Comparing a domain-specific and global measure of perfectionism in competitive female figure skaters.
Dunn, John G.H. ; Craft, Janelle M. ; Dunn, Janice Causgrove 等
Theorists and researchers generally conceptualize the personality
trait of perfectionism as a multidimensional construct (Frost, Marten,
Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990; Hewitt & Flea, 1991), with the core
facet representing an individual's tendency to set extremely high
standards for personal performance (Stoeber & Otto, 2006). Although
a variety of multidimensional self-report instruments have been
developed to assess perfectionism (for reviews see Enns & Cox, 2002;
Flett & Hewitt, 2002; Stoeber & Otto, 2006), test-users face a
critical decision when choosing a perfectionism measure because the
choice of instrument will have implications for the validity,
interpretation, and generalizability of results (Flett & Hewitt,
2002). The choice of instrument seems especially important in studies
examining perfectionism in competitive sport settings because many
researchers have argued that perfectionism in sport should be
conceptualized and measured as a domain-specific construct (e.g., Anshel
& Eom, 2003; Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, & Syrotuik, 2002; Stoeber,
Otto, Pescheck, Becker, & Stoll, 2007). Therefore, the primary
purpose of the current study was to directly compare potential benefits
of using a domain-specific versus global (i.e., domain-free or
dispositional) measure of perfectionism in a competitive sport setting.
Measuring perfectionism as a domain-specific construct is not a
novel idea. Indeed, long before the creation of global measures of
perfectionism (i.e., instruments which provide no situational/contextual
frame of reference for respondents when they evaluate their
perfectionist tendencies) theorists argued that individuals'
perfectionist tendencies are likely to vary across different life
settings (see Hollender, 1965; Missildine, 1963). Empirical support for
the view that perfectionist tendencies vary across situational contexts
has been provided in both sport (e.g., Dunn, Gotwals, & Causgrove
Dunn, 2005) and non-sport settings (e.g., Mitchelson & Burns, 1998).
Dunn et al. (2005) found that male (n = 133) and female (n = 108)
intercollegiate varsity athletes reported significantly higher mean
levels of perfectionism across all three dimensions of Hewitt and
Flett's (1991) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Hewitt-MPS)
when asked to respond in the context of sport versus school/academe or
life in general. Similarly, Mitchelson and Bums found that 67 working
mothers reported significantly greater mean levels of perfectionism
(when measured with the Hewitt-MPS) at work than at home.
While the results of the aforementioned studies support the view
that the situational context should be considered when measuring
perfectionism, some sport psychologists have taken this idea further and
have shown that there may be dimensions of perfectionism unique to the
competitive sport environment. For example, Dunn et al. (2002) and
Anshel and Eom (2003) independently identified a dimension of
perfectionism that reflects athletes' perceptions of the role that
coaches play in exerting pressures on athletes to reach certain
performance standards. Dunn et al. (2002) posited that this Perceived
Coach Pressure dimension of perfectionism is a domain-specific aspect of
Socially Prescribed Perfectionism (SPP)--where SPP is defined as the
degree to which an individual feels that personal performance standards
and performance expectations are imposed and judged by significant
others (Hewitt & Flett, 1991). If, as previous research suggests,
perceived coach pressure is an important dimension of perfectionism to
consider in sport-perfectionism research (see Dunn, Causgrove Dunn,
Gotwals, Vallance, Craft, & Syrotuik, 2006; Dunn, Gotwals, Causgrove
Dunn, & Syrotuik, 2006; Sagar & Stoeber, 2009; Vallance, Dunn,
& Causgrove Dunn, 2006), the implications of choosing an instrument
to measure perfectionism in sport are further reinforced because global
perfectionism measures do not specifically identify the coach as a
source of socially prescribed perfectionism.
According to Stoeber, Uphill, and Hotham (2009), the most widely
used domain-specific measure of perfectionism in sport is Dunn et
al.'s (2002) Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale
(Sport-MPS). The Sport-MPS was created around the conceptual framework
provided by Frost et al.'s (1990) Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale (Frost-MPS) and measures four dimensions of perfectionism in
sport: Personal Standards, Concern Over Mistakes, Perceived Coach
Pressure, and Perceived Parental Pressure. The Sport-MPS has undergone
extensive psychometric evaluation, has consistently demonstrated good
factorial and criterion-related validity evidence, and has repeatedly
demonstrated acceptable levels of internal consistency (see Dunn,
Causgrove Dunn et al., 2006; Dunn et al., 2002). However, the extent to
which the Sport-MPS provides a greater understanding of perfectionism in
sport beyond that provided by existing global (or domain-free) measures
of perfectionism is still undetermined. Consequently, the first purpose
of the current study was to compare the predictive power of the
Sport-MPS against an established measure of global perfectionism in a
competitive sport setting.
When comparing the measurement benefits of different instruments
(that measure similar constructs), researchers can also benefit from
examining the degree to which the different instruments produce results
that contribute to an enhanced understanding of the nomological network
(Cronbach & Meehl, 1955) that encompasses the construct being
measured (Messick, 1989). Stated differently, decisions to use
domain-specific measures over global measures of personality traits
should also be based on the extent to which the domain-specific
instrument sheds light on theoretical issues surrounding the
construct-in-question beyond that which can be achieved by a global
instrument (see Gauvin & Russell, 1993). To this end, the second
purpose of this study was to determine if a domain-specific measure of
perfectionism was capable of adding to the theoretical knowledge-base
surrounding perfectionism in sport beyond that which could be achieved
by a global measure of perfectionism. The particular theoretical issue
of interest in this study was the healthy versus unhealthy nature of
perfectionism in sport.
