首页    期刊浏览 2024年11月14日 星期四
登录注册

文章基本信息

  • 标题:Advancing wellbeing research: would Americans be happier if they lived like Australians?
  • 作者:Patulny, Roger ; Fisher, Kimberly
  • 期刊名称:Australian Journal of Social Issues
  • 印刷版ISSN:0157-6321
  • 出版年度:2012
  • 期号:September
  • 语种:English
  • 出版社:Australian Council of Social Service
  • 摘要:Every happiness is but lent by chance for an uncertain time, and may therefore be demanded back the next hour.
  • 关键词:Emotions;Time management

Advancing wellbeing research: would Americans be happier if they lived like Australians?


Patulny, Roger ; Fisher, Kimberly


Introduction

Every happiness is but lent by chance for an uncertain time, and may therefore be demanded back the next hour.

--Arthur Schopenhauer

Subjective wellbeing increasingly is recognised as a key indicator of quality of life. Positive psychologists have spent much of the last decade promoting the emotional qualities of subjective wellbeing such as happiness or satisfaction as critical components of good mental health (Seligman et al. 2005). Such emotions may complement--or even provide a credible alternative to--income in national economic measures of social progress (Diener & Seligman 2004; Layard 2005). Many of these studies are limited, however, by their focus on measuring the qualities associated with generalised domains, such as satisfaction with life as a whole, or with work, family or leisure. We argue that these broad assessments miss the variation in emotional experiences associated with daily activities and the consequences for wellbeing when routines change.

Measures of 'emotional wellbeing'--which we define here as the continuous (or hedonic) flow of emotions and enjoyment derived from the activities performed in the specific sequences that fill a day--often provide better indicators of subjective wellbeing than more conventional life-domain measures. Kahneman and Krueger find through experiments and reviews of experimental research (Kahneman et al. 1997, 2006; Krueger & Schkade 2007) that life-domain assessments are over-weighted by extreme and recent experience, and by moods. They observe question-order effects and find indications that life-domain measures asked two weeks apart are less reliable than diaries collecting emotion data alongside time use information (thus capturing emotion in context). Kahneman and Krueger thus distinguish between generalised measures that ask 'How happy/satisfied are you in general?' (Easterlin 1974; Heady & Wearing 1992; Oswald 1997; Helliwell 2003) and hedonic measures that ask 'How much time do you spend doing enjoyable activities?' (Juster & Stafford 1985; Robinson & Godbey 1997; Kahneman & Krueger 2006; Gershuny 2011), and they emphasise the importance of the latter measures.

A growing number of researchers in the field of time diary surveys have started to collect data on emotions. These studies capture the range and frequency of the daily activities that take place across a society, and situate emotional responses in the contexts that shape these emotions (Kahneman & Krueger 2006; Gershuny 2012; Koll & Pokutta 2012). This approach has the analytical benefit of increasing the variability of responses, in that the metric used is much wider than a single 10-point scale of happiness, and it also captures data on location, co-presence and enjoyment based over 1,440 minutes per day. When conducted on a national scale, time and emotion diary surveys form a useful compendium of measures of the subjective wellbeing of nations, complementing national income and production measures (Kahneman & Krueger 2006; Gershuny 2012; Koll & Pokutta 2012). They allow analysis of variations in wellbeing across countries and regions resulting from local policy settings that impact time-schedules, such as business or school or service opening times, labour laws, child support arrangements, transportation and service availability.

Time diary and affect measures also offer a unique opportunity to address a dilemma concerning assessing the unintended consequences of making policies. Daily activities take place in cycles bounded by the biological needs, cultural conventions, and social structures of communities and nations. Policies that change one element of behaviour may have unintended consequences for other aspects of daily life. For example, policies aimed at reducing time spent commuting can open space in daily schedules that people can fill with other activities which could be generally perceived as 'positive' (for example, exercise) or 'negative' (for example, watching television). Time use methods can assess the impact of policy and behavioural changes and associated satisfaction in ways that more general questions about wellbeing cannot. Simply asking people how satisfied they are with life in general does not reveal whether it was the behaviour change inspired by the policy that made them more or less satisfied, or whether this resulted from other changes in activities that occurred as an unintended consequence of the policy.

In this paper, we contribute to the growing literature exploring the potential of time use data to model these potential consequences of policy and behavioural change on emotions. We do this by comparing time use patterns in two countries with relatively similar cultural and institutional contexts, Australia and the United States, to see how people in the United States might feel if they altered--or policy encouraged them to alter--their routines to live more like Australians.

To date, nationally representative surveys of time use and emotion only have been conducted in three countries, (1) which limits the direct comparison of emotional wellbeing across countries. Nevertheless, we can use the available affect profiles and associated time use patterns for a country such as the United States counterfactually, to see how emotional wellbeing there might be improved (or degraded) if people living in the United States were to modify their routines to live more like people in another country. This technique builds on counterfactual analytical techniques widely used in political science and historical analysis (Tetlock & Belkin 1996). However, rather than modelling changes in daily routines from a consequent change in policy, we map the ordinal emotion rating associated with activities in one time-pattern (that of the United States) to estimate the emotional responses with the alternative time-pattern (that of Australia). Our strategy is a modest version of that adopted by Gershuny (2011, 2012), who first observed similar emotion ratings associated with activities undertaken in the 1980s in the United States and United Kingdom. His approach was to map an amalgamated emotion rating based on these affect ratings to the contemporary time use patterns of 15 countries in order to compare the emotional experiences that countries with different welfare regime types might confer upon their national populations.

It is important to note that we are not directly comparing Americans and Australians, nor do we assert that Australians would respond in the same way as Americans to various activities. Rather, we estimate how people in the United States might feel if they altered their routines to live more like Australians, in order to investigate the consequences of changing time-based behaviour patterns for emotional wellbeing. In this context, cultural differences between the two countries in how people feel about activities become irrelevant. Nevertheless, as we will demonstrate, there are a sufficient number of similarities between these two countries to make the use of Australian patterns a realistic counterfactual to examine how changes in American behaviours might impact American emotional wellbeing.

