When Is Presidential Behavior Public and When Is It Private?
JAMIESON, KATHLEEN HALL ; ADAY, SEAN
On February 27, 1998, the Philadelphia Inquirer quoted a hospital
administrator who runs a Republican professional women's group in
Louisiana as saying,
Well, let me tell you about my dearest friend. Her son has a rare heart
disease, and at one time she had a $160,000 medical bill because she
couldn't get health insurance. So we're talking about the sex scandal, and
she tells me, "Because Clinton's in the White House, I can get the
insurance now." That's all she call come up with! This is a person with a
big IQ. I wanted to say to her, "What about the fact that because of
Clinton, our moral foundation is built on sand?"
There is a similar sense of bewilderment in press accounts. U.S.
News and World Report, for example, asked on its February 2 cover,
"Is He Finished?" and showed a stern Clinton dwarfed by a
grainy, smiling Lewinsky. In that issue, columnist Gloria Borger's
disbelief is evident in her hypothetical explanation for the
voters' continued approval of Clinton's presidency:
Look at it this way: Hillary Clinton's ability to separate her own personal
anguish from the political anger she feels is not unlike Bill Clinton's
ability to persuade voters to separate his private life from his public
character. Most voters now even accept this notion that somehow one side of
the brain doesn't affect the other.(1)
Where pundits predicted that the allegations would undercut public
confidence in his presidency, the opposite seemed to be the case.
Clinton's approval ratings stayed above 60 percent through May.
When a reporter questioned whether it was healthy for the public to feel
that presidents' personal lives are not relevant, Clinton declined
to answer the question.
What explains this disconnection between the assumptions of
reporters and the polled responses of citizens? The answer is simple:
the public makes a distinction between private and public character and
between the personal and the presidential. So, for example, 80 percent
of a national sample reported in late February(2) that "In judging
Clinton, we should focus on how the country is doing and his policies,
and not on his private life." Sixty-five percent agreed with the
statement, "The public has become more realistic and accepts that
political leaders should be judged on their job performance not their
personal life."(3) Sixty-seven percent told the same pollsters that
the president "should be able to withhold certain private
matters," and a majority (59 percent) found it "understandable
[that] he would not tell the truth about his sex life."(4) Even if
one supposes that Clinton did have an affair, 66 percent believe that
that is "his business and has nothing to do with this job."(5)
Even when pollsters explicitly ask about ethics, the public-private
distinction holds up, with 59 percent saying that it is "possible
for Clinton to be unethical in his personal life while maintaining
integrity as president."(6)
Why the distinction between the personal and the presidential? In
part it is because revelations about the sex lives of presidents from
Franklin Roosevelt through John Kennedy and Lyndon Johnson have lowered
public expectations about marital fidelity in the White House.
Eighty-eight percent report that "other presidents have had equally
bad private lives."(7) Similarly, three out of four say,
"Clinton's faults are no worse than other presidents."(8)
Based in part on the conclusion that some of the nation's great
presidents have led less than exemplary private lives, 84 percent now
tell surveyors that "Someone can be a good president even if you
disapprove of his personal life."(9)
Also, it is possible that at least the 24 percent who say they can
imagine "circumstances where you'd commit adultery"(10)
have concluded that those who live in glass houses should not throw
stones. This conclusion is borne out by the finding that 51 percent
think that Clinton's moral standards are the same as the average
married man's, while 6 percent consider them higher.(11)
Nor do the allegations break an implicit compact that candidate
Clinton forged with voters. As Democratic pollster Peter Hart observes,
"You're assuming that the American public at some stage said
that this is a person of high moral values. In truth, they've never
believed that."(12) Fifty-six percent report that "before
recent allegations," they thought that "Clinton had other
affairs."(13)
Indeed, throughout the spring of 1998, polls showed a consistent
public belief that Clinton and Lewinsky had a sexual relationship,
despite the depositions of both to the contrary and despite the
president's stern denial the first week after the story broke. From
late March to late May, between 46 and 56 percent of Americans said they
believed a sexual affair took place, while between 22 and 41 percent
said they did not believe the relationship existed. Indeed, despite the
dismissal of the Paula Jones suit and the damage to the credibility of
Kathleen Willey, the strongest majority believing Clinton had a sexual
relationship with Lewinsky was found in a May 20 TIME/CNN poll, with 56
percent believing the affair took place and only 22 percent saying they
thought it did not. Yet, throughout this period, Clinton's support
remained high, with 62 percent saying in a May 23 CBS poll that they
approved of his performance.