Healthy vs. Unhealthy Perfectionism
Some theorists argue that perfectionist tendencies are primarily
unhealthy in nature (e.g., Flett & Hewitt, 2002; Hall, 2006) whereas
others have proposed that perfectionist tendencies can be either healthy
or unhealthy (e.g., Hamachek, 1978; Rice & Ashby, 2007). On the
basis of a review of 35 empirical studies, Stoeber and Otto (2006)
recently presented evidence supporting the distinction between healthy
and unhealthy perfectionism and concluded that healthy perfectionism was
best conceptualized as the combination of high perfectionist strivings
(i.e., high personal standards and high self-oriented perfectionism)
with low perfectionist concerns (i.e., low concern over mistakes and low
socially prescribed perfectionism). In contrast, Stoeber and Otto
proposed that unhealthy perfectionism was best conceptualized as the
combination of high perfectionist strivings with high perfectionist
concerns. Unfortunately, none of the studies in Stoeber and Otto's
(2006) review were conducted with athletes in competitive sport
settings, so the debate over whether there is a distinction between
healthy and unhealthy perfectionism in sport is still largely
unresolved.
Although the majority of perfectionism research that has been
conducted in sport tends to support the view that perfectionism is a
dysfunctional personality trait (see Hail, 2006, for a review), it
should be noted that the majority of sport-based perfectionism research
has used global measures to assess the construct in athletes. However, a
small but emerging body of research has employed domain (i.e., sport)
specific measures to assess athletes' perfectionist orientations
and has provided evidence of both unhealthy and healthy perfectionism in
sport (e.g., Dunn et al., 2002; Stoeber & Becker, 2008; Stoeber et
al., 2007).
The ongoing debate over the functional nature of perfectionism in
sport (see Hall, 2006) may potentially be addressed by examining the
relationship between athletes' perfectionist orientations and
attitudinal body image. Attitudinal body image can be defined as the
manner in which individuals feel satisfied or dissatisfied with some
aspect (or aspects) of their physical appearance (Cash & Szymanski,
1995). Athletes who become highly dissatisfied with their bodies
(especially as they pertain to being overweight) may be at risk of
developing eating disorders or experiencing psychological distress (see
Davis, 2002).
According to theory, unhealthy perfectionists are more self
critical, less self-accepting of personal flaws, and have higher
self-presentational concerns than healthy perfectionists (Hamachek,
1978; Stoeber & Otto, 2006). Consequently, unhealthy perfectionists
would be expected to be more concerned or dissatisfied with their
physical appearance than healthy perfectionists. Research that has
investigated this theoretical proposition (e.g., Davis, 1997; Pearson
& Gleaves, 2006) has relied on global measures of perfectionism. In
line with the majority of perfectionism research in sport psychology,
these studies have produced considerable evidence supporting the link
between unhealthy perfectionist orientations and negative body image
(see Davis, 2002) but little evidence supporting a link between healthy
perfectionist orientations and positive body image. It is currently
unknown if similar findings would have emerged had domain-specific
measures of perfectionism been employed, and whether the domain-specific
conceptualization and measurement of perfectionism would have provided
evidence supporting the theorized link between healthy/adaptive
perfectionism and positive/healthy attitudinal body image.
To summarize, given the absence of studies that directly compare
the potential benefits of measuring perfectionism with domain-specific
versus global measures of perfectionism in sport (see Flett &
Hewitt, 2005) the present research had two major objectives. First, the
study sought to compare the ability of a domain-specific measure of
perfectionism (i.e., the Sport-MPS: Dunn et al., 2002) and a global
measure of perfectionism (i.e., the Hewitt-MPS: Hewitt & Flett,
1991) to predict attitudinal body image in a sample of athletes. Second,
the study sought to compare the ability of the two instruments to shed
light on the ongoing debate over whether perfectionist tendencies are
primarily unhealthy in nature or whether perfectionist tendencies may
also play a healthy/adaptive role in sport. The Sport-MPS and Hewitt-MPS
were selected as the measures of choice because both have been used to
measure perfectionism in athletes and both have undergone extensive
psychometric evaluation (see Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, et al. 2006; Enns
& Cox, 2002).
Method
Participants
A total of I 19 competitive female figure skaters from clubs across
Canada (M age = 14.56 years, SD = 3.42) voluntarily participated in the
study. (1) The sample contained the full range of competition levels
that are sanctioned by Skate Canada (the sport's national governing
body in Canada) and included 40 juvenile (M age = 11.75 years, SD =
1.31), 20 pre novice (M age = 13.25 years, SD = 1.21), 32 novice (M age
= 15.13 years, SD = 1.21), 12 junior (M age = 16.58 years, SD = 1.62),
and 15 senior athletes (M age = 21.00 years, SD = 3.61). Each
competitive level provides an opportunity for athletes to compete in
national championship competitions. On average, the combined sample had
been involved in figure skating for 6.21 years (SD = 3.30) and engaged
in 2.69 hours of on-ice training (SD = .95) and 1.59 hours of off-ice
training (SD = 1.11) each day. The sport of female figure skating was
chosen because perfectionist tendencies and body image concerns are
believed to be prevalent in female athletes who compete in aesthetic
sports (such as figure skating) where pressures are often exerted on
athletes to maintain an "ideal" lean body type (Taylor &
Ste-Marie, 2001).
Instruments
Athletes completed five self-report instruments: (1) a demographic
questionnaire, (2) a skating-modified version of the Sport-MPS, (3) the
Hewitt-MPS, (4) a skating-modified version of the Multidimensional Body
Self Relations Questionnaire-Appearance Scale (MBSRQ-AS: Cash, 2000a),
and (5) a skating-modified version of the Body-Image Ideals
Questionnaire (BIQ: Cash & Szymanski, 1995).
