Examination of the motivational climate in the athletic training room.
Brinkman-Majewski, Rachel E. ; Weiss, Windee M.
Returning injured athletes back to their original playing status,
as soon as possible, is a main objective for athletic trainers. Many
factors play a role in how an athlete progresses through their injury
rehabilitation. Some factors cannot be controlled by the athletic
trainer, whereas, others may be within their control. For instance,
situational factors (e.g., injury type, severity of injury) and external
factors (e.g., life stress, social support) have proven influential on
thoughts, actions, and recovery outcomes of the injured athlete (Brewer,
1994). Another factor that may have an effect on rehabilitation is the
overall environment of the athletic training room. This environment can
be described in terms of the motivational climate. Ames (1992a)
highlighted the importance of the motivational climate in changing
one's perceptions, influence, and actions within achievement
setting such as the classroom, sports field, or athletic training room.
A performance motivational setting emphasizes norm-referenced success
and performance outcomes, while a mastery motivational climate
emphasizes learning, effort, improvement, and success determined by
self-reference criteria (Ames, 1992a). Individuals will perceive a
setting as either a performance or mastery motivational climate
depending on the environmental structure put in place by the authority
figure (e.g., teachers, coaches, athletic trainers) (Ames, 1992a).
Previous research on the motivational climate in the sport setting
has shown a relationship between the motivational climate perceived by
the athlete and their psychosocial beliefs (Seifriz, Duda, & Chi,
1992; Treasure & Roberts, 1998). In two studies with basketball
players, perceptions of a mastery motivational climate promoted
enjoyment and intrinsic motivation (Seifriz et al., 1992) and
satisfaction and positive beliefs about success (Treasure & Roberts,
1998), while perceptions of a performance climate were related to
increased anxiety and getting punished for mistakes (Seifriz et al.,
1992), and only being satisfied with normative success (e.g., playing
better than a teammate) (Treasure & Roberts, 1998).
Based on these findings on the influence of the motivational
climate in sport settings, it is possible that the motivational climate
also has a role in other settings, such as injury rehabilitation. If the
perception of the motivational climate in the athletic training room has
an influence on athletes' psychological beliefs (e.g., enjoyment,
intrinsic motivation, anxiety), then potentially the athletic trainer
has the ability to generate the most conducive motivational climate in
the rehabilitation setting. Athletic settings have a structural system
with an adult authority figure that creates a recognized reward system
(Ames, 1992a).
The structure and reward system of a learning environment is what
ultimately creates the motivational climate (Ames, 1992b). The athletic
trainer holds the authority position in the athletic training room just
as the coach in the athletic environment. Thus, the athletic
trainers' rules, reprimands, and behaviors construct the
motivational climate in the athletic training room.
The possibility of improving rehabilitation adherence and outcomes
could then perhaps be determined by the motivational climate the
athletic trainer creates. With the findings (Seiffiz et al., 1992;
Treasure & Roberts, 1998) reported in sports settings, it would seem
more favorable to create a mastery climate in rehabilitation settings
which should promote higher levels of enjoyment and intrinsic
motivation, a trusted interaction with the athletic trainer, and the
belief that success is related to effort and self-improvement during
injury rehabilitation. By improving these psychosocial beliefs in the
athletes, an improved adherence rate with their rehabilitation should
follow. Injured athletes who responded to their rehabilitation with a
higher level of self-motivation, belief that their treatment was
successful, greater perceptions of social support, and a sense of
personal control over their recovery, completed the rehabilitation
exercises more often than those who did not respond to recovery with
these situational and personal factors (Duda, Smart, & Tappe, 1989).
Investigation of the relationship between adherence rates and
rehabilitation outcomes in sport injury recovery is limited. The
available research has produced mixed results with an early report that
adherence measures did not significantly correlate with outcome measures
in ACL reconstruction surgery (Brewer, Van Raalte, Cornelius, &
Petitpas, 2000). However, in a more recent study (Brewer et al., 2004)
relating rehabilitation adherence and ACL reconstruction outcomes, the
patients who attended more rehabilitation sessions and who were rated
higher by their therapists on an adherence scale experienced fewer
adverse knee symptoms compared to non-adhering patients. Therefore,
improvements in sport injury rehabilitation outcomes such as pain,
function, and return to play may result from increased adherence quality
and rates of athletes.
