's well inside the margin of error.
For the Gore campaign, there's nothing fuzzy about this kind
of math. NBC's Claire Shipman begins tonight's in-depth
reporting with the Gore campaign's new emphasis on what it calls
Governor Bush's mistakes and missteps.
Claire Shipman: Al Gore picking up some light reading material
during a break from his debate preparations. But while he stays sunny
and above the fray, his campaign unleashes a torrent of negative
firepower against George W. Bush today, pointing up his campaign
bloopers, as Gore staffers call them, and attacking his record as Texas
governor on everything from health care to the environment.
Joe Andrew (Democratic National Committee [DNC]): He says he wants
to be judged by his own record; we're going to make sure he's
judged by his own record.
Shipman: The Democratic National Committee says Joe Lieberman will
head to Texas after Wednesday's debate for a, quote, "failed
leadership tour," and it's spending several million dollars on
three biting new ads in battleground states.
Excerpt from Gore ad: Now take a deep breath and imagine Seattle
with Bush's Texas-style environmental regulation. George Bush:
before he talks about cleaning up Washington, maybe he should clean up
Texas.
Shipman: Why the sudden assault when the public says in poll after
poll it's not in the mood for negativity? Because the Gore team
believes it has to counter Bush's relentless attacks on the vice
president's credibility, which it fears are working.
George W. Bush: The voters are going to have to draw their own
conclusion about somebody who seems to have a consistent pattern of
exaggerating.
Shipman: And the same message on the Bush Web site, "The Gore
Files: Anything to Get Elected." The DNC counters with "Bush
Light: Less Leadership, Less Experience, More Right-Wing Flavor."
Will any of this work?
Larry Sabato (University of Virginia): Negative campaigning almost
always works to some degree. It's bound to add a point or two to
the side that's most--most effective at it.
Shipman: The Gore team so far is trying to have it both ways.
Candidate Gore is staying positive, while his running mate, his aides,
and his ads do the dirty work. And Bush doing some of the jabbing
himself clearly hopes his attacks will hurt his opponent more than they
hurt him. Claire Shipman, NBC News, Sarasota.
Political discourse often involves inductive argument, moving from
specific facts to a general conclusion. In this case, the Gore campaign
cites specific environmental problems in Texas, argues that they
constitute a failure on George Bush's part, then concludes that he
is therefore unprepared to assume the presidency. However, the NBC News
report ignores the specifics cited by both sides to focus on the fact
that they are attacking each other and to explain the strategy behind
the attacks. We learn where the attacks are taking place (Web sites,
advertisements), and a political science professor explains the efficacy
of attacking. But a citizen looking for information to assist him or her
in casting her vote would have trouble finding anything useful in this
report. One study comparing network news coverage of the past four
presidential elections found that 71 percent of stories in 2000 were
primarily concerned with the "horse race" as opposed to
issues, compared to 48 percent in 1996 and 58 percent in both 1992 and
1988 (Lichter 2001).
When an issue is relatively uncomplicated and the positions of the
candidates unambiguous, only a minimum of explanation and repetition on
the part of the news is necessary for most of the electorate to
appreciate the differences between the candidates. Gore was pro-choice,
and Bush was pro-life; even though abortion was not a topic of frequent
discussion, each candidate's position was understood by
approximately two-thirds of voters. But when their proposals are
complex, the differences subtle, the details arcane, and the candidates
pledging support to the same goals, news has an extra responsibility to
confront the issue in depth and repeatedly. In 2000, the
news--particularly television news--largely failed to do so.
For issue positions to be recalled by voters, they have to be
repeated by the candidates and the news. Just as surveys such as ours
show that most voters do not know most of a candidate's issue
positions at the end of the campaign, experiments have demonstrated that
voters quickly forget issue information unless they are exposed to it
repeatedly (Lodge, Steenbergen, and Brau 1995). The reluctance of
journalists to repeat information they have already reported--in other
words, the need for the news to be new--hampers the ability of voters to
make choices based on issues. Whether Gore would have won the election
had he been more aggressive in highlighting the issue positions on which
he aligned with a majority of the public is difficult to say, but his
rhetoric does not appear to have been successful.