Sport-Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Sport-MPS). The
Sport-MPS (Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, et al., 2006; Dunn et al., 2002) is a
30-item instrument that assesses four dimensions of perfectionism in
sport: Personal Standards (PS), Concern Over Mistakes (COM) Perceived
Parental Pressure (PPP) and Perceived Coach Pressure (PCP). Respondents
rate the extent to which they agree with each item using a 5-point scale
(1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree) with higher composite
subscale scores reflecting higher levels of perfectionism on each
respective dimension. The Sport-MPS has consistently demonstrated
acceptable levels of internal consistency (as > .70) across all
subscales in a variety of studies (see Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, et al,
2006; Dunn et al., 2002; Vallance et al., 2006), and factor analyses of
Sport-MPS data have consistently identified the same four factors across
independent samples of athletes (see Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, et al., 2006;
Dunn et al., 2002).
The Sport-MPS was originally developed for use with Canadian
Football players (Dunn et al., 2002). As such, the original items were
written for team-sport contexts. Consequently, a number of items were
slightly re-worded in this study to make them more relevant to the sport
of figure skating. Specifically, the words "play",
"player" and "players" in the original inventory
were respectively replaced by the words "skate", "figure
skater" and "figure skaters." Multidimensional
Perfectionism Scale (Hewitt-MPS). The Hewitt-MPS (Hewitt & Flett,
1991) is one of the most widely used and well-established measures of
perfectionism in the extant literature (Enns & Cox, 2002). It
conceptualizes and measures perfectionism as a global personality trait,
and contains 45 items that measure three dimensions of perfectionism:
Self-Oriented Perfectionism (SOP), Socially Prescribed Perfectionism
(SPP), and Other-Oriented Perfectionism (OOP). Respondents rate the
degree to which they agree that items reflect their "personal
characteristics and traits" on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly
disagree; 7 = strongly agree). Higher composite subscale scores reflect
higher levels of perfectionism. A large number of clinical-, social-,
and personality-psychology studies have provided reliability and
validity evidence supporting use of the Hewitt-MPS as a measure of
multidimensional global perfectionism (see Enns & Cox, for a
review). Internal consistency levels for the three subscales have been
shown to be acceptable ([alpha]s > .70: see Hewitt & Flett) and
criterion-related validity evidence has been established for the
subscales by way of statistically significant theoretically meaningful
correlations with other measures of global perfectionism (see Enns &
Cox).
Multidimensional Body Self Relations Questionnaire-Appearance Scale
(MBSRQ-AS). The MBSRQ-AS contains 34 items and is a shortened version of
the original 69-item MBSRQ (see Cash, 2000a). The instrument measures
both cognitive/motivational and affective aspects of attitudinal
body-image and contains five subscales that are labeled Appearance
Evaluation (AE), Appearance Orientation (AO), Overweight Preoccupation
(OP), Self-Classified Weight (SCW), and the Body Areas Satisfaction
Scale (BASS). Five-point scales are used by respondents to answer items
in each subscale: AE, AO, and OP (1 = definitely disagree; 5 =
definitely agree), SCW (1 = very underweight; 5 = very overweight), and
BASS (1 = very dissatisfied; 5 = very satisfied). Unhealthy/negative
attitudinal body image is reflected in high AO, OP and SCW scores and
low AE and BASS scores. All subscales have demonstrated acceptable
levels of internal consistency (i.e., [alpha]s > .70) and test-retest
reliability over a one month period (rs > .74) with female
participants (Cash, 2000a).
A number of minor changes were made to the wording of several
MBSRQ-AS items in an attempt to increase the relevance of the instrument
for the current sample of figure skaters. For example, the phrase
"Before going out in public ..." was changed to "Before
going out to the skating arena ..." and "... the way my
clothes fit me" was changed to "... the way my skating outfits
fit me." Given the potential for a few athletes within the current
sample to be at a prepubescent stage of development, we deleted two
appearance evaluation items that asked respondents to consider if they
viewed their bodies as "sexually appealing" and whether they
liked the way their bodies looked without clothing.
Body-Image Ideals Questionnaire (BIQ). The BIQ (Cash &
Szymanski, 1995) is an 11-item measure of evaluative body image that
asks respondents to rate themselves according to a variety of physical
characteristics. For each item respondents rate (a) the perceived
discrepancy (0 = exactly as I am; 3 = very unlike me) between their
ideal self and actual self on the physical characteristic in question
(e.g., skin complexion, weight, etc.) and (b) the degree to which it is
important (0 = not important; 3 = very important) that they reach their
ideal self on the physical characteristic in question. A weighted
discrepancy score is calculated for each item whereby the cross product
of the discrepancy and importance ratings is computed (see Cash &
Szymanski, for detailed explanation of scoring). Higher positive
"weighted discrepancy scores" reflect a heightened discrepancy
between the actual self and ideal self, and for this discrepancy to
assume importance in the individual's life (i.e., higher scores
reflect a more unhealthy body image). The BIQ has demonstrated
acceptable levels of internal consistency (i.e., [alpha] > .70), and
convergent validity evidence has been demonstrated through statistically
significant theoretically meaningful correlations with several measures
of body image (see Cash & Szymanski) in female samples. An overview
of the construct validity evidence surrounding the BIQ is reported by
Cash (2000b).
A number of minor modifications were made to the BIQ to make the
instrument more relevant to the sport of figure skating. First, the
phrase "as a figure skater" was added to each item (e.g.,
"My ideal height is ..." was changed to read, "My ideal
height as a figure skater is..."). Second, given the potential for
a few athletes to be at a pre-pubescent stage of development, one item
that asked respondents to rate their chest size was removed, leaving a
total of 10 items in the instrument.
Procedures
Standard ethical and informed consent procedures were adopted
throughout the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all
athletes, and written parental consent was obtained for all athletes who
were under the age of 18 years at the time of the study. Instruments
were individually administered to athletes by one member of the research
team at each skater's respective club. The demographic
questionnaire was always administered first, with the remaining
instruments being presented in eight different presentation orders (to
minimize potential order effects). Athletes took approximately 45
minutes to complete the instruments.