With the connections of a mastery motivational climate to improved
psychosocial beliefs to increased adherence rates and finally to
enhanced rehabilitation outcomes, athletic trainers should be able to
modify their actions and generate a mastery motivational climate for
their athletes. But what individual characteristics cause some athletes
to perceive the motivational climate differently than others? Previous
research examined individuals' goal orientations. By Nicholls'
(1984) theory, people regard their ability as the center of one of two
different goal types. There are those individuals who judge their
personal success through social comparison or by norm-referenced means,
which has been termed as ego-involved. Other individuals believe their
success is based on personal improvement or a product of self-referenced
goals, which has been known as task-involved. Goal orientation is
specific to individual beliefs about self, while the motivational
climate describes an environments' atmosphere as a whole. The
motivational climate attributes (i.e., mastery and performance) are
closely related to goal orientation characteristics (i.e., task and
ego). A pattern emerged in a study (Newton & Duda, 1999) examining
the interaction between the motivational climate and goal orientations
with athletes' perceived ability and beliefs about causes of
success. Ego-involved orientation and performance climate were related
to beliefs about success being ability focused, whereas effort centered
beliefs of success were predicted by an interaction between
task-involved orientation and a mastery climate.
The motivational climate in the rehabilitation setting, along with
athlete's psychosocial beliefs about the motivational climate has
yet to be examined. Therefore, the aim of our study is to determine if
differences in athletes' personal characteristics are related to
their perceptions of the motivational climate and to identify whether
perceptions of the motivational climate in the athletic training room
are related to athletes' individual goal orientation. We
hypothesized differences in the athletes' goal orientations and
other personal characteristics (e.g., gender, starter status, injury
status) will be related to differences in their perception of the
motivational climate in the athletic training room. A third purpose is
to determine if higher perceptions of a mastery climate predicts greater
rehabilitation enjoyment and perceived competence, while a higher
perception of a performance climate will predict lower perceptions of
competence and enjoyment with rehabilitation.
Methods
Participants
Both male (n = 92) and female (n = 108) intercollegiate athletes
from 13 different sports volunteered to complete the research
questionnaire at one university. Participants ranged in age from 18 to
26 years (M= 20.0, SD = 1.4), with 59.0% of the athletes reported being
a "starter," 24.0% non-starter status, and 11.0% reported
their status as a redshirt or medical hardship. Of the participants,
77.0% had sustained an injury during their collegiate career, and 90.0%
(n = 180) reported having received treatment/rehabilitation from an
athletic trainer during their collegiate career. Therefore, final
analyses included only these athletes who had interacted with an
athletic trainer (males n = 87, females n = 93). Additionally, 56.0%
reported an occurrence of an injury during their current competitive
season. Athletes were predominately Caucasian (90.8%) with African
American (8.2%) and Hispanic (1.0%) also represented.
Procedure
Upon receiving IRB approval, head coaches of each competitive team
were asked to cooperate with the investigator to schedule a team
meeting. At the meeting, only the primary investigator and participants
were present. Athletes were given a brief description of the study,
provided an explanation of the procedures, and reminded that their
participation was voluntary. Only those athletes who agreed to
participate were asked to read and sign the informed consent. Once
signed, the questionnaires were distributed and completed.
Measures
Perceived motivational climate in the athletic training room. The
motivational climate in the athletic training room as perceived by the
intercollegiate athletes was assessed using the Perceived Motivational
Climate in Sport Questionnaire-2 (PMCSQ-2) (Newton, Duda, & Yin,
2000). The PMCSQ-2 was designed to have two principle scales
(performance vs. mastery involving climates) and each of those to have
three subscales Perceptions of a performance climate include (a)
intra-team member rivalry (e.g., teammates competing against each
other), (b) unequal recognition (e.g., only starters receive attention),
(c) punishment for mistakes (e.g., athletes are ridiculed for error).
Perceptions of a mastery climate include (a) cooperative learning (e.g.,
athletes are encouraged to work together), (b) effort/improvement (e.g.,
improvement is rewarded), (c) important role (e.g., each athlete is
treated like an important member of the team). The PMCSQ-2 evaluated the
perceptions of mastery versus performance oriented climates in the
athletic training room. Athletes were asked to think of the general
atmosphere in the athletic training room and then asked to rate their
agreement on the 33 items designed to tap the six subscales. Mean scores
for each subscale were then calculated. Slight wording modifications
were used to be specific to rehabilitation in the athletic training
room. Each statement began with "In this athletic training
room..." and ended with different items related to either a mastery
motivational climate (e.g., "... athletes feel successful when they
improve") or a performance motivational climate (e.g., "...
the athletic trainer has their favorites"). Athletes rated their
level of agreement on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Task and ego orientation in sport. The Task and Ego Orientation in
Sport Questiomiaire (TEOSQ) (Duda, 1989) was used to measure each
athlete's goal orientation in sport. Athletes were asked to think
of a time when they felt most successful in their sport and respond to
13 items related to task- and ego-oriented criteria. All items began
with the statement "I feel most successful in sport when..."
and ended with either a task-oriented phrase (e.g., "... 1 learn a
new skill and it makes me want to practice more") or an
ego-oriented phrase (e.g., "... 1 am the only one who can do the
play or skill"). A 5-point Likert-type scale was used to measure
their responses, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
Athletes' mean scores were then calculated for each subscale, with
scores ranging from high to low for both task- and ego-orientation.