Despite the belief that an affair took place--and the implied
belief that Clinton lied in denying it--polls have consistently found
that people make a distinction between private sexual behavior and
presidential conduct. Typically, about 60 percent of respondents say
they do not think a president's sex life is relevant to his job;
only about a third indicate otherwise. As long as the public judges the
allegations a private rather than a public matter, high confidence in
the conduct of the presidency can coexist with doubts about the
truthfulness of a president's responses to the charges about what
is perceived to be his private life.
Furthermore, when two-thirds (64 percent) say that it is very
important for the president to provide moral leadership, approve of his
work as president (66 percent), and say that he is hiding something
about Lewinsky (67 percent),(14) one can plausibly conclude that the
public-private distinction is at work here, as well. Voters are defining
moral leadership as "understanding the problems of people like
you," which 63 percent say describes Clinton. They are not, by
contrast, defining it as having "high personal moral and ethical
standards," a standard only 28 percent think Clinton meets.(15)
The public-private distinction works to Clinton's advantage in
a second way when the public perceives that those raising and
investigating the allegations have violated privacy rights. Note the
high level of public disapproval of Linda Tripp's taping of
conversations with Lewinsky (71 percent disapprove, according to a
January 30 Fox poll), Kenneth Starr's requiring Lewinsky's
mother to testify (64 percent disapprove according to a February 16
CNN/USA Today/Gallup poll), and his request that Secret Service agents
appear before the grand jury (53 percent disapprove according to the
same poll).
Indeed, approval of Clinton's presidency parallels disapproval
of special prosecutor Kenneth Starr and his investigation. In poll after
poll, a solid majority thought Starr was going too far in his
investigation (58 percent),(16) that he was principally concerned with
hurting Clinton rather than finding the truth (56 percent),(17) and that
his investigation was more partisan than impartial (52 percent on May
8,(18) 58 percent on May 23(19)). And from the beginning of the
investigation, poll after poll showed that only about a third of
respondents felt that the investigation should continue, despite their
corresponding belief that an affair had taken place and that Clinton had
lied.
There appears to be a public suspicion that the press has intruded on what should be private space, as well. What some in the press regard
as investigative journalism seems to the person in the street to be
simple voyeurism. More than two-thirds of those surveyed think that the
media should stop reporting on the sex lives of public figures, holding
that the sex life of a politician should not be a public issue.(20)
Public opinion regarding the news media deteriorated dramatically in a
short period of time: 50 percent of respondents in a March CNN/USA
Today/Gallup poll said they felt the media had acted responsibly in
covering the story; by April 14, a Marist College Institute poll found
that only 32 percent said they felt the press were doing their job,
compared with 68 percent who said they felt the news media had
overstepped their role.
The public apparently disregarded the press frame. In a March 20
TIME/CNN poll that asked if Clinton should be impeached if "the
evidence shows that [he] had an extramarital affair" with Lewinsky,
70 percent said no. Polls showed that a consensus in favor of
impeachment did not even emerge when the question was framed in terms of
Clinton committing perjury, with 55 percent telling an April NBC/Wall
Street Journal poll that they felt the president should remain in office
under that circumstance, and only 40 percent saying he should be
impeached.
The polls suggest where the public draws the line between private
and public behavior. While people overwhelmingly said Clinton should not
be impeached or resign for having a sexual relationship with Lewinsky,
and a majority did not even think that perjury would warrant his
removal, a majority consistently favored impeachment when asked
specifically about suborning perjury. A News Interest Index poll
released April 3 found that when asked only about the sexual
relationship, 40 percent favored impeachment; when asked specifically
about the president committing perjury, the number climbed to 45
percent; but when the question was put in terms of encouraging Lewinsky
to lie under oath, 54 percent supported Clinton being removed from
office. This seems to indicate that the public believes private behavior
becomes public when it extends beyond his personal behavior and creates
incentives for someone to lie under oath. By this logic, if Clinton and
Lewinsky were to lie independently of one another, the public would deem
it a private matter. But if Clinton encourages her to lie, perhaps by
offering a quid pro quo for a job, the public would judge it a public
matter involving a conspiracy. Further evidence of the public's
lack of tolerance for illegalities not related to sex is that
Clinton's approval rating dropped in June amid reports that he may
have broken the law regarding campaign contributions from Chinese
donors.