Results
Preliminary data analyses. Results of three separate MANOVAs (one
for Sport-MPS subscales [n = 4], one for Hewitt-MPS subscales [n = 3],
and one for body image variables [n = 6] entered as dependent variables)
with "competitive level" entered as the independent variable
found only three (out of a possible 130) between-group differences
across the perfectionism and body-image variables at the univariate
level of analysis. All homogeneity tests of covariance (Box's M)
that were conducted in conjunction with each MANOVA were not
significant, indicating that the covariance matrices associated with the
perfectionism and body image variables across each of the five
competitive levels did not significantly differ. Given that there were
very few between-group differences across the five competitive levels on
the perfectionism and body image variables (and no differences in the
structure of the covariance matrices), the data across the competition
levels were combined into a single data set (N = 119) for all subsequent
analyses.
Table 1 contains descriptive statistics and internal consistency
values for all 13 subscales for the combined sample. With the exception
of the other-oriented perfectionism (OOP) subscale, all subscales had
acceptable levels of internal consistency ([alpha] > .70). On the
basis of the marginal level of internal consistency associated with
other-oriented perfectionism ([alpha] = .67) readers are advised to
interpret results pertaining to this subscale with some degree of
caution. Table 2 contains the bivariate correlations among all subscales
for the combined sample. All correlations were < |.70 | indicating
that there would be no multicollinearity problems among the variables
that could potentially affect the results of the upcoming regression
analyses (see Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996, p. 86).
Assessing the Predictive Power of the Sport-MPS vs. the Hewitt-MPS
To determine if the Sport-MPS and the Hewitt-MPS explained
different amounts of unique variance in attitudinal body image, a series
of hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted. For the
prediction of each body-image variable, the three Hewitt-MPS subscales
were entered simultaneously in the first step and the four Sport-MPS
subscales were entered simultaneously in the second step. The change in
[R.sup.2] values and associated F-statistics for each step are contained
in Table 3. For four of the six body-image variables (AO, AE, BASS, and
BIQ) the Sport-MPS explained a significant amount of variance over and
above the variance explained by the Hewitt-MPS (all ps < .05)
suggesting that the Sport-MPS was valuable in explaining variance in
attitudinal body image that had not been explained by the Hewitt-MPS.
Upon completion of these initial regression analyses, the entry
order of the perfectionism measures was then reversed such that the
Sport-MPS subscales were entered in the first step and the Hewitt-MPS
subscales were entered in the second step. This reversal of entry order
was important because we wanted to determine if the Hewitt-MPS would be
able to explain variance in attitudinal body image that was not
explained by the Sport-MPS. In each of the six analyses, the Sport-MPS
explained a significant amount of body-image variance when entered in
the first step (all [R.sup.2] >. 13; all ps < .005). However, when
the Hewitt-MPS subscales were entered in the second step in each
analysis, no statistically significant changes in [R.sup.2] were
obtained (all [DELTA][R.sup.2] < .036; all ps > .20). These
findings indicate that the Hewitt-MPS was unable to explain significant
amounts of body-image variance beyond that which had been accounted for
by the Sport-MPS.
Assessing the Functional Nature of Perfectionism with the Sport-MPS
and Hewitt-MPS
Given that perfectionism researchers and theorists have argued that
the healthy vs. unhealthy nature of perfectionism is best determined by
considering scores across all perfectionism subscales simultaneously
(e.g., Dunn et al., 2002; Frost et al., 1990; Gotwals, Dunn, &
Wayment, 2003; Rice & Ashby, 2007; Stoeber & Otto, 2006),
separate canonical correlation analyses were employed to examine the
multivariate relationships between (a) perfectionism in sport (as
measured by the Sport-MPS) and attitudinal body-image, and (b) global
perfectionism (as measured by the Hewitt-MPS) and attitudinal
body-image.
In accordance with the recommendations of Tabachnick and Fidell
(1996), prior to conducting the canonical correlation analyses, the data
were screened for the presence of multi variate outliers. Separate
screening procedures (using Mahalonobis distances and [chi square] tests
of significance) were conducted on the Sport-MPS and body-image data
(i.e., the data to be included in the first canonical correlation
analysis), and on the Hewitt-MPS and body-image data (i.e., the data to
be included in the second canonical correlation analysis). One
multivariate outlier was identified in each analysis, and upon further
inspection of the data it was revealed that the outlier was the same
person, Consequently, this individual was removed from the two canonical
correlation analyses. Subject-to-variable ratios exceeded the minimum
criterion (i.e., 10:1) required for each canonical correlation analysis
(see Tabachnick & Fidell).
The five MBSRQ-AS subscales and the BIQ weighted discrepancy score
were entered as the criterion set in both canonical correlation
analyses. The four subscales of the Sport-MPS were entered as the
predictor set in the first analysis and the three Hewitt-MPS subscales
were entered as the predictor set in the second analysis. Tabachnick and
Fidell (1996) recommend that canonical functions should be retained only
if they are statistically significant and practically meaningful. To
this end, canonical functions were retained only if the canonical
correlation coefficient ([R.sub.C) was statistically significant (p <
.05) and exceeded a minimum criterion value of .30. Using these
criteria, two significant canonical functions were extracted when the
Sport-MPS subscales were entered as the predictor set ([R.sub.C1] = .63;
[R.sub.C2] = .43), and one significant function was extracted when the
Hewitt-MPS subscales were entered as the predictor set ([R.sub.C] =
.58). Within each significant canonical function, only variables with
canonical loadings [greater than or equal to] [absolute value of .30] on
their respective canonical variates were considered for interpretation
purposes.