Sport rehabilitation enjoyment. The level of sport rehabilitation
enjoyment was measured through a modified version of the sport enjoyment
scale (Scanlan, Simons, Carpenter, Schmidt, & Keeler, 1993). The
term "rehabilitation" replaced "sport" in order to
adapt the instrument accordingly. The following three statements
assessed sport rehabilitation enjoyment: "How much fun is
rehabilitation for you?"; "How much do you like your
rehabilitation?"; "How much do you enjoy rehabilitation?"
A 5-point Likert-type scale was used ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5
(very much so).
Perceived rehabilitation competence. The athletes' level of
competence in rehabilitation was assessed using items from the athletic
subscale of the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter, 1988).
The 5 items were modified to say "Some athletes ..." rather
than "Some teenagers ... and "sport" was replaced with
"rehabilitation." The items were completed using a structured
alternative response format, having the athletes first decide which of
the two statements was more true for them, and then whether that
statement was either "sort of" or "really" true for
them (Harter, 1988). Scores for the items ranged from 1 (low perceived
ability) to 4 (high perceived ability).
Athlete demographics. Athletes completed additional demographic
information including age, gender, race, sport, starter status, injury
status, injury history, and treatment status questions.
Statistical Analysis
All data was analyzed using SPSS. Preliminary analyses included
frequencies, descriptives, reliabilities, and correlations. Following
preliminary analyses, a series of multivariate analysis of variance
(MANO VA) were conducted to determine any demographic differences on the
athletes' perceptions of the motivational climate in the athletic
training room. For all analyses, the PMCSQ-2 subscales (cooperative
learning, effort/improvement, important role [mastery climate] and
intra-team member rivalry, unequal recognition, punishment for mistakes
[performance climate]) were the dependent variables. Athlete demographic
information was considered the independent variables.
Second, in order to determine if the athletes' goal
orientations play a role in their perceptions of the motivational
climate, a MANOVA was conducted. Athletes were grouped based on z-scores
of their task and ego goal orientations. These subsequent groups of
athletes were then compared on their perceptions of the motivational
climate. Particularly, the grouped athletes were compared on their
mastery climate perceptions of cooperative learning, effort/improvement,
and important role, and on their performance climate perceptions of
intra-team member rivalry, unequal recognition, and punishment for
mistakes. Due to the number of analyses involving the same motivational
constructs, a Bonferroni adjustment was made, and the significance level
was set at p < .01 for all MANOVAs.
Lastly, separate multiple regression analyses were conducted to
determine if the athletes' perceptions of the motivational climate
predicted psychosocial beliefs about rehabilitation. Level of enjoyment
and perceived competence for rehabilitation were considered dependent
variables, while the six motivational climate subscales (cooperative
learning, effort/improvement, important role, intra-team member rivalry,
unequal recognition, and punishment for mistakes) were independent
variables. The significance level was set at p < .05.
Results
Scale Reliabilities
Alpha coefficients were calculated to determine scale reliabilities
for all constructs: six motivational climate subscales, two goal
orientation subscales, perceived competence, and enjoyment. All
reliability values were acceptable, demonstrating alphas [greater than
or equal to] .70, with the exception of one subscale for the
performance-related motivational climate, intra-team member rivalry
([alpha] = .36). Due to the low reliability for this subscale,
intra-team member rivalry was not used in further analysis. Table 1
shows alpha coefficients along the diagonal in italics, and correlations
among the variables below the diagonal.
Demographic Characteristics and Perceptions of the Motivational
Climate
Demographic differences were examined on the five subscales for the
motivational climate through a series of MANOVA's. The MANOVA for
gender differences was significant: Wilks' [lambda] = .91, F (5,
174) = 3.59,p < .005. A total of 9% of the variance of perceptions of
the motivational was accounted for by gender differences. Male and
female athletes differed significantly on perceptions of important role,
unequal recognition, and punishment for mistakes (See Table 2). Analysis
of the means revealed female athletes' perceptions of important
role were significantly higher than males' perceptions, whereas the
male athletes had significantly higher perceptions of unequal
recognition and punishment for mistakes compared to female athletes.
A second MANOVA was conducted to determine if there was a
difference between athletes' of varying starter status and their
perceptions of the motivational climate in the athletic training room.
The MANOVA was significant: Wilks' [lambda] = .88, F (10, 324) =
2.24,p < .02, with a 12% of the variance being accounted for by group
differences. Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations by starter
status for all motivational climate constructs. Differences emerged
between the groups on unequal recognition. Post-Hoc Tukey tests revealed
that non-starters had significantly higher perceptions of unequal
recognition compared to starters. Ultimately, non-starters were more
likely to perceive athletic trainers as having their favorites.