Key differences in policies affecting these two countries (discussed below) include those of gender and wellbeing. Such differences are likely to produce potentially large differences in wellbeing worth investigating through survey research. Stevenson and Wolfers (2009) use the American General Social Survey to examine gendered wellbeing in America from the 1970s to 2006, and find that women's happiness has declined absolutely and relative to men's since the 1970s. Stevenson and Wolfers (2009) point to the contradiction between the improvement in women's income and employment situations over this time period, and a decline in their wellbeing. However, they do not adequately identify why this pattern has emerged, beyond suggesting only that women now compare their employment success to their male peers rather than to the careers of women from previous generations. They do not engage with time-based measures and changes in behaviours.

Kahneman and Kruger (2006), on the other hand, bring a time-based explanation to similar findings. They observe that the amount of time women report that they have experienced negative emotions has remained fairly constant since 1965, while men's reporting of negative emotions has decreased, mainly due to men's reduced paid work time. Such assessments suggest that the impact of policy on behaviours, and thus on wellbeing, may impact the sexes differently.

This paper maps activity-associated emotion ratings from the 2006 Princeton Affect and Time Survey onto time-use patterns in the Multinational Time Use Study (MTUS) version of the 2006 American Time Use Survey and the 2006 Australian Bureau of Statistics National Time Use Survey. Our purposes are to determine whether Americans would experience a net gain or loss in 'pleasant' time from shifting to Australian time patterns; to identify in what areas or activities Americans would gain and lose from such a change; and to identify the consequences for gender equity and men and women's emotional wellbeing.

Time, emotional wellbeing and gender--United States and Australia

Both Australians and Americans report feeling generally happy in international surveys of wellbeing (Leigh & Wolfers 2006). Given relatively abundant material prosperity in both countries, this is unsurprising. Heady and colleagues (2004) reveal through analysis of five separate national panel datasets (including one from Australia) that material resources have a much stronger impact upon life satisfaction than income. Kahneman and Deaton (2010) assess satisfaction and emotional wellbeing amongst nearly half a million responses to the American Gallup-Healthways Well-Being Index. They find that positive life evaluation (satisfaction) increases with income, though is subject to diminishing returns. That is, the relationship is non-linear and increases with the log of income, rather than income itself. However, they also find that while emotional wellbeing increases with income, the relationship diminishes and flat-lines at an annual income of approximately (US) $75,000. Beyond this, increased income does not improve emotional wellbeing. Emotions thus demonstrate a variability that transcends purely material conditions, and given the cultural and institutional differences between Americans and Australians, we would expect the time use patterns of both countries to generate differences in total pleasant and unpleasant time.

Table 1 displays the daily time allocation of working-aged women and men in Australia and the United States, with reference to 21 other countries. Australian men work relatively long hours in paid employment (fifth of 23) and also spend the highest total time caring for children. American men have similar patterns and spend only modestly less time in these activities. They engage in marginally more housework than Australian men, but rank similarly compared to men in many other countries in terms of their commitment to paid and unpaid work. Women in the United States and Australia have similarly high levels of time spent caring for children. Australian women perform both housework and paid work at a middle-range level, whereas American women work more hours for pay and engage in relatively fewer hours of housework. As with men, women's total commitment to paid and unpaid work is similar in both countries. Women and men in the United States spend roughly 30 minutes more per day socialising, and around one-quarter of an hour more on weekdays and half an hour more on weekends in voluntary activities than Australians. In sum, Australian and American men and women spend similar and substantial amounts of time in paid and unpaid work, but Americans spend more time in social and voluntary activities.

Australians' and Americans' emotional reactions to paid work and child-care activities may be influenced by national policy contexts (Hook 2010; Craig & Mullan 2010). National and State governments in both countries have instigated policies to encourage women's transition into the workplace, though Australian legislators have shown greater willingness to regulate private child care facilities (Craig & Siminski 2010; Craig & Mullan 2010), and assist with childcare costs. Australia has also recently introduced its first national paid maternity leave scheme, while the United States lacks any such comprehensive national scheme. A further difference (and possible consequence of the aforementioned polices) is that Australian mothers with young children are more likely to take part-time jobs, while a relatively high proportion of mothers with children aged less than 12 in the United States work full-time, when compared with other OECD countries (OECD 2011).

Working parents in Australia and the United States also tend to work long hours, compared with working parents in other OECD countries (OECD 2011) --and even though more women than men work for pay part-time, women's overall lower time in paid work is counter-balanced by their higher unpaid work time (Fisher & Robinson 2010). This means that working mothers and fathers face similar constraints on the proportion of time they can allocate to the more enjoyable activities. While the literature exploring the gendered division of domestic labour by welfare regime type is extensive (Rice et al. 2006; Drobnic & Guillen 2011), so far the time-use literature has not investigated the gendered distribution of emotional experiences across various daily activities.

Current research suggests that men and women tend to report different levels of emotion during activities, and that different profiles of time spent in such activities across countries will thus contribute greatly to differences in emotional wellbeing (or lack thereof). In general, women are more likely to enjoy social time and activities that are oriented more towards informal support and emotional relations rather than work-orientated connections (Emmerick 2006). Compared to men, women report less satisfaction with (and interest in) paid work (Sloane & Williams 2000), more stress at work (Hutri & Lindeman 2002), and more frequent and longer pain durations, particularly associated with intensity of work (Dao & LeResche 2000; Burchell & Fagan 2004). In general, longer work hours also are thought to cut into more traditionally feminised 'social' activities, such as time looking after children while talking with other parents (Maher et al. 2008; Gray 2009). As recent income support policies in many countries encourage parents to work longer hours, single parents, and single mothers in particular, not only cope with more severe financial constraints than couple parents (De Vaus et al. 2009), they also face a greater likelihood of social exclusion from the time binds imposed by care and total work responsibilities (Patulny & Wong 2013).

In Australia, women report high levels of time-stress reconciling paid work and unpaid care commitments (Craig & Sawrikar 2009), and the role strain Australian mothers experience balancing work and care is associated with increasing mental health problems (Bryson et al. 2007). Similar findings emerge in research into stress experienced by women in the United States (Epstein & Kalleberg 2004). Offer and Schneider (2011) attribute the greater stress and work-family conflict experienced by American women to their greater tendency to multitask across domains of activity (for instance, child care overlapping with leisure, or domestic work overlapping with personal care) compared to American men.