Perfectionism and unhealthy attitudinal body image. As seen in
Table 4, the canonical loadings associated with all of the body-image
and perfectionism variables were [greater than or equal to] |.30| in the
first canonical function produced by the initial canonical correlation
analysis (i.e., when the Sport-MPS subscales were entered as the
predictor set). Appearance orientation, overweight preoccupation,
self-classified weight and the BIQ weighted discrepancy variables had
moderate to strong positive loadings, while the appearance evaluation
and body areas satisfaction subscales had moderate negative loadings on
the body image variate. Collectively this pattern of loadings reflects
an unhealthy/negative attitudinal body image. All four Sport-MPS
subscales had moderate to strong positive loadings on the perfectionism
variate in the first canonical function. This pattern of loadings is
reflective of an unhealthy perfectionist orientation (cf. Dunn et al.,
2002; Stoeber & Otto, 2006). The magnitude and direction of the
canonical correlation between these two canonical variates ([R.sub.C1] =
.63) indicates that the predisposition to adopt an unhealthy/negative
attitudinal body image in figure skating is associated with a heightened
tendency to possess an unhealthy perfectionist orientation in sport.
Similar results were obtained in the canonical correlation analysis
conducted with the Hewitt-MPS subscales entered as the predictor set. In
this analysis, all global perfectionism and body-image variables had
interpretable canonical loadings (i.e., [greater than or equal to]
|.30|) on their respective variates (see Table 4). The direction and
magnitude of all six body-image loadings were very similar to those
corresponding with the unhealthy body-image variate in the first
canonical function of the initial canonical analysis (when Sport-MPS
subscales had been entered as the predictor set). In addition, all three
Hewitt-MPS subscales had positive loadings on the perfectionism variate;
this pattern of perfectionism loadings reflects unhealthy/maladaptive
perfectionism (see Rice & Ashby, 2007; Sherry, Hewitt, Besser,
McGee, & Flett, 2004). The magnitude and direction of the canonical
correlation ([R.sub.C]= .58) between these two variates indicates that
the predisposition to adopt an unhealthy/negative attitudinal body image
in figure skating is associated with a heightened tendency to possess an
unhealthy/maladaptive profile of global perfectionism.
Perfectionism and healthy attitudinal body image. As reported
earlier, the initial canonical correlation analysis with the Sport-MPS
subscales entered as the predictor set produced a second significant
canonical function ([R.sub.C2]= .43). The patterns of canonical loadings
in this second canonical function were quite different from those in the
first canonical function (see Table 4). Three of the six body-image
subscales and three of the four sport perfectionism subscales had
interpretable loadings. Appearance evaluation (AE) and the body areas
satisfaction scale (BASS) had strong positive loadings and the BIQ
weighted discrepancy had a moderate negative loading on the body image
variate. Collectively, this pattern of loadings reflects a healthy or
positive attitudinal body image. The personal standards subscale of the
Sport-MPS had a moderate positive loading on the perfectionism variate
of the second canonical function (see Table 4). In contrast, the concern
over mistakes and perceived coach pressure subscales had low to moderate
negative loadings on this perfectionism variate. This pattern of
loadings is consistent with theorized descriptions of normal/healthy
perfectionism (see Hamachek, 1978; Stoeber & Otto, 2006). The
magnitude and direction of the canonical correlation between these two
canonical variates ([R.sub.C2]= .43) indicates that the predisposition
to adopt a healthy or positive attitudinal body image in figure skating
is associated with a heightened tendency to possess a healthy or
adaptive perfectionist orientation in sport.
Discussion
The first purpose of this study was to compare the predictive power
of the Sport-MPS against the Hewitt-MPS in explaining attitudinal
body-image variance in the current sample of female figure skaters.
Regression analyses showed that for four of the six body-image variables
the Sport-MPS subscales explained a significant amount of unique
variance over and above that which was explained by the Hewitt-MPS
subscales. In contrast, when the Hewitt-MPS subscales were entered in
the second step of each regression analysis, the Hewitt-MPS was unable
to explain a significant amount of unique variance in any of the six
body-image variables beyond that which had already been explained by the
Sport-MPS. In other words, the domain-specific measurement of
perfectionism had greater predictive power than the global measurement
of perfectionism with respect to accounting for variance in figure
skaters' attitudinal body-image.
Similar results supporting the superior predictive power of
domain-specific measures over global measures of psychological
dispositions have been documented in the extant literature (e.g., Bing,
Whanger, Kristl Davison, & Van Hook, 2004; Hunthausen, Truxillo,
Bauer, & Hammer, 2003; Laux, Glanzmann, & Schaffer, 1985;
Martens & Simon, 1976). However, to the best of our knowledge this
is the first direct comparison of the predictive power of a
domain-specific vs. global measure of perfectionism in sport. Although
it would be difficult to generalize the current findings beyond the
current sample of competitive female figure skaters, these results
provide the first evidence that domain-specific assessments may be more
useful than global assessments when measuring perfectionism in sport
settings. Clearly more research is needed to determine if these results
are sample-specific, and if they would generalize to other groups of
athletes or to the prediction of other sport-related cognitive,
affective, and behavioral variables such as competitive anxiety,
competitive anger, or competitive performance. Also, it is important to
recognize that other multidimensional sport-specific measures (e.g., the
Multidimensional Perfectionism Inventory in Sport: see Stoeber et al.,
2007) and global measures (e.g., the Frost-MPS: Frost et al., 1990) of
perfectionism are available to researchers, therefore, the current
results cannot be generalized to these inventories. Nevertheless, the
superior predictive power of the Sport-MPS over the Hewitt-MPS in this
study informs researchers about a potential benefit that the
domain-specific measurement of perfectionism might have over the global
measurement of perfectionism in a sport setting.