Lastly, a series of MANOVAs were conducted to compare
athletes' injury and surgical history and their perceptions of the
motivational climate in the athletic training room. With regards to the
athletes' injury history during college and the athletes'
perceptions of the motivational climate, the MANOVA was non-significant:
Wilks' [lambda] = .97, F (5, 174) = 1.23, p = .30. The MANOVA for
sustaining an injury in the current competitive season was also
non-significant: Wilks' [lambda] = .98, F (5, 174) = .12, p = .61.
Therefore, the athletes' motivational climate perceptions were not
dependent on whether the athlete had or had not experienced an injury
while at college and/or during the current competitive season. The
MANOVA for history of surgery was also non-significant: Wilks'
[lambda] = .99, F (5, 170) = .51,/? = .77. Thus, no differences in
perceptions of the motivational climate existed between those who had
surgery and those who had not.
Task and Ego Goal Orientations and Perceptions of the Motivational
Climate
In order to compare the athletes' of varying goal orientations
on their perceptions of the motivational climate in the athletic
training room, athletes needed to be grouped according to their task and
ego orientation responses. First, each task and ego subscale was
converted to a z-score. Second, the athletes were classified into one of
three groups for both task and ego subscales (low, moderate, high) based
on [+ or -] 1 standard deviation (Newton & Duda, 1999). In order to
group the athletes by the combination of task and ego goal orientations,
9 groups were formed (low/moderate/high task x low/moderate/high ego).
Of these 9 groups, 4 groups were selected because they represented the
most distinct goal orientation profiles: (a) low task--low ego (n = 35),
(b) low task--high ego (n = 34), (c) high task--low ego (n = 23), (d)
high task--high ego (n = 29). Only those athletes demonstrating these
four goal orientation profiles were included in subsequent analyses. See
Table 4 for means and standard deviations by goal orientation group.
A MANOVA was conducted to determine if athletes with different goal
orientations differed on their perceptions of the motivational climate
in the athletic training room. The MANOVA was significant: Wilks'
[lambda] = .55, F (15, 312.35) = 5.01, p < .0001. A total of 45% of
the variance of motivational climate perceptions was accounted for by
group differences. Significant differences were found for each of the
motivational climate subscales: cooperative learning,
effort/improvement, important role, unequal recognition, and punishment
for mistakes. In regards to differences on mastery climate perceptions,
Post-Hoc Tukey test revealed, (a) low task--high ego athletes had
significantly lower perceptions of cooperative learning than both high
task--low ego and high task--high ego groups, who were not different
from each other, (b) both high task groups had significantly higher
effort/ improvement perceptions than both low task groups, and (c) high
task--low ego athletes had significantly higher perceptions of important
role compared to all other groups of athletes, with high task--high ego
athletes reporting higher perceptions than low task--high ego athletes.
Additionally, goal orientation differences existed on performance
motivational climate perceptions, (a) high task--low ego athletes had
significantly lower perceptions of unequal recognition compared to all
other groups, and high task--high ego athletes had lower perceptions of
unequal recognition than low task--high ego athletes, and (b) high
task--low ego goal orientation athletes had significantly lower
perceptions of punishment for mistakes compared to all other groups of
athletes.
Perceptions of the Motivational Climate Predicting Level of
Enjoyment and Perceived Competence in Rehabilitation
Separate multiple regression analyses were conducted to determine
the relationship between athletes' perceptions of the motivational
climate and athletes' level of enjoyment and perceived competence
for rehabilitation. The regression on level of enjoyment was not
significant: F (5, 174) = 1.87, p = .10. The relationship between the
motivational climate perceptions and perceived competence in
rehabilitation was also not significant: F (5, 162) = 1.96 ,p = .09.
Athletes' perceptions of rehabilitation competence were not
predicted by perceptions of the motivational climate.
Discussion
The goal of this research was to specifically examining whether
different personal characteristics, along with the athletes' goal
orientation, would influence their perceptions of the motivational
climate. Another purpose was to identify if the athletes'
perceptions of the motivational climate in the athletic training room
were related to their levels of enjoyment and perceived competence with
rehabilitation. Results revealed certain athlete characteristics were
associated with specific motivational climate subscale perceptions while
other characteristics were not related. Numerous findings emerged with
relationships between goal orientation and perceptions of the
motivational climate. When examining psychosocial beliefs, the
motivational climate did not predict rehabilitation enjoyment, or
perceived competence in rehabilitation.
In regards to demographic findings, gender differences emerged on
perceptions of the motivational climate in the athletic training room.