Men in general are likely to report more unhappy time at work arising from working longer hours. Men are potentially more likely to suffer greater tiredness from sleeping slightly less than women (Robinson & Michelson 2010). Gershuny (2011) found that men in the United States and the United Kingdom were less likely than women to report feeling happy while working. Nevertheless, working fathers still experience less work-related stress and burn-out than mothers when children are young (Hill et al. 2008).

Leisure and social contact patterns may also differ more by gender than by country. Australian men are more likely to report loneliness and depression with regards to social activities, or the lack thereof (Franklin & Tranter 2008). Both Australian (van Wanrooy & Wilson 2006) and American (Epstein & Kalleberg 2004) men are likely to have seen their friendship networks and social time deteriorate as a consequence of long working hours. Australian men (Patulny 2009) and American women (Cornwell 2011) also are more likely to lose collegial ties upon retirement.

Gershuny (2011) found that in both the United States and United Kingdom in the 1980s, women tended to enjoy in-home leisure more than men (excluding television, which both genders rate roughly equally). Gershuny also observed that women rated shopping and travel more highly than men, though for both sexes these activities are among the more unpleasant of the activities that people undertake (2011).

Methods

Our procedure for estimating whether Americans would experience a net gain or loss in 'pleasant' time from shifting to Australian-style time-use patterns involved four stages: (1)selecting time-use data (MTUS)and affect data (PATS); (2) using regression models to estimate the likelihood of a PATS episode being 'unpleasant' based on the activities and demographic qualities (explained further in Patulny & Fisher 2013); (3) imputing the likelihood of episodes in the MTUS data being 'unpleasant' for Americans based on PATS ratings by use of the models from the previous step; and (4) aggregating the time in the 'unpleasant' MTUS episodes to estimate 'unpleasant minutes per day'. We now briefly summarise these stages.

1) Selecting data

The 2006 Princeton Affect and Time Survey (PATS) serves as our source to generate activity-related affect scores. PATS is a nationally representative telephone survey of 11,905 respondents in the United States. Sample weights adjust the demographic distribution to match the United States Current Population Survey. PATS asked participants to describe their activities from the previous 24-hour day, then collected six affect ratings for three randomly chosen 15-minute episodes during that day (2). The survey asked diarists to rate their perceptions on a 0-6 scale of how much they felt happy, interested, in pain, sad, stressed and tired during the time period. Table 2 displays a sample diary, enabling readers to get a feel for the flexible structure of the data.

The Multinational Time Use Survey (MTUS) offers a usefully harmonised version of both the 2006 American Time Use Survey and the 2006 Australian National Time Use Survey episode data. In the MTUS file, each row case represents a change in main activity, secondary activity, location, mode of transport or who else was present. The main and secondary activity codes are harmonised into 69 categories that appear in a large majority of time use surveys. In this format, the surveys not only are prepared for direct comparison but also organised in a similar format to the PATS file.

2) Estimating the likelihood of an episode being 'unpleasant'

Kahneman and Krueger (2006) argue that measures of unpleasant time yield more robust results than measures of positive enjoyment. We repeated Kahneman and Krueger's (2006) approach for determining which episodes are unpleasant, dividing activities into binary categories, pleasant (0, the default), and unpleasant (1), representing cases where the scores for any of the negative emotions outweigh the score for the positive emotions for that particular episode. They note that this is a conservative approach to measuring unpleasantness as people are less likely to report intense negative as opposed to positive emotions, and as a consequence produces fewer unhappy episodes, with only 20 per cent of episodes found to be 'unpleasant' by their measure. We analysed the PATS dataset to identify 'unpleasant' episodes.

We next ran regression models to determine the impact of demographic characteristics and activities on the likelihood of the episode being 'unpleasant.' This approach mirrors that undertaken by Gershuny (2011, 2012), who used activity-level emotion information collected in time diary surveys in the United States in 1985 and in the United Kingdom in 1987 to make similar projections about potential implications for national time accounts. In that paper, Gershuny used much older time and affect data from full 24-hour diaries rather than the more detailed utility measures for three random intervals that we make use of from the PATS survey. Nevertheless, it is worth noting that Gershuny found little difference in the utility profiles for the British and Americans, and that the gender differences were greater than the country differences (Gershuny 2011, 2012). We have no data at present to say how closely Australian and American utility profiles might map.

We used Linear Probability Regression (LPM) to predict unpleasantness on the basis of the demographic qualities of the person and the main activity that took place during the episode. The equation used to impute the probability of an MTUS episode being unpleasant took the following form:

[Pu.sub.e] = [a.sub.k]X + [b.sub.j]T

Here, [Pu.sub.i] is the probability of each episode e being rated as unpleasant. T is a vector of j activity variables (such as "sleep", "watching television" and so on) that correspond to the "main activities" in the MTUS time-diary datasets that may influence the unpleasantness of activities, while [b.sub.i] is the set of coefficients relating this vector to each episode. X is a vector of k (demographic) control variables that may influence the enjoyment of activities, and [a.sub.i] is the set of coefficients relating this vector to each to each episode. The results suggest that the model provides a reasonable estimation of the affect and the unpleasantness associated with activities in the United States (see Patulny & Fisher 2012 for more detail).

3) Imputing the 'unpleasantness' likelihood of Australian and American time diary episodes

Next we used the beta coefficients derived from the regression to impute the likelihood of episodes in the Australian and American MTUS data being 'unpleasant'. We uses the MTUS version of the 2006 ATUS data instead of the PATS in part to access a larger sample and in part to compare two datasets into which we mapped utility profiles, minimising the potential that the mapping exercise itself might impact on the results. The results from the imputation suggest that the model provides a reasonable estimation of the affect and the unpleasantness associated with activities in the United States. The mean episode unhappiness--what Kahneman and Krueger call u-index--estimated for the American MTUS data was nearly identical to actual episode u-index in the PATS data: around 0.18 (Patulny & Fisher 2013).