Ideological Perceptions
Ideology provides clues to how the candidate will act in the future
and what positions he will take on issues that have yet to emerge. As a
consequence, ideological assessments provide a window into the more
general conclusions people make about candidates. We asked our survey
respondents to rate the candidates on an ideological scale running from
very liberal to very conservative. The results indicate that many
people--particularly Republicans--viewed the candidates through their
own ideological lenses. While Democrats and Republicans both placed Bush
at nearly the same point on the ideological scale (with Republicans
actually rating him as slightly more conservative), there were stark
differences in how people rated Gore. Forty percent of Democrats thought
Gore was moderate, while only 6 percent described him as very liberal.
Republicans, on the other hand, viewed Gore as much farther to the left.
Only 18 percent of Republicans rated him as moderate, while 32 percent
described him as very liberal.
For Bush, the differences were much slighter. Seventeen percent of
Democrats rated Bush very conservative, compared to 8 percent of
Republicans. This means that the ratio of Democrats to Republicans
placing Bush at the extreme of the ideological scale was just more than
2:1, while the ratio of Republicans to Democrats situating Gore at the
extreme was 5.5:1. Among the most intense partisans--those describing
themselves as strong Democrats or strong Republicans--the differences
were clear as well. Twenty-one percent of strong Democrats described
Bush as very conservative, while twice as many strong Republicans (43
percent) described Gore as very liberal. In sum, Democrats viewed Bush
in much less extreme terms than Republicans viewed Gore (see Figure 4).
[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]
This is not a phenomenon unique to the 2000 election. In most
presidential campaigns dating back to 1972 (the first for which NES data
on this question are available), voters across the political spectrum
have viewed the Republican candidate in roughly the same ideological
terms. The sole exception is Ronald Reagan in 1984, whom Republicans
actually saw as substantially more conservative than other voters did.
In every other case, it is when they evaluate the Democratic candidate
that partisans diverge. Republicans always perceive the Democratic
candidate as much more liberal than Democrats and independents perceive
him to be. Bill Clinton is the clearest case: while Democrats and
independents placed him at about the same ideological position as most
other Democratic candidates, in 1996 strong Republicans thought Bill
Clinton was more liberal than previous strong Republicans had found
Michael Dukakis, Walter Mondale, and even George McGovern. In 2000, Bush
voters were slightly more enthusiastic about their candidate than Gore
voters were about theirs, but Bush voters' views of Gore were
substantially less favorable than Gore voters' views of Bush. Brady
and Sniderman (1985) observe that "liberals do not like
conservatives; however, they do not dislike them nearly as much as
conservatives dislike liberals" (p. 1066). Bill Clinton inspired
unusual anger and dislike among conservatives; for many of them,
"very liberal" may have been synonymous with "immoral." It is also true that conservatives perceive
liberals generally--not just Democratic politicians--to be farther to
the left on issues than they in fact are (Sniderman, Brody, and Tetlock
1991).
When the principal contenders converge on a virtually
indistinguishable rhetoric that masks the differences in their policy
agendas, there can be negative consequences for governing, as George W.
Bush discovered in the first few months of his presidency. In the wake
of criticism over some of his cabinet nominees, Bush offered this
defense two days before his inauguration in an interview with Brit Hume on Fox News:
The reason I sit here able to talk to you as a person soon to be
sworn as president is because of the positions I took in the
campaign. I was basically running against an incumbent. He had a
pretty good economy going for him. And the world was kind of at
peace. And I wish I could say it was my charming personality or the
ability to string a couple of sentences together. The truth of the
matter is that I am sitting here because I took firm positions on
important issues and didn't back off. And I am not backing off the
minute I arrive in Washington. Quite the contrary. I'm going to
take those issues I campaigned on and campaign hard for their
enactment because I believe it's the right thing for the country.
(Fox News, January 18, 2001)
Despite Bush's protestations, citizens were largely unclear
about his policy agenda. Nonetheless, there is little indication they
translated that confusion into a more comprehensive judgment about
Bush's ideology. If Bush's rhetoric had been entirely
successful, we would expect that voters would have perceived him as more
moderate than they had perceived past Republican presidential
candidates. In fact, this was not the case. When we use NES data to
compare ideological ratings of Bush to those of previous Republican
candidates (the NES has asked this question since 1972), we see that
Bush appears to have been perceived quite similarly to those his party
had nominated in the past. The variance between the mean ratings is
extremely small, ranging from a low of 4.8 (Nixon 1972) to a high of 5.3
(Reagan 1984) on a 7-point scale, with higher numbers being more
conservative. Bush falls in the middle of his Republican counterparts,
with a mean rating of 5.0, equivalent to a rating of slightly
conservative.