Females had greater perceptions of the mastery motivational climate
subscale important role, while males held higher perceptions of the
performance climate subscales unequal recognition and punishment for
mistakes, in the athletic training room. Previous research on the
motivational climate in sport did not report differences between male
and female perceptions; however, there are explanations for this
finding. A cognitive-mediational model on coaches' behavior (Smoll
& Smith, 1989) suggested that players' interpretations of the
coaches' behaviors are influenced by their own personal
characteristics (e.g., gender, age, race, etc). Thus, the athletes'
perceptions of the athletic trainers' actions may also be
influenced by similar characteristics such as gender. Another
explanation for the differences in gender may be due to the athletic
trainer creating a motivational climate which accommodates to the gender
of athletes. Previous research (Gilbert & Trudel, 2004) found that
coaches identified gender of the athlete as an important influence on
how they conducted practice. Such gender differential expectations may
also affect athletic trainers' decisions and behaviors in the
athletic training room. Additionally the athletes' perceptions of
the motivational climate may have been influenced by the gender of the
athletic trainer or possibly each athletic trainer's specific
method of giving feedback during treatment and rehabilitation. This may
warrant further research involving how the athletic trainers generate
the motivational climate (mastery vs. performance) in regards to the
gender of both the athletic trainer and the athlete.
Athletes' starter status also showed differences in
perceptions of the motivational climate, specifically indicating that
non-starters felt a higher level of unequal recognition from the
athletic trainer than did the starters. Horn (1984) and Rejeski,
Darracoot, and Hutslar (1979) compared youth coaching behaviors directed
toward higher-ability athletes and lower-ability athletes, and reported
there was a difference in the type and amount of feedback received from
the coach. Interestingly, those athletes categorized as lower-ability
were given more instruction and feedback, both in general and for
mistakes, than the higher-ability athletes. However, the higher-ability
athletes experienced more athlete-coach interactions than the
lower-ability athletes. A possible reason for lower-ability athletes to
be given more feedback was due to the youth competitive level and
coaches giving more assistance to those players who needed the help, or
the lower-ability athletes (Horn, 1984). In the current study, athletes
competing at the collegiate level may not perceive the biased
instruction and feedback from their athletic trainer, and only interpret
the frequency of athlete-athletic trainer interaction on a regular
basis. Further research exploring the amount and quality of time
athletic trainers give to differing starter status athletes along with
perceptions of the motivational climate should be carried out to gain
better insight.
The athletes' perceptions of the motivational climate were not
dependent on whether they had experienced an injury, in the past or
currently, or had a history of surgery. It was hypothesized that
athletes working closely with the athletic trainers (i.e., those injured
or post-surgery completing rehabilitation) would perceive the athletic
training room as a climate that rewards hard work and persistence rather
than effortless recognition. Previous research in sport (Fry &
Newton, 2003; Newton & Duda, 1999; Pensgaard & Roberts, 2000;
Treasure & Roberts, 1998) revealed that individuals perceiving a
mastery climate had higher enjoyment, intrinsic motivation, and a belief
that one's own effort was the cause for success. Creating a climate
where effort and improvement is the main goal for rehabilitating
athletes would provide a more enjoyable rehabilitation, better adherence
rates, and possibly better rehabilitation outcomes (Brinkman &
Weiss, 2010). For this study, injured and post-surgical athletes did not
have significantly different motivational climate perceptions compared
to non-injured athletes. Speculation can be made that the motivational
climate is not influenced by working directly with an athletic trainer
during rehabilitation, but rather by the daily interactions with the
athletic trainer and the overall atmosphere they create in the athletic
training room.
In the sport setting (Seifriz et al., 1992) a relationship was
reported between perceptions of a mastery motivational climate and a
task goal orientation, as well as an association between perceptions of
a performance climate and an ego goal orientation. The current research
supports that the same is true in the athletic training room. First, in
regards to mastery motivational climate perceptions and differences
among athletes' goal orientations, those athletes with high task
orientations, regardless of ego orientation, had the highest perceptions
of effort/improvement in the athletic training room. Task oriented
athletes judged their success based on their own effort and individual
improvements, similar to past research in the sport setting (Newton
& Duda, 1999). For other mastery perceptions, higher task athletes
had higher perceptions of important role and cooperative learning.
Again, both high task groups were related to greater perceptions of a
climate focused on learning and skill development. It is interesting
that for important role, the high task--low ego athletes'
perceptions were higher than all other groups, even the high task--high
ego athletes. This implies that having a high ego orientation does not
overshadow a high task orientation. However, for cooperative learning
the higher task athletes' perceptions were only greater than the
low task--high ego, but not the low task--low ego athletes. Different
from the effort/improvement and important role subscales, perceptions of
cooperative learning did vary depending on whether the athletes had high
or low ego goal orientations.