The facts of the Lewinsky story plausibly support both the
private/sex and the public/legal frame. The question is, how can we know
which frame the public is adopting? One clue seems to lie in the fact
that Clinton's approval ratings remain high. This suggests that
people do not think he encouraged Lewinsky to lie, since in that
circumstance a majority supports his removal from office. Furthermore,
when asked a generic question, such as "Based on what you have read
or heard, do you believe that President Clinton should be impeached and
removed from office," 71 percent said "no" in a March 20
TIME/CNN survey; 76 percent said "no" when the question was
asked again in an April 10 poll released by the same organizations.
These numbers are similar to those elicited when people were asked if
Clinton should be impeached just for having a sexual relationship with
Lewinsky. They are far removed, however, from the majority or near
majority that supported his removal in the event that he suborned
perjury. This suggests that, left to frame the question on its own, the
public is casting the allegations in terms of sex and that people do not
think that a president's sex life is relevant to the job of chief
executive.
The Lewinsky story exposes a dramatic difference between the public
on one hand and the press on the other when it comes to framing and
understanding the personal behavior of the president. American citizens
may be interested in the salacious details of political sex scandals, as
attention to news reports in the first few weeks of the story suggests,
but they are not necessarily making punitive judgments based on that
information. Although reporters openly questioned Clinton's ability
to avoid impeachment for what appeared to be at least perjury and at
most suborning perjury, and Republican congressional leaders said to
varying degrees that impeachment proceedings might be warranted, the
public did not see the alleged activities in legal terms.
This raises three important points. First, the American people are
capable of absorbing large amounts of information from the press without
losing the ability to think for themselves. The press, although
certainly interested in the more tawdry aspects of the case, initially
applied a public/legal frame to the Lewinsky story that was largely
disregarded by a public that reframed it in terms of private behavior.
Second, this difference in framing suggests that the press is out of
step with the public over how to judge individual politicians. The
public seems more concerned with job performance than personal
proclivities. Finally, the public has a line it does not expect
politicians to cross, and that line is drawn at obstruction of justice.
The polls suggest that support for Clinton would drop dramatically were
it shown that he had tried to induce others to cover up his alleged
indiscretions. At that point, private behavior would become a matter of
public censure.
Notes
(1.) Gloria Borger, "The Clinton's Marriage," U.S.
News and World Report, February 2, 1998, pp. 30, 32.
(2.) Harris poll, February 25, 1998.
(3.) NBC/Wall Street Journal poll, March 5, 1998.
(4.) New York Times/CBS poll, February 23, 1998.
(5.) ABC/Washington Post poll, February 19, 1998.
(6.) Los Angeles Times poll, February 1.
(7.) Harris poll, February 25, 1998.
(8.) CNN/USA Today poll, February 1, 1998.
(9.) CBS/New York Times poll, February 23, 1998.
(10.) Fox poll, February 20, 1998.
(11.) TIME/CNN poll, January 30, 1998.
(12.) Dan Balz, "Willey Gets a Shrug; Public Seems to Divorce
Clinton from Shadow; New Allegation Rates a Shrug from Public,"
Washington Post, March 19, 1998, p. A14.
(13.) Los Angeles Times poll, February 1, 1998.
(14.) CNN/USA Today/Gallup poll, February 16, 1998.
(15.) ABC/Washington Post poll, February 19, 1998.
(16.) TIME/CNN poll, March 30, 1998.
(17.) Washington Post poll, April 5, 1998.
(18.) CBS poll, May 5, 1998.
(19.) CBS poll, May 23, 1998.
(20.) Fox poll, January 29, 1998.
KATHLEEN HALL JAMIESON
Dean, Annenberg School for Communication, and
Director, Annenberg Public Policy Center
University of Pennsylvania
Kathleen Hall Jamieson is the dean of the Annenberg School for
Communication and director of the Annenberg Public Policy Center of the
University of Pennsylvania. Her most recent book, coauthored with Joseph
Cappella, is Spiral of Cynicism: The Press and the Public Good.
SEAN ADAY
Ph.D. Candidate, Annenberg School for Communication
University of Pennsylvania
Sean Aday, M.A., Annenberg School for Communication, University of
Pennsylvania, is a Ph.D. candidate at the Annenberg School for
Communication. He is a former reporter who received his undergraduate
degree at Northwestern University's Medill School of Journalism.