Gauvin and Russell (1993) argued that when decisions are being made
about which instrument to choose for research purposes, instruments that
can clarify or expand theoretical knowledge around a particular
construct or phenomenon are generally preferred over measures that
cannot achieve this research objective. To this end, the second purpose
of the current study was to compare the Sport-MPS and the Hewitt-MPS in
their abilities to shed light on the ongoing debate over whether
perfectionism in sport has both healthy/adaptive and
unhealthy/maladaptive qualities (see Dunn et al., 2002), or if
perfectionism is primarily an unhealthy/debilitative construct in sport
(see Flett & Hewitt, 2005). To achieve this goal, canonical
correlation analyses were conducted that permitted a comparison between
the Sport MPS and Hewitt-MPS regarding the information they provided
about the functional nature of perfectionism in the current sample of
figure skaters.
Regardless of whether the Sport-MPS or Hewitt-MPS was used to
measure perfectionism, both canonical correlation analyses produced
solutions which depicted profiles (i.e., canonical variates) resembling
unhealthy/maladaptive perfectionism that were significantly related to
profiles of negative/unhealthy attitudinal body image. The profiles of
unhealthy/ maladaptive perfectionism from the Sport-MPS and Hewitt-MPS
directly reflect Stoeber and Otto's (2006) description of unhealthy
perfectionism that is characterized by the combination of high
perfectionist strivings (i.e., high personal standards or high
self-oriented perfectionism) with high perfectionist concerns (i.e.,
high concern over mistakes, high perceived coach pressure, and/or high
socially prescribed perfectionism). These results are not only
consistent with the body-image literature that has documented the
association between unhealthy/maladaptive perfectionism and
unhealthy/negative body image (see Davis, 2002), but are also consistent
with the perfectionism literature in both clinical psychology (e.g.,
Flett & Hewitt, 2002; Rice, Bail Castro, Cohen, & Hood, 2003)
and sport psychology (e.g., Flett & Hewitt, 2005; Hall, 2006) where
perfectionism has been largely characterized as a debilitating,
unhealthy, and self-defeating construct.
It seems reasonable to conclude that both the Sport-MPS and
Hewitt-MPS were equally useful in identifying the anticipated
relationship between unhealthy/maladaptive perfectionism and
negative/unhealthy attitudinal body image. This position is further
supported by the fact that the multivariate relationship between
unhealthy perfectionism (as defined by the pattern of Sport-MPS subscale
loadings) and unhealthy attitudinal body image ([R.sub.C]= .63) was
similar in magnitude to the relationship between unhealthy perfectionism
(as defined by the pattern of Hewitt-MPS subscale loadings) and
unhealthy attitudinal body image ([R.sub.C]= .58).
In marked contrast to the ability of both the Sport-MPS and
Hewitt-MPS to capture the positive relationship between
unhealthy/maladaptive perfectionism and unhealthy/negative attitudinal
body image, only the Sport-MPS provided results that supported the
theorized link between healthy/adaptive perfectionism and
healthy/positive attitudinal body image. Specifically, the canonical
results showed a profile of healthy/adaptive perfectionism (as measured
by the Sport-MPS) that directly reflected Stoeber and Otto's (2006)
description of healthy perfectionism: namely, high perfectionist
strivings (i.e., high personal standards) were combined with low
perfectionist concerns (i.e., low concern over mistakes and low
perceived coach pressure). This finding is important on a number of
fronts. First, it provides evidence supporting the view that
perfectionism can have healthy/adaptive functions in sport (see Dunn et
al., 2002; Stoeber et al., 2007). Second, it highlights the usefulness
of employing analytical techniques that consider scores across all
perfectionism dimensions simultaneously when examining the functional
(i.e., healthy vs. unhealthy) nature of perfectionism (Stoeber &
Otto). Third, it provides empirical support for a link between healthy
perfectionism and healthy/ positive attitudinal body image that has
received little support in the perfectionism/body-image literature where
global measures of perfectionism have been predominantly used. Lastly,
and most importantly from the perspective of this study's
objectives, this finding indicates that the Sport-MPS was capable of
enhancing theoretical knowledge (through an understanding of healthy
perfectionism in sport) that the Hewitt-MPS was apparently unable to
achieve.
Overall, the regression results and canonical correlation results
obtained in this study indicate that the Sport-MPS fared better than the
Hewitt-MPS in (a) the prediction of unique variance in attitudinal body
image, and (b) the production of knowledge that advances
researchers' understanding of the functional nature of
perfectionism in a sport setting. Taken together, such findings indicate
that the Sport-MPS may be a better choice of instrument than the
Hewitt-MPS when assessing perfectionism within sport contexts. Having
said this, we wish to emphasize that we are not recommending the
abandonment of the Hewitt-MPS as a measure of perfectionism in sport,
nor are we suggesting that the Hewitt-MPS does not have an important
role to play in the assessment of perfectionism in sport settings. For
example, the Hewitt-MPS measures an aspect of perfectionism (i.e.,
other-oriented perfectionism) that the Sport-MPS does not measure, and
it is conceivable that other-oriented perfectionism may play a role in
certain team-sport contexts where athletes expect (or require) high
performance standards from their team mates. Additionally, the
contextual/domain specificity of the Hewitt-MPS can be modified quite
easily to fit a variety of achievement domains (see Dunn et al., 2005;
Mitchelson & Bums, 1998) whereas the item content of the Sport-MPS
does not readily lend itself to this task. As such, the Hewitt-MPS is a
much more useful measurement tool if the researcher is interested in
comparing perfectionist tendencies across different achievement domains
(e.g., Dunn et al.; Mitchelson & Bums). Indeed, the sport-specific
nature of many items contained within the Sport-MPS (e.g., the perceived
coach pressure items) would likely undermine the relevance of the
instrument in any research setting other than competitive sport.