For performance motivational climate perceptions and goal
orientations, findings again supported past research (Newton & Duda,
1999) of ego oriented athletes having higher perceptions of a climate
centered on norm-reference success compared to task oriented athletes.
Unequal recognition perceptions were lowest in athletes with a high
task--low ego orientation, and athletes with high task--high ego had
lower perceptions of unequal recognition than low task--high ego
athletes. Likewise, athletes with a high task--low ego goal orientation
had the lowest perceptions of punishment for mistakes. The two groups
represented with these findings hold the most extremes of the task and
ego goal orientation and may be the reason for their significance. It is
important to further explain the significance of an athlete's goal
orientation in rehabilitation. For most injury rehabilitations, the
exercises to be completed and goals to be met can be considered simple
or unchallenging for most, if not all, athletes. In their normal
competitive setting, athletes are giving their all in order to "be
the best," "win the game," and "take home the
championship." However, rehabilitation can take athletes down to a
level where they need to get motivated to do a task as simple as quad
sets. Athletic trainers need the athletes to feel successful when the
only improvement they may make during a rehabilitation session is
increasing their knee flexion by five degrees. Increasing an
athlete's task goal orientation may be the way to accomplish this.
With more of a task orientation, the athlete perhaps would perceive more
of a mastery climate, and in turn, be more motivated, feel satisfied
with simple successes, and ultimately produce enhanced outcomes with
rehabilitation. Further research examining the relationship between
motivational climate perceptions and athletes' goal orientation may
help in determining why specific task and ego goal orientations are
related to certain mastery and performance perceptions of the
motivational climate and how these characteristics can lead to improved
rehabilitation adherence and outcomes.
Another objective of this research was to attempt to link the
motivational climate in the athletic training room to athletes'
psychosocial beliefs; for this study, level of enjoyment and perceived
competence with rehabilitation. Past research in sport (Seifriz et al.,
1992; Theeboom, De Knop, & Weiss, 1995) reported individuals were
more likely to enjoy their activity when either a learning and
improvement based climate was implemented or when perceptions of a
climate evaluating success as self-referenced were higher than
perceptions of a climate with norm-referenced evaluated success. This
study did not find the motivational climate to predict levels of
enjoyment with rehabilitation; however, future studies should focus on
the possibility of athletes' perceptions of the motivational
climate predicting their psychosocial beliefs such as enjoyment.
Additionally for this study, perceived competence in rehabilitation was
not predicted by the motivational climate, which supports Theeboom et
al. (1995) findings: children's perceived competence in martial
arts skills, when tested, did not differ depending instruction in either
a mastery or performance climate; yet the interview data revealed
children in the personal improvement and self comparison (i.e., mastery)
program believed they were able to learn new skills quickly, where as
children in the traditional norm-comparison group (i.e., performance)
believed it took a longer time to learn new skills. The same may be true
for the current research, and the measure (Harter, 1988) used to examine
perceived competence in sport rehabilitation did not tap the
athletes' apparent capability with rehabilitation. Further research
with other measures and techniques to better understand athletes'
perceived competence in sport rehabilitation is needed.
We acknowledge there are limitations to this research beyond those
commonly present with data collection through survey (i.e., participant
honesty, survey errors, etc.). The small sample was made up of all
competitive level athletes without the inclusion of recreational
athletes and general public patients. This in mm provided only for data
on perceptions of the motivational climate in a collegiate athletic
training room and not in a clinic or hospital setting. The combination
of these limitations may have played a role in our overall results, but
this research should launch several other investigations to further
explore the influence of the motivational climate in the rehabilitation
settings with varying clientele--age, competitive level, activity level;
and should examine additional constructs and outcomes. Other
psychosocial belief factors, such as satisfaction and self-efficacy with
rehabilitation, may be related to the motivational climate, which may
also lead to better adherence to the rehabilitation program. Examining
predictors of adherence constructs, such as motivation, commitment, and
emotional responses to injury, may provide better results to make the
link between increased adherence and improved outcomes. Duda et al.
(1989) reported athletes had better adherence to rehabilitation when
they had a higher level of self-motivation, belief that their treatment
was successful, a sense of personal control, and high perceptions of
social support. High social support from the physiotherapist was a
predictor of higher rehabilitation adherence rates of athletes (Levy,
Polman, & Borkoles, 2008). Further research with these psychosocial
factors as the focus is encouraged. Brinkman and Weiss (2010) outlined
strategies for the athletic trainer to promote a mastery motivational
climate in the rehabilitation setting that would provide a supportive
and conducive environment for the athletes' rehabilitation.
Subsequently, the athletes' psychosocial beliefs would facilitate
higher adherence rates and ultimately a quicker and enhanced recovery
with higher quality outcomes.