Although the candidates' rhetoric did not translate into
visible differences in the assessments voters made of their overall
ideologies compared to that of previous candidates, and despite the
similarity between Bush's positions and those of previous
Republican nominees, the idea that Bush was a "different kind of
Republican" became a major theme of campaign coverage. Early in the
campaign, a study by the Project for Excellence in Journalism (2000)
showed that fully 40 percent of comments about Bush in the press
concerned the idea that he was "a different kind of
Republican." This image of Bush resulted not from any difference in
the issue positions he adopted but from his issue agenda decisions and
stylistic differences in his campaign, including frequent appearances
with members of minority groups. The Republican convention in
Philadelphia featured so many African-American speakers that one
conservative commentator compared the gathering to a Utah Jazz
basketball game: an all-white audience in the stands watching black
performers (Brooks 2000).
Conclusion
Does rhetorical convergence matter? If presidential campaigns were
merely sound and fury, signifying nothing, then the answer would be no.
But in fact, campaigns serve a number of vital functions. As Hart (2000)
put it, "Every four years, the American nation reconstitutes
itself, thereby giving its citizens an opportunity to reflect on who
they are and what they want to become" (p. 8). Campaigns do more
than choose leaders; each is a vehicle for the citizenry to assess the
past and set an agenda for the future.
Although citizens may not have made a summary judgment about
Bush's ideology, some reporters did. When Bush began to govern, he
received criticism in some quarters for implementing a conservative
policy agenda that seemed at odds with the rhetoric of the campaign.
"The reviews are in," said Tucker Carlson on CNN's
Crossfire one month after Bush's inauguration. "George W. Bush
is more conservative than expected" (February 20, 2001). The
problem was not that Bush was implementing an agenda that differed from
the positions he took during the campaign but rather that his agenda was
at odds with the rhetoric through which it was presented.
During a campaign, a candidate attempts to reduce a complicated
policy position to a single message (perhaps one that has a few
different elements). But when policies are implemented, citizens can see
effects that may or may not correlate precisely with the rhetoric used
to justify them. While in a campaign rhetoric usually overwhelms policy
details, in the process of legislating and governing, there is time for
details to receive more examination. When a CBS/New York Times poll
conducted in mid-July 2001 asked respondents if they believed Bush was a
"different kind of Republican" or a "typical
Republican," 63 percent answered that he was a typical Republican
(Berke and Elder 2001).
These data suggest some of the difficulty presidents can encounter
when they move to implement their agendas. While vagueness often has
strategic value in the context of a campaign, it presents a particular
problem for governance. The further a candidate is from the center of
public opinion on a given issue, the greater his incentive to be vague;
if a candidate's position is popular, he will feel free to discuss
it in detail. It is precisely on issues where the candidate's
positions are unpopular--and therefore where he has been vague during
the campaign--that he will encounter the greatest displeasure with his
policy when he attempts to implement it. Rhetorical convergence is not
problematic if both candidates mean what they say and genuinely intend
to pursue similar policies. But this is rarely the case in a
presidential election, not because the Democrat and Republican do not
agree on anything but because both the candidates and the press tend to
ignore the issues on which they do agree. Because the vast majority of
voters do not delve into the details of policy but may remember the
broad arguments a president made when he was a candidate, they are
likely to punish a candidate if his policies appear to diverge from
those they were led to expect.
(1.) The National Annenberg Election Survey employed a rolling
cross-section methodology, in which a new set of respondents was
interviewed every day for the duration of the presidential campaign,
from late 1999 through early 2001. The data presented in this article
concern awareness of candidate issue positions, on which there was only
slow movement over the course of the campaign. To focus on what voters
knew by the campaign's end, the data presented cover only the
period from after the final Bush-Gore debate (October 18) to the day
before election day (November 6). This sample contains 6,191 cases.
Although the sample adheres well to actual demographic breakdowns in
most cases, one variable in which it does not is education; as in most
telephone surveys, the National Annenberg Election Survey overrepresents
those with high education and underrepresents those with low education.
For this reason, and because the article presents simple percentages of
respondents, the data are weighted by number of adults and phone lines
in household, age, race, Hispanicity, education, and region. Readers
should note, however, that the results obtained using unweighted data
vary from the presented results by no more than four percentage points
in any case.
(2.) At the insistence of Democrats in Congress, the voucher
provision was dropped from the education bill that eventually passed
during Bush's first months in office.
(3.) These data are taken from the week following the final debate
in each election: 1,028 interviews conducted October 17 to 23, 1996, and
997 interviews conducted October 18 to 24, 2000.