4) Estimating unpleasant minutes per day in Australian and American MTUS data

The final step was to use the predicted affect scores in the MTUS data to find 'unpleasant' and 'pleasant' episodes of time in the American and Australian data subsets, and then to aggregate these into 'unpleasant' and 'pleasant' time to produce a sum total of 'pleasant' and 'unpleasant' minutes per day that can be compared. Given that the modelling produced estimates of the likelihood of episodes being pleasant or unpleasant (rather than definitively as yes '1', or no '0') it was necessary to choose a cut-off for defining episodes as one status or another. We therefore coded episodes in MTUS as 'unpleasant' if the predicted affect score for an episode was higher than the mean u-index score in the PATS data of 0.18, or as 'pleasant' if the score was equal to or lower than 0.18. Once episodes were defined as 'unpleasant' or 'pleasant', the time associated with each episode was aggregated and summed into total 'pleasant' and 'unpleasant' minutes per day. This measure is then amenable to comparison across the time-schedules of different countries.

The procedure described above produces data amenable to descriptive analysis and further regression modelling. As noted above, the unpleasantness score is based on American data, so any derived results will not say how Australians feel, but rather how people from the United States would feel if they followed the time patterns of Australians. From here we turn to our descriptive results.

Results

Unpleasant time

We first look at aggregate net unpleasant time if Americans were to live like Australians (see Figure 1). These figures exclude time spent asleep, as sleep time greatly exaggerates this difference. The broad results show that Americans would have experienced a kind of double 'loss' if they lived like Australians, in that they would experience both more unpleasant and less pleasant time. Adding these two gaps together, Americans would have 45 minutes more net 'unpleasant' time per day if they transferred their affective responses to an Australian time schedule.

[FIGURE 1 OMITTED]

Next we examine unpleasant time by activity category (see Figure 2). Americans would reduce their unpleasant time in paid work, and they would gain more pleasant time in personal care. However, the shift to Australian patterns would also substantially increase their time spent in unpleasant unpaid domestic work, as well as increasing unpleasant time spent watching television, which is one of the least valued leisure activities, with decreasing pleasure the longer people remain in front of the television. Americans also would lose a substantial amount of the pleasant time they currently spend doing in-home free time and leisure (mostly social) activities. In particular, Americans spend more enjoyable time having friends over for dinner than do Australians, and this would make a big difference to their overall level of wellbeing in shifting between the two aggregate patterns of time use. Americans also would stand to lose time in travel related to shopping (much of which they enjoy). As things stood in 2006, American men spent less time in unpleasant travel for work than they would have done living like Australian men.

Overall, if Americans were to live like Australians, they would gain a measure of 'happiness' in terms of slightly increased pleasant time through less paid work and more pleasant personal care activities. However, they would lose more than they gained in terms of taking on more unpleasant domestic work, television and work-travel time, and particularly in the loss of pleasant in-home social time and leisure-travel time.

[FIGURE 2 OMITTED]

Unpleasant time by gender

When we separate women and men's experiences, large gender differences appear. Whichever of the two patterns of time use they followed, American men would enjoy more pleasant time and experience less unpleasant time than women. The data show greater differences between the emotional accounts of women and men than between the lifestyles of Americans and Australians (if adopted by Americans). This matches expectations from much of the research cited above. By shifting to Australian time patterns, American women would have 3.5 hours more net unpleasant time a day than American men. This would represent an improvement over the actual American gender gap of 3.8 hours more unpleasant time experienced by women. This is reflected in the gender-country gap. There is a sizeable gap between the lifestyles of American and Australian men of 57 minutes per day, such that American men would be nearly an hour worse off per day if they lived like Australian men. The gap in enjoyment between the lifestyles of American and Australian women is smaller, but still substantial at 37 minutes per day, or close to 4.5 hours per week. This suggests that a change to Australian lifestyles would have a smaller negative impact upon American women than men. While American women would lose out in terms of overall emotional experience, they would benefit in a relative sense from a reduced gender gap in unpleasant time (see Figure 3).

The differences noted above make more sense when examined in the context of activities. In Figure 3, the striped bars represent what Americans might feel living like Australians, while the solid bars are the actual American patterns in 2006 (in each case, the lighter colours are for men, the darker colours are for women). The gender gaps are partially explained by some familiar time patterns. The vast bulk of men's net unpleasant activity is in paid work. A small fraction thereafter is located in education and unpaid domestic work. Every other activity is, overall, pleasant for men. The bulk of women's unpleasant time is spent in paid work and unpaid domestic work combined together.

[FIGURE 3 OMITTED]

Most other activities are pleasant, but men spend more time than women in pleasant personal care, civic activity, sports, in-home leisure, media and computing, television-watching and travel. The standout differences are in personal care, in-home leisure, travel, and particularly television-watching. Unlike men, women find television-watching to be a largely unrewarding activity. This may be due to the quality of the time spent watching television. American men not only watch television for 10-15 minutes longer per day than American women, American men in couples also reported 'watching television with their spouses' for an average of 10 minutes longer than American women in couples reported doing the same activity with their male partners. Men also are more likely to watch television for fewer and longer spells than women (Fisher et al. 2007). These differences are also more likely to arise in the early evening than at other times of day, and are associated with women making more transitions between leisure and paid and unpaid work than men. As women also multi-task more than men (Epstein & Kalleberg 2004), this difference also may reflect the stress of overlapping television-based leisure with (largely unpaid) work.

The country-gaps involve some deviations from this pattern. Both American men and women would reduce their unpleasant work time but increase their unpleasant unpaid work time were they to adopt the time-patterns of their Australian counter-parts. American men would gain slightly more pleasant media time, but lose a larger amount of pleasant in-home leisure, television-watching, and travel time if they lived like Australian men. American women would gain some pleasant personal care time, but lose pleasant in-home free time in particular, as well as media, computing, and travel time. American men and women would thus lose out from switching to Australian time patterns through loss of pleasant in-home leisure and travel for both sexes, and loss of pleasant television-watching time amongst men (see Figure 4).