Conclusion
This study revealed that the motivational climate is present in the
athletic training room and can be predicted by athletes' goal
orientation. The variations of perceptions found with differing
demographics and individual goal orientations suggest that
athletes' personal characteristics influence their perceptions of
the mastery and performance climates. With the knowledge of the
motivational climate existing in the athletic training room, athletic
trainers now have the awareness of their words and actions creating an
environment in which athletes receive treatment and rehabilitation.
Depending on their goal orientation (i.e., task vs ego orientation) or
individual demographic characteristics (e.g., gender, starter status,
race), certain athletes will have differing psychosocial beliefs about
one motivational climate compared to another, and they may respond and
adhere better to treatment and rehabilitation when that environment is
present. It will be beneficial to the athlete and athletic trainer for
the generated motivational climate to have the most conducive atmosphere
possible for rehabilitation in order to increase adherence rates of
athletes and in turn produce better outcomes.
References
Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and
motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and
exercise (pp. 161-176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ames, C. (1992b). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261-271.
Brewer, B.W. (1994). Review and critique of models of psychological
adjustment to athletic injury. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 6,
87-100.
Brewer, B.W. Cornelius, A.E., Van Raalte, J.L., Brickner, J.C.,
Sklar, J.H., Corsetti, J.R., Pohlman, M.H., Ditmar, T.D., & Emery,
K.(2004). Rehabilitation adherence and anterior cruciate ligament
reconstruction outcome. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 9 (4),
163-175.
Brewer, B.W., Van Raalte, J.L., Cornelius, A.E., & Petitpas,
A.J. (2000). Psychological factors, rehabilitation adherence, and
rehabilitation outcome after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction.
Rehabilitation Psychology, 45 (1), 30-37.
Brinkman, R.E., & Weiss, W.M. (2010). The motivational climate
in the rehabilitation setting. Athletic Therapy Today, 15 (2), 44-46.
Duda, J.L. (1989). Relationship between task and ego orientation
and the perceived purpose of sport among high school athletes. Journal
of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 11, 318-335.
Duda, J. L., Smart, A. E., & Tappe, M. K. (1989). Predictors of
adherence in the rehabilitation of athletic injuries: An application of
personal investment theory. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
77, 367-381.
Fry, M.D. & Newton, M. (2003). Application of achievement goal
theory in an urban youth tennis setting. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 15, 50-66.
Gilbert, W.D., & Trudel, P. (2004). Role of the coach: How
model youth team sport coaches frame their roles. The Sport
Psychologist, 18, 21-43.
Harter, S. (1988). Causes, correlates and the functional role of
global self-worth: A life-span perspective. In J. Kolligian & R.
Sternberg (Eds.), Perceptions of competence and incompetence across the
life-span (pp. 206-242). New Haven, CT: Yale University.
Horn, T.S. (1984). Expectancy effects in the interscholastic
athletic setting: Methodological considerations. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 7, 60-76.
Levy, A.R., Polman, R.C.J., & Borkoles, E. (2008). Examining
the relationship between perceived autonomy support and age in the
context of rehabilitation adherence in sport. Rehabilitation Psychology,
53, 224-230.
Newton, M., & Duda, J. L. (1999). The interaction of
motivational climate, dispositional goal orientations, and perceived
ability in predicting indices of motivation. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 30, 63-82.
Newton, M., Duda, J. L., & Yin, Z. (2000). Examination of the
psychometric properties of the perceived motivational climate in sport
questionnaire-2 in a sample of female athletes. Journal of Sports
Sciences, 18, 275-290.
Nicholls, J.G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of
ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance.
Psychological Review, 91, 328-346.
Pensgaard, A.M., & Roberts, G.C. (2000). The relationship
between the motivational climate, perceived ability and sources of
distress among elite athletes. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 191-200.
Rejeski, W., Darracott, C., & Hutslar, S. (1979). Pygmalion in
youth sports: A field study. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 311-319.
Scanlan, T. K., Simons, J. R, Carpenter, P. J., Schmidt, G. W.,
& Keeler, B. (1993). The sport commitment model: Measurement
development for the youth-sport domain. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 15, 16-38.
Seifriz, J.J., Duda, J.L., & Chi, L. (1992). The relationship
of perceived motivational climate to intrinsic motivation and beliefs
about success in basketball. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,
14, 375-391.
Smoll, F.L., & Smith, R.E. (1989). Leadership behaviors in
sport: A theoretical model and research paradigm. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 19, 1522-1551.
Theeboom, M., DeKnop, R, & Weiss, M.R. (1995). Motivational
climate, psychological responses and motor skill development in
children's sport: A field-based intervention study. Journal of
Sport & Exercise Psychology, 17, 294-311.