(4.) In Gore's case, all of the differences are statistically
significant at p < .01. The same is true of Bush's scores in all
cases except gays in the military; the difference there is significant
at p < .10.
References
Alvarez, R. Michael. 1998. Information and elections. Ann Arbor:
University of Michigan Press.
Berke, Richard, and Janet Elder. 2001. Bush loses favor despite tax
cut and overseas trip. The New York Times, June 21, A1.
Brady, Henry, and Paul Sniderman. 1985. Attitude attribution: A
group basis for political reasoning. American Political Science Review
79:1061-78.
Brooks, David. 2000. Report from Philly: Why the GOP is playing
nice. Newsweek, August 3, 10.
Campbell, James. 1983. Ambiguity in the issue positions of
presidential candidates: A causal analysis. American Journal of
Political Science 27:284-93.
Cantril, Albert, and Susan Davis Cantril. 1999. Reading mixed
signals: Ambivalence in American public opinion about government.
Washington, DC: Woodrow Wilson Center Press.
Conover, Pamela Johnston, and Stanley Feldman. 1989. Candidate
perception in an ambiguous world: Campaigns, cues, and inference
processes. American Journal of Political Science 33:912-40.
Dalager, Jon. 1996. Voters, issues and elections: Are the
candidates' messages getting through? Journal of Politics
58:486-515.
Downs, Anthony. 1957. An economic theory of democracy. New York:
Harper.
Gilens, Martin. 2001. Political ignorance and collective policy
preferences. American Political Science Review 95:379-96.
Green, Donald, and Ian Shapiro. 1994. Pathologies of rational
choice theory. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hart, Roderick. 2000. Campaign talk: Why elections are good for us.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Iyengar, Shanto, and Donald Kinder. 1987. News that matters.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Jacobs, Lawrence, and Robert Shapiro. 2000. Politicians don't
pander: Political manipulation and the loss of democratic
responsiveness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Jamieson, Kathleen Hall, and Paul Waldman. 1997. Mapping campaign
discourse: An introduction. American Behavioral Scientist 40:1133-38.
--, eds. 2001. Electing the president 2000: The insiders'
view. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Jarvis, Sharon. 1999. Party style: Partisanship in presidential
campaign speeches, 1948-1996. Paper presented at the conference of the
International Communication Association, San Francisco.
King, David. 1997. The polarization of American parties and
mistrust of government. In Why people don't trust government,
edited by Joseph Nye, Philip D. Zelikow, and David C. King. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
Krosnick, John. 1990. Americans' perceptions of presidential
candidates: A test of the projection hypothesis. Journal of Social
Issues 46:159-82.
Kuklinski, James, Paul J. Quirk, Jennifer Jerit, David Schwieder,
and Robert F. Rich. 2000. Misinformation and the currency of democratic
citizenship. Journal of Politics 62:790-816.
Lichter, S. Robert. 2001. A plague on both parties: Substance and
fairness in TV election news. Harvard Journal of Press/Politics 6
(3):8-30.
Lodge, Milton, Marco Steenbergen, and Shawn Brau. 1995. The
responsive voter: Campaign information and the dynamics of candidate
evaluation. American Political Science Review 89:309-26.
Norpoth, Helmut, and Bruce Buchanan. 1992. Wanted: The education
president. Public Opinion Quarterly 56:87-99.
Page, Benjamin. 1976. The theory of political ambiguity. American
Political Science Review 70:742-52.
Pew Research Center for the People and the Press. 2000.
Presidential debate clouds voters' choice. Available from
www.people-press.org.
Project for Excellence in Journalism. 2000. A question of
character: How the media have handled the issue and how the public has
reacted. Available from www.journalism.org.
Shepsle, Kenneth. 1972. The strategy of ambiguity: Uncertainty and
electoral competition. American Political Science Review 66:555-68.
Sniderman, Paul Richard Brody, and Philip Tetlock. 1991. Reasoning
and choice: Explorations in political psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
West, Paul. 2000. For Gore, a battle for green territory. The
Baltimore Sun, October 24, A1.
Paul Waldman is associate director of the Annenberg Public Policy
Center at the University of Pennsylvania.
Kathleen Hall Jamieson is Walter H. Annenberg dean and professor of
communication at the Annenberg School for Communication of the
University of Pennsylvania. Her latest book (with P. Waldman) is The
Press Effect: Politicians, Journalists, and the Stories That Shape the
Political World (Oxford University Press, 2002).