[FIGURE 4 OMITTED]

Modelling pleasantness and unpleasantness in American and Australian time patterns

We can now look at which emotions are associated with the most unhappiness, comparing genders and controlling for a range of demographic factors, using Ordinary Least Squares regression analysis. Demographic controls are limited to those available in each of the PATS and the MTUS datasets, and include country, sex, marital status, age, employment status and education. All controls are coded as binary dummies, and the consequent reference category is a fairly typical mid-life American female--married with children, aged 35-65, not in the labour force, and having completed secondary education but no post-secondary education.

The models largely confirm the descriptive findings above that American time schedules are more pleasant (for Americans) than Australian time schedules, and reveal an important socio-economic dimension to this picture. Table 3 shows the marginal effects of various demographic characteristics on unpleasant and pleasant minutes per day. The country and gender effects are as expected before we bring in controls. American men have much less unpleasant and more pleasant time than American women (the reference category). American men have more pleasant activity patterns than women (whichever of the two patterns they follow), though the model predicts that they would be less happy if they followed the time schedules of Australian men. These effects are consistent regardless of which control variables are introduced into the model.

However, the model does reveal that socio-economic conditions have an influence on the time and affective experiences of American women. Once we control for high and low levels of education, Australian time schedules shift from being significantly more unpleasant to significantly less unpleasant for American women (the coefficient switches from plus to minus). In other words, while American women of lower and higher levels of education would experience more net unpleasant time living like Australians, middle-educated women would experience less net unpleasant time from such a behaviour shift. So it seems that these American women (at least) would benefit from switching to an Australian lifestyle.

Discussion and conclusion

In order to see clearly the factors that drive differences in the experience of emotions, it is essential to understand the social context and everyday activities associated with emotional states. This paper demonstrates that time and affect diary surveys can help with this by setting up counterfactual explorations of the potential changes in emotional wellbeing from one country shifting its daily routine patterns to more closely resemble those of another. Such an exercise complements policy debates. Americans can test propositions suggesting that they would enjoy their time more if only they had the labour market policies of the Republic of Korea, or the family policies of Sweden, etcetera, by combining time diary information from the favoured country with American time and affect data. Such analysis would allow informed speculation as to the impact on emotional wellbeing arising from time-based activity changes that presumably result from policy. At a minimum, this technique can show that a venerated pattern of life as an aspiration for policy may not necessarily encourage change that would leave a population better off.

The results here suggest there is fertile ground for cross-national research once affect dimensions are added to the time use surveys of more countries. This particular example suggests that Americans would experience more unpleasant time and less pleasant time if they lived like Australians. Primarily, these emotional 'losses' would arise from more unpleasant unpaid domestic work and television-watching, as well as a loss of pleasant in-home free-time and social leisure. Americans would experience some emotional 'gains' that would partly offset these losses--such as engaging in less unpleasant paid work and more pleasant personal care--but these positive changes leave a deficit of 45 minutes less enjoyable time per day.

Of particular significance, this research demonstrates the presence of a gender gap in negative emotional experiences in the United States which is associated with behaviour patterns, largely reflecting that men enjoy more pleasant personal care, in-home leisure, travel and television-watching time than women. This research also suggests that adjustments in behaviour have potential to alter the depth of this gap. In 2006, women in the United States lived with 3.8 hours of more unpleasant time than men. Were Americans to shift to Australian lifestyles, this gender gap would shrink slightly to women experiencing only 3.5 hour of more unpleasant time than men. This adjustment would occur as American men would lose out more--losing 57 minutes of pleasant time shifting to an Australian lifestyle--while women would lose only 37 minutes of pleasant time. The gender gap finding has a qualifier; middle-educated American women with secondary-school (but not tertiary) education should feel happier leading an Australian lifestyle. We might speculate on the role that the Australian welfare system plays in this, given its propensity towards delivering a large number of family tax benefits to Australian middle classes, though it is wiser here to simply refer this finding as a topic for future research.

Though we use Australian data to set up a counterfactual analysis of emotional wellbeing in the United States, this paper raises questions to explore in Australia if and when emotion data becomes available in this country. It highlights the importance of social connection and isolation in improving wellbeing. In-home leisure is a great source of pleasantness for Americans that some Australians (relatively) lack. Social contact has been previously linked to subjective wellbeing, with strong links recorded between subjective wellbeing and social capital, as measured by ties to friends and neighbours, workplace ties, civic engagement, trustworthiness and trust (Helliwell et al. 2010). Mellor and colleagues (2008) find that loneliness is strongly associated with the discrepancy between need to belong and satisfaction with personal relationships, and with life in general. The finding that Australians do not spend as much time in (potentially) pleasant social activity as they (comparatively) might, and that this might impact upon their wellbeing may indicate that it is time to start thinking of 'barbecue-starter' policies in Australia. Pending more research on social connections and wellbeing in different urban contexts, there are implications for policies aiming to support families and for urban and suburban planning as well.

We emphasise that emotion matters in wellbeing research, but further work needs to determine the best strategy for collecting these data. Kahneman and Krueger (2006), Gershuny (2012), Robinson and Godbey (1997), and others have demonstrated the limitations in conventional wellbeing research, and the need to collect time-use and affect diaries to measure the contextual mechanisms associated subjective wellbeing. Nevertheless, the few surveys collecting these data adopt a variety of approaches. The surveys collected in the United States and the United Kingdom in the mid-1980s (summarised in Gershuny 2012) asked diarists to complete one column rating their level of happiness during each entry in the diary, though these columns employ a variety of scale points. The 2009-10 French national time use study collected by the official statistical office INSEE adopted a similar approach. The Kahneman and Krueger approach summarised in this paper collected ratings for six emotions for three randomly selected 15-minute intervals. The 2010 American Time Use Survey collected by the United States Bureau of Labour Statistics asked the same six items designed by Kahneman and Krueger, but asked these questions of three randomly selected episodes. Future research needs to compare results of these methods to test which measures of emotion offer most analytic potential, and also to investigate whether fewer questions asked of all episodes or more questions asked of fewer episodes provides that optimal research potential.