In closing, the present study represents an initial examination of
the potential benefits that can be accrued by employing a
domain-specific measure of perfectionism over a global measure of
perfectionism in a competitive figure skating setting. Given that
perfectionism levels differ across achievement domains (Mitchelson &
Bums, 1998) and because individuals' behaviors vary across
situational contexts (Mischel, Shoda, & Mendoza-Denton, 2002), we
speculate that the superior performance of the Sport-MPS in this study
may have been a function of athletes' uncertainties about the
situational context they should choose when responding to Hewitt-MPS
items (see Bing et al., 2004, for a related discussion). This
uncertainty may have produced error variance in athletes' responses
to the Hewitt-MPS that was not produced when the athletes responded to
the Sport-MPS. Clearly answers to this type of speculation will not be
achieved until more research is conducted that compares the ability of
domain-specific and global measures of perfectionism to predict
different cognitive, affective, and behavioral variables in a variety of
competitive sport settings.
References
Anshel, M. H., & Eom, H. J. (2003). Exploring the
dimensionality of perfectionism in sport. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 34, 255-271.
Bing, M. N., Whanger, J. C., Kristl Davison, H., & Van Hook, J.
B. (2004). Incremental validity of the frame-of-reference effect in
personality scale scores: A replication and extension. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 89, 150-157.
Cash, T. F. (2000a). The Multidimensional Body-Self Relations
Questionnaire: MBSRQ users 'manual (3rd revision, January, 2000).
Norfolk, VA: Old Dominion University.
Cash, T. F. (2000b). Manual for the body-image ideals
questionnaire. Norfolk, VA: Old Dominion University.
Cash, T. F., & Szymanski, M. L. (1995). The development and
validation of the Body Image Ideals Questionnaire. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 64, 466-477.
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in
psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281-302.
Davis, C. (1997). Normal and neurotic perfectionism in eating
disorders: An interactive model. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 22, 421-426.
Davis, C. (2002). Body image and athleticism. In T. F. Cash &
T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of theory, research, and
clinical practice (pp. 219-225). New York: Guildford Press.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., Gotwals, J. K., Vallance, J.
K. H., Craft, J. M., & Syrotuik, D. G. (2006). Establishing
construct validity evidence for the Sport Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 57-79.
Dunn, J. G. H., Causgrove Dunn, J. L., & Syrotuik, D. G.
(2002). Relationship between multidimensional perfectionism and goal
orientations in sport. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24,
376-395.
Dunn, J. G. H., Gotwals, J. K., & Causgrove Dunn, J. (2005). An
examination of the domain specificity of perfectionism among
intercollegiate student-athletes. Personality and Individual
Differences, 38, 1439-1448.
Dunn, J. (2 H., Gotwals, J. K., Causgrove Dunn, J., & Syrotuik,
D. G. (2006). Examining the relationship between perfectionism and trait
anger in competitive sport. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 4, 7-24.
Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (2002). The nature and assessment of
perfectionism: A critical analysis. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt
(Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research and practice (pp. 33-62).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2002). Perfectionism and
maladjustment: An overview of theoretical, definitional, and treatment
issues. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory,
research and practice (pp. 5-31). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2005). The perils of
perfectionism in sports and exercise. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 14, 14-18.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990).
The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
14,449-468.
Gauvin, L., & Russell, S. J. (1993). Sport-specific and
culturally adapted measures in sport and exercise psychology research:
Issues and strategies. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant
(Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 891-900). New
York: Macmillan.
Gotwals, J. K., Dunn, J. G. H., & Wayment, H. A. (2003). An
examination of perfectionism and self-esteem in intercollegiate
athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 26, 17-37.
Hall, H. K. (2006). Perfectionism: A hallmark quality of world
class performers, or a psychological impediment to athletic development?
In D. Hackfort & G. Tenenbaum (Eds.), Perspectives in sport and
exercise psychology: Vol. 1. Essential processes for attaining peak
performance (pp. 178-211). Oxford, U.K.: Meyer and Meyer.
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic
perfectionism. Psychology, 15, 27-33.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism and self
and social contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with
psychopathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
456-470.
Hollender, M. H. (1965). Perfectionism. Comprehensive Psychiatry,
6(2), 94-103.
Hunthausen, J. M., Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., & Hammer, L.
B. (2003). A field study of frame-of-reference effects on personality
test validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 545- 551.
Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., & Schaffer, P. (1985). General vs.
situation specific traits as related to anxiety in ego-threatening
situations. In C.D. Spielberger, I.G. Sarason, & P.B. Defares
(Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 9, pp. 121-128). New York: Hemisphere.
Martens, R., & Simon, J.A. (1976). Comparison of three
predictors of state anxiety in competitive situations. Research
Quarterly, 47, 381-387.
Messick, S. (1989). Validity. In R. L. Linn (Ed.), Educational
measurement (3rd ed., pp. 13-103). New York: American Council on
Education.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2002).
Situation-behavior profiles as a locus of consistency in personality.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(2), 50-44.
Missildine, W. H. (1963). Your inner child of the past. New York:
Simon and Schuster.
Mitchelson, J. K., & Bums, L. R. (1998). Career mothers and
perfectionism: stress at work and at home. Personality and Individual
Differences, 25, 477-485.
Pearson, C. A., & Gleaves, D. H. (2006). The multiple
dimensions of perfectionism and their relation with eating disorder
features. Personality and Individual Differences, 41,225235.
Rice, K. G., & Ashby, J. S. (2007). An efficient method for
classifying perfectionists. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 72-85.
Rice, K. G., Bair, C. J., Castro, J. R., Cohen, B. N., & Hood,
C. A. (2003). Meanings of perfectionism: A quantitative and qualitative
analysis. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 17,39-58.
Sagar, S. S., & Stoeber, J. (2009). Perfectionism, fear of
failure and affective responses to success and failure. The central role
of fear of experiencing shame and embarrassment. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology, 31,602-627.
Sherry, S. B., Hewitt, P. L., Besser, A., McGee, B. J., &
Flett, G. L. (2004). Self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism
in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism subscale. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 69-79.