Treasure, D.C., & Roberts, G.C. (1998). Relationship between
female adolescents' achievement goal orientations, perceptions of
the motivational climate, belief about success and sources of
satisfaction in basketball. International Journal of Sport Psychology,
29, 211-230.
Rachel E. Brinkman-Majewski
Upper Iowa University
Windee M. Weiss
University of Northern Iowa
Address correspondence to: Rachel E. Brinkman-Majewski Assistant
Professor of Athletic Training, Coordinator of Athletic Training
Clinical Education, Upper Iowa University PO box 1857 Fayette, IA 52142.
Email: majewskir@uiu.edu
Table 1.
Correlations, Alpha Coefficients, and Descriptive Statistics
for All Variables
Cooperative Effort/ Important
learning improvement role
Cooperative .86
learning
Effort/ .46 * .73
improvement
Important .49 * .57 * .82
role
Unequal -.22 -.41 * -.60 *
recognition
Punishment -.17 -.46 * -.47 *
for
mistakes
Task .34 * .54 * .40 *
orientation
Ego -.15 -.08 -.33 *
orientation
Enjoyment 14 .20 .15
Perceived -.02 .16 .04
competence
(3)
Mean 3.30 (0.62) 4.10 (0.38) 4.07 (0.56)
Standard
deviation
Punishment
Unequal for Task
recognition mistakes orientation
Cooperative
learning
Effort/
improvement
Important
role
Unequal .89
recognition
Punishment .61 * .79
for
mistakes
Task -.35 * -.27 * .76
orientation
Ego .33 * .28 * .14
orientation
Enjoyment -.03 -.09 .11
Perceived -.07 -.18 .25
competence
(3)
Mean 2.08 (0.65) 1.87 (0.49) 4.19 (0.43)
Standard
deviation
Perceived
Ego competence
orientation Enjoyment (a)
Cooperative
learning
Effort/
improvement
Important
role
Unequal
recognition
Punishment
for
mistakes
Task
orientation
Ego .88
orientation
Enjoyment -.07 .91
Perceived .10 .14 .70
competence
(3)
Mean 3.13 (0.85) 2.47 (0.98) 3.06 (0.48)
Standard
deviation
Note. Alpha coefficients for each subscale can be seen along
the diagonal in italics, with correlations among the
variables seen below the diagonal.
(a) = Perceived competence on 4-point scale, others on a 5-
point scale; * p < .01.
Table 2.
Means and Standard Deviations for Perceptions of
the Motivational Climate by Gender
Males Females
(n = 87) (n = 93)
Cooperative learning 3.21 (0.69) 3.38 (0.55)
Effort & improvement 4.08 (0.39) 4.10 (0.37)
Important role 3.95 (0.59) 4.17 * (0.50)
Unequal recognition 2.18 * (0.67) 1.98 (0.63)
Punishment for mistakes 1.98 * (0.49) 1.77 (0.47)
Note. Standard deviations in parentheses below the means.
* = significant difference, p < .001
Table 3.
Means and Standard Deviations for Perceptions of the
Motivational Climate by Starter Status
Redshirt/
medical
hardship Starter Non-starter
(n = 203) (n = 106) (n = 43)
Cooperative learning 3.23 3.33 3.26
(0.64) (0.66) (0.52)
Effort & improvement 3.98 4.11 4.10
(0.37) (0.41) (0.30)
Important role 3.98 4.09 3.96
(0.46) (0.58) (0.53)
Unequal recognition 2.16 1.95* 2.41**
(0.55) (0.59) (0.71)
Punishment 1.91 1.82 2.02
for mistakes (0.48) (0.44) (0.49)
Note. Standard deviations in parentheses below the means.
*, ** = groups significantly different from each other, p < .01
Table 4.
Means and Standard Deviations for Perceptions of
the Motivational Climate by Goal
Orientation
Low task-- Low task-- High task-- High task--
low ego high ego low ego high ego
(n = 43) (n = 37) (n = 28) (n = 30)
Cooperative 3.22 3.05 * 3.62 ** 3.44
learning (0.55) (0.56) (0.50) (0.70)
Effort & 3.96 * 3.91 * 4.40" 4.23 **
improvement (0.35) (0.38) (0.32) (0.32)
Important role 4.04 ** 3.71 ** 4.58 * 4.17 **
(0.45) (0.53) (0.45) (0.49)
Unequal 2.05' * 2.54 * 1.53" 2.03 **
recognition (0.39) (0.67) (0.64) -0.73
Punishment 1.90 ** 2.14 ** 1.48 * 1.93 **
for mistakes (0.35) (0.40) (0.43) (0.66)
Note. Standard deviations in parentheses below the means.
*, ** = groups significantly different from each
other, p < .0001