We conclude by suggesting future surveys should also be more cognisant of sociological issues and approaches. Kahneman and Krueger's interest is primarily in the psychological enjoyment of activities, rather than in the social interactions that shape enjoyment and emotional wellbeing. They are therefore interested in and include only a limited number of emotions in their surveys, and do not measure the prevalence and influence of the more 'socially stigmatised' emotions such as anger, shame and envy. Similarly, they do not account for Hochschild's key contention (1979, 1983) that emotions can be 'managed', in that people adjust their emotions according to the normative expectations of the people around them in specific social contexts. This usually occurs in the workplace, but might be endemic to family, friends and indeed every social interaction. It is difficult to assess when those emotions most commonly managed are the stigmatised emotions such as anger, shame and envy that people often try to hide. We suggest then that there is also a need for an 'audit' of emotions and emotion work, to be undertaken through a range of methods including time-diaries. We encourage this as a way forward for future research into time and emotion in Australia and other countries. Further suggestions and details as to how this might be achieved are outlined by the authors elsewhere (Patulny 2012).

References

Bryson, L., Warner-Smith, P., Brown, P. & Fray, L. (2007) 'Managing the work-life roller-coaster: private stress or public health issue?', Social Science & Medicine, 65 (6), 1142-1153.

Burchell, B. & Fagan, C. (2004) 'Gender and the intensification of work: evidence from the European Working Conditions Survey', Eastern Economic Journal, 30 (4), 627-42.

Cornwell, B. (2011) 'Age trends in daily social contact patterns', Research on Aging, 33 (5), 598-631.

Craig, L. & Mullan, K. (2010) 'Parenthood, gender and work-family time in USA, Australia, Italy, France and Denmark', Journal of Marriage and Family, 72 (5), 1344-1361.

Craig, L & Sawrikar, P. (2009) 'Work and family: how does the (gender) balance change as children grow?', Gender, Work & Organisation, 16 (6), 684-709.

Craig, L. & Siminski, P. (2010) 'Men's housework, women's housework and second births in Australia', Social Politics 17 (2), 235-266.

Dao, T.T. & LeResche, L. (2000) 'Gender differences in pain', Journal of Orofacial Pain, 14 (3), 169-95

De Vaus, D. Gray, M. Qu, L. & Stanton, D. (2009) 'The effect of relationship breakdown on income and social exclusion', paper presented to the Australian Social Policy Conference, University of New South Wales, Sydney, 8 July.

Diener, E. & Seligman, M.E.P. (2004) 'Beyond money: toward an economy of well-being', Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5 (1), 1-31.

Drobnic, S. & Guillen, A.M. (eds) (2011) Work-life Balance in Europe: The Role of Job Quality, Basingstoke, Palgrave Macmillan.

Easterlin, R.A. (1974) 'Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence'. In A.D. Paus & W.R. Melvind (eds), Nations and Households in Economic Growth: Essays in Honor of Moses Abramovitz, New York, Academic Press, Inc.

Emerick, I.J. (2006) 'Gender differences in the creation of different types of social capital: a multilevel study', Social Networks, 28 (1), 24-37.

Epstein, C. & Kalleberg, A.L (eds) (2004) Fighting for Time: Shifting Boundaries of Work and Social Life, New York, Russell Sage Foundation.

Fisher, K., Egerton, M., Gershuny, J.I. & Robinson, J.P. (2007) 'Gender convergence in the American Heritage Time Use Study (AHTUS)', Social Indicators Research, 82 (1), 1-33.

Fisher, K. & Robinson, J.P. (2010) Daily routines in 22 countries: diary evidence of average daily time spent in thirty activities, Technical Paper 2010-01, Centre for Time Use Research, University of Oxford.

Franklin, A. & Tranter, B. (2008) Loneliness in Australia, Housing and Community Research Unit Paper 13, University of Tasmania.

Gershuny, J.I. (2000) Changing Times: Work and Leisure in Postindustrial Society, Oxford, Oxford University Press.

Gershuny, J.I. (2011) Time-use surveys and the measurement of national well-being, Centre for Time-use Research, Department of Sociology, University of Oxford.

Gershuny, J.I. (2012) 'National utility: measuring the enjoyment of activities', European Sociological Review, On-line first, DOI 10.1093/esr/jcs077.

Gray, A. (2009) 'The social capital of older people', Ageing and Society, 29 (1), 5-31.

Heady, B. & Wearing, A. (1992) Understanding Happiness: A Theory of Subjective Well-being, Melbourne, Longman Cheshire.

Heady, B, Muffels, R. & Wooden, M. (2004) Money doesn't buy happiness ... or does it? A reconsideration based on the combined effects of wealth, income and consumption, Discussion Paper 1218, Bonn, Institute for the Study of Labor.

Helliwell, J.F. (2003) 'How's life? Combining individual and national variables to explain subjective well-being', Economic Modelling, 20 (2), 331-60.

Helliwell, J.F, Barrington-Leigh, C.P., Harris, A. & Huang, H. (2010) 'International evidence on the social context of well-being'. In E. Diener, J.F. Helliwell & D. Kahneman (eds) International Differences in Well-being, Oxford, Oxford University Press.

Hill, J.E., Jacob, J.I., Shannon, L.L., Brennan, R.T., Blanchard, V.L. & Martinengo, G. (2008) 'Exploring the relationship of workplace flexibility, gender, and life stage to family-to-work conflict, and stress and burnout', Community, Work & Family, 11 (2), 165-81.

Hochschild, A. R. (1979) 'Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure', American Journal of Sociology, 85 (3), 551-75.

Hochschild, A. (1983) The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feeling, Berkeley, University of California Press.

Hook, J.L. (2010) 'Gender inequality in the welfare state: sex segregation in housework, 1965-2003', American Journal of Sociology, 115 (5), 1480-1523.

Hutri, M. & Lindeman, M., (2002) 'The role of stress and negative emotions in an occupational crisis', Journal of Career Development, 29 (1), 19-36.

Inglehart, R., Foa, R., Peterson, C. & Welzel, C. (2008) 'Development, freedom, rising happiness: a global perspective, 1981-2007', Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3 (4), 264-85.