Stoeber, J., & Becker, C. (2008). Perfectionism, achievement
motives, and attribution of success and failure in female soccer
players. International Journal of Psychology, 43, 980-987.
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of
perfectionism: Approaches, evidence, challenges. Personality and Social
Psychology Review, 10, 295-319.
Stoeber, J., Otto, K., Pescheck, E., Becker, C., & Stoll, O.
(2007). Perfectionism and competitive anxiety in athletes:
Differentiating striving for perfectionism and negative reactions to
imperfection. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 959-969.
Stoeber, J., Uphill, M. A., & Hotham, S. (2009). Predicting
race performance in triathlon: The role of perfectionism, achievement
goals and personal goal setting. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 31, 211-245.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate
statistics (3rd ed.). New York:
HarperCollins.
Taylor, G. M., & Ste-Marie, D. M. (2001). Eating disorders
symptoms in Canadian female pair and dance figure skaters. International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 32, 21-28.
Vallance, J. K.H., Dunn, J.G.H., & Causgrove Dunn, J. (2006).
Perfectionism, anger, and situation criticality in competitive youth ice
hockey. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28, 383-406.
Footnote
With the exception of two athletes, the current sample formed a
sub-sample in a study conducted by Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, et al. (2006).
As such, the means, standard deviations, and correlations among the
Sport-MPS and Hewitt-MPS subscales are based on the same data used in
the Dunn, Causgrove Dunn, et al. publication. However, none of the
current results associated with any of the body-image data have been
previously published.
Author's Note
1.) John Dunn and Janice Causgrove Dunn are with the Faculty of
Physical Education & Recreation, University of Alberta.
2.) Janelle Craft was a graduate student at the University of
Alberta at the time of this study.
3.) John Gotwals is with the School of Kinesiology, Lakehead
University.
4.) This paper was based upon the Masters thesis of Janelle Craft
(nee Dunham) conducted under the supervision of John Dunn. The research
was supported by a grant from the Sport Science Association of Alberta
that was awarded to the first two authors.
Table 1. Subscale Means, Standard Deviations, and Internal
Consistency (a) Values for Combined Sample (N=119)
Subscales M SD a
Sport-MPS
Personal standards 3.35 0.85 .85
Concern over mistakes 2.39 0.96 .87
Perceived parental pressure 2.41 0.94 .84
Perceived coach pressure 2.16 0.87 .87
Hewitt-MPS
Self-oriented perfectionism 5.05 1.00 .87
Socially prescribed perfectionism 3.32 0.99 .85
Other-oriented perfectionism 3.59 0.73 .67
MBSRQ-AS
Appearance orientation 3.51 0.72 .84
Appearance evaluation 3.47 0.83 .76
Overweight preoccupation 2.52 0.98 .77
Self-classified weight 3.04 0.61 .92
Body areas satisfaction 3.64 0.72 .84
BIQ
Body image ideal weighted discrepancy 1.06 1.49 .83
Table 2. Bivariate Correlatios (r) Among Subscales
Sport-MPS
Subscales PS COM PCP PPP
COM .55 --
PCP .26 .59 --
PPP .12 .45 .47 --
SOP .66 .64 .35 .18
SPP .38 .69 .60 .61
OOP .21 .16 .14 .03
AO .50 .49 .27 .20
AE -.02 -.34 -.33 -.22
OWP .42 .49 .29 .25
SCW .13 .33 .22 .26
BASS -.04 -.36 -.28 -.21
BIQ .14 .43 .30 .30
Hewitt-MPS Body Image
Subscales SOP SPP OOP AO AE OWP SCW BASS BdQ
COM
PCP
PPP
SOP --
SPP .44 --
OOP .22 .13 --
AO .42 .33 .17 --
AE -.12 -.30 .02 -.01 --
OWP .39 .41 .22 .52 -.45 --
SCW .31 .26 .03 .13 -.52 .59 --
BASS -.21 -.35 .04 -.20 .66 -.47 -.45 --
BIQ .32 .35 .12 .31 -.61 .56 .51 -.62 --
Note. Correlations > 1.181 are significant at p <.05.
Subscale abbreviations: PS= Personal standards COW =Concern over
mistakes. PCP = Perceived coach pressure. PPP =Perceived parental
pressure. SOP= Self-oriented perfectionism. SPP= Socially
prescribed perfectionism. OOP= Other-oriented perfectionism AO =
Appearance orientation AE = Appearance evaluation. OWP =
Overweight preoccupation. SCW = Self-classified vveight. BASS
=Body areas satisfaction scale. BIQ= Body image ideal
questionnaire (weighted discrepancy).
Table 3. Results of Hierarchical Regression Models Used for the
Prediction of Body Image Variables
[DELTA]R
Block Instrument [R.sup.2] F Change p
Appearance orientation (Final model: F [7, 111] = 6.849,
p < .001, [R.sup.2] = .30)
1 Hewitt-MPS .197 9.432 < .001
2 Sport-MPS .104 4.145 < .005
Appearance evaluation (Final model: F [7, 111 ] = 3.679,
p < .005, [R.sup.2] = .19)
1 Hewitt-MPS .091 3.815 < .05
2 Sport-MPS .098 3.344 < .05
Overweight preoccupation (Final model: F [7, 111] = 6.117,
p < .001, [R.sup.2] = .29)
1 Hewitt-MPS .227 11.268 < .001
2 Sport-MPS .051 1.969 = .104
Self-classified weight (Final model: F [7, 111 = 3.084,
p < .01, [R.sup.2] = .16)
1 Hewitt-MPS .116 5.040 < .005
2 Sport-MPS .047 1.545 = .194
Body areas satisfaction (Final model: F [7, 111] = 3.873,
p < .005, [R.sup.2] = .20)
1 Hewitt-MPS .116 5.040 < .005
2 Sport-MPS .080 