Juster, F.T. & Stafford, F.P. (1985) Time, Goods, and Well-being, Ann Arbor, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.

Kahneman, D. & Deaton, A. (2010) 'High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being', Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107 (38), 16489-16493.

Kahneman, D. & Krueger A.B. (2006) 'Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being', The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20 (1), 3-24.

Kahneman, D., Wakker, P. & Sarin, R. (1997) 'Back to Bentham? Explorations of experienced utility', Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112 (2), 375-405.

Kroll, C. & Pokutta, S. (2012) 'Just a perfect day? Developing a happiness optimised day schedule', Journal of Economic Psychology, On-line first, DOI 10.1016/j.joep.2012.09.015.

Krueger, A. & Schkade, D.A. (2007) 'The reliability of subjective well-being measures', Journal of Public Economics, 92 (8-9), 1833-1845.

Layard, P.R.G. (2005) Happiness: Lessons from a New Science, New York, Penguin Press.

Leigh, A. & Wolfers, J. (2006) 'Happiness and the Human Development Index: Australia is not a paradox', Australian Economic Review, 39 (2), 176-84.

Maher, J.M., Lindsay, J. & Franzway, S. (2008) 'Time, caring labour and social policy: understanding the family time economy in contemporary families', Work, Employment and Society, 22 (3), 547-58.

Mellor, D., Stokes, M., Firth, L., Hayashi, Y. & Cummins, R.A. (2008) 'Need for belonging, relationship satisfaction, loneliness, and life satisfaction', Personality and Individual Differences, 45 (3), 213-18.

Multinational Time Use Study, Versions World 5.5.3, 5.80 and 6.0 (released October 2011). Created by Gershuny, J. & Fisher, K. with Altintas, E., Borkosky, A., Bortnik, A., Dosman, D., Fedick, C., Frederick, T., Gauthier, A. H., Jones, S., Jun, J., Lai, A., Lin, Q., Lu, T., Lui, F. MacRae, L., Monna, B., Gimenez Nadal, J. I., Pauls, M., Pawlak, C., Shipley, A., Tinonin, C., Torres, N., Victorino, C. & Yeung, O. Centre for Time Use Research, University of Oxford, United Kingdom.

Offer, S. & Schneider, B.L. (2011) 'Revisiting the gender gap in time-use patterns: multitasking and well-being among mothers and fathers in dual-earner families', American Sociological Review, 76 (6), 809-833.

OECD (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development) (2011). StatExtracts, complete databases available via the OECD library, www.oecd.org (extracted 15 December 2011).

Oswald, A. (1997) 'Happiness and economic performance', Economic Journal, 107 (445), 1815-1831.

Patulny, R. (2009) 'The golden years? Social contact amongst retired men and women in Australia', Family Matters, (83), 39-47.

Patulny, R. (2012) 'The need for an emotion work survey', paper presented to the Australian Sociological Association Conference, Brisbane, 26-29 November.

Patulny, R. & Fisher, K. (2013) An implementation of an experimental time use methodology: an 'Australianised counterfactual' estimate of national subjective wellbeing in the USA, Technical Paper 2012-01, Centre for Time Use Research, University of Oxford.

Patulny, R & Wong, M. (2012) 'Poor mothers and lonely single males: the 'essentially' excluded women and men of Australia', Social Policy and Society, Available on CJO doi:10.1017S1474746412000449.

Rice, J.M, Goodin, R.E & Parpo, A. (2006) 'The temporal welfare state: a cross-national comparison', Journal of Public Policy, 26 (3), 195-228.

Robinson, J. & Godbey, G. (1997) Time for Life: The Surprising Ways Americans Use Their Time, University Park, Pennsylvania State University Press.

Robinson, J.P. & Michelson, W. (2010) 'Sleep as a victim of the "time crunch" --a multinational analysis', Electronic International Journal of Time Use Research, 7 (1), 61-72.

Seligman, M.E.P., Steen, T.A., Park, N. & Peterson, C. (2005) 'Positive psychology progress: empirical validation of interventions', American Psychologist, 60 (5), 410-21.

Sloane, P.J. & William, H. (2000) 'Job satisfaction, comparison earnings, and gender', Labour, 14 (3), 473-502.

Stevenson, B. & Wolfers, J. (2009) 'The paradox of declining female happiness', American Economic Journal: Economic Policy, 1 (2), 190-225.

Svanberg-Miller, A. (2004) 'Gender differences in subjective wellbeing: A cross-cultural perspective', Proceedings of the 6th Australian Conference on Quality of Life, Deakin University.

Tetlock, P.E. & Belkin, A. (Eds.) (1996) Counterfactual Thought Experiments in World Politics: Logical, Methodological, and Psychological Perspective, Princeton, Princeton University Press.

Van Wanrooy, B. & Wilson, S. (2006) 'Convincing the toilers? Dilemmas of working long hours in Australia', Work, Employment and Society, 20 (2), 349-68.

Endnotes

(1) Time and affect studies have been conducted in the United States (University of Michigan 1985 American's Use of Time Project, Kahneman and Krueger's 2006 PATS--Princeton Affect and Time Survey, a supplement of the Bureau of Labor Statistics 2010 American Time Use Survey); United Kingdom (1987 Unilever Time Diary Survey and 2011 Five Cities Project conducted by Trajectory with input from the Centre for Time Use Research); and France (a supplement of one sub-sample of the INSEE 2009-2010 Emploi du Temps, the French contribution to the second round of the Harmonised European Time Use Study).

(2) There is contention over whether Kahneman and Krueger's Day Reconstruction Method (DRM) of getting participants to recall and complete a diary of the previous days' activities and emotions is as robust as 'beeper studies', where participants electronically record their activities and emotions in real time when prompted by a beep or mobile phone app signal. Nevertheless, the DRM does collect emotion data very close to the actual experience, and also collects the full context of activities in sequence, unlike beeper surveys. As the DRM involves a series of prompts which enable participants to remember a great deal of contextual detail--where, what and with whom they were doing things--which serve as useful prompts for reducing ambiguity and increasing recall about how they kit. Further, a key strength of the DRM is that is not contemporaneous. It is therefore not subject to emotional response bias in recording emotions--such as being too upset or angry to bother recording that one feels upset or angry when randomly 'beeped'.
Table 1: Average minutes per day * spent by women and
men aged 18-64 in Australia and the USA on selected
activities on weekdays and weekend days, compared to
21 other countries **

                               Men

                   Australia            USA

Paid work          (5 of 23)         (8 of 23)
Weekdays        7 hours 28 min    7 hours 20 min
Weekend days    2 hours 22 min     2 hours 7 min

Child care         (1 of 23)         (3 of 23)
Weekdays            29 min            25 min
Weekend days        40 min            28 min

Housework          (8 of 23)       (=6/7 of 23)
Weekdays         1 hour 45 min     1 hour 51 min
Weekend days    2 hours 51 min     3 hours 2 min

Volunteering      (20 of 23)       (=1/2 of 23)
Weekdays             8 min            20 min
Weekend days        12 min            46 min

Socialising       (19 of 23)        (15 of 23)
Weekdays            48 min         1 hour 19 min
Weekend days     2 hours 0 min    2 hours 35 min

                               Women

                   Australia            USA

Paid work         (12 of 23)         (6 of 23
Weekdays        4 hours 20 min     5 hours 9 min
Weekend days     1 hour 8 min      1 hour 21 min

Child care         (1 of 23)         (3 of 23)
Weekdays         1 hour 28 min     1 hour 3 min
Weekend days     1 hour 16 min        47 min

Housework        (=9/10 of 23)      (20 of 23)
Weekdays        3 hours 39 min     3 hours 1 min
Weekend days    4 hours 13 min     4 hours 5 min

Volunteering      (13 of 23)         (2 of 23)
Weekdays            14 min            29 min
Weekend days        13 min            52 min

Socialising       (17 of 23)         (8 of 23)
Weekdays            56 min         1 hour 28 min
Weekend days     1 hour 55 min    2 hours 33 min

Source: Fisher & Robinson 2010

* These average time figures are weighted to
account for sample errors and to evenly distribute
days of the week. These means reflect time spent
per person across the whole population small time
in volunteering reflects the low participation
rate on any given day the original source contains
participation rates as well as average time per
day in activities.

** Countries covered in tables: Australia,
Belgium, Brazil, Bulgaria, Canada, Estonia,
Finland, France, Germany, Italy, Latvia,
Lithuania, Netherlands, Norway, Poland, Republic
of Korea, Slovenia, Spain, Sweden, Turkey, United
Kingdom, United States--the number of 23 in the
top of each cell reflects the rank in total time
commitment 1 represents the country where people
of that gender spend the most time in that
activity.

Table 2: Example PATS diary

Time            Main Activity     Where    Who with

04:00-07:00         Sleep         Home     Partner
07:00-07:30     Shower, dress     Home      Alone
07:30:08:00     Eat breakfast     Home     Partner
08:00-09:00        Commute        Train   Strangers
09:00-12:00          Work         Work    Colleagues
12:00-13:00       Eat lunch       Work      Alone
13:00-17:00          Work         Work    Colleagues
17:00-18:30    Drink, socialise    Pub     Friends
18:30-19:00          Cook         Home     Partner
19:00-19:30       Eat dinner      Home     Partner
19:30-21:00        Watch TV       Home     Partner
21:00-21:30          Read         Home     Partner
21:30-04:00         Sleep         Home     Partner

Time           Happy   Sad   Stressed   U-index

04:00-07:00
07:00-07:30      4      2       2          0
07:30:08:00
08:00-09:00
09:00-12:00
12:00-13:00      5      1       1          0
13:00-17:00      3      2       4          1
17:00-18:30
18:30-19:00
19:00-19:30
19:30-21:00
21:00-21:30
21:30-04:00

Note: proportion of PATS episodes where negative emotion rating >
positive emotion rating > around 20%"

Table 3: Weighted OLS Regressions on unpleasant and pleasant time

                                          Unpleasant Time

                              Country            Country+ Gender
                              + Gender        + Socio-demographics

                       B       S.E     Sig     B       S.E     Sig

Intercept            563.8                   720.5
US men               -106.0    4.7     ***   -97.4     4.0
Australian women      23.0     4.5     ***    21.5     4.1     ***
Australian men        3.0      6.5            14.0     5.6     **
Single, no child                             121.8     3.7     ***
Single, has child                             19.6     6.2     ***
Couple, no child                             -17.3     3.6     ***
Aged 35 years                                -25.9     3.3     ***
  or less
Aged 65 years                                197.5     6.6     ***
  or more
Employed                                     109.7     4.1     ***
Student                                      -106.5    6.0     ***
Retired                                      -132.6    7.3     ***
No secondary                                 119.0     3.9     ***
  education
Post secondary                               -137.1    3.3     ***
  education
N                             24,996                  24,996
Adjusted [R.sup.2]             0.04                    0.31

                                         Pleasant Time

                             Country            Country+ Gender
                             + Gender        + Socio-demographics

                       B      S.E     Sig     B       S.E     Sig

Intercept            354.1                  171.4
US men               121.5    4.4     ***   107.4     3.6     ***
Australian women     -14.1    4.3     ***    38.3     3.6     ***
Australian men       -16.9    6.2     ***   -28.1     4.9     ***
Single, no child                            132.3     3.3     ***
Single, has child                           -29.5     5.4     ***
Couple, no child                             7.4      3.1      **
Aged 35 years                                3.1      2.9
  or less
Aged 65 years                               190.7     5.8     ***
  or more
Employed                                    153.7     3.7     ***
Student                                     139.5     5.3     ***
Retired                                     156.5     6.4     ***
No secondary                                -129.9    3.4     ***
  education
Post secondary                              156.8     2.9     ***
  education
N                            24,996                  24,996
Adjusted [R.sup.2]            0.05                    0.41

Reference Category--US women, in couple with dependent child,
aged 35 to 65, working age but not in labour force, completed
secondary education but no further qualifications
* P < 0.05 ** P < 0.01 *** P < 0.001
联系我们|关于我们|网站声明
国家哲学社会科学文献中心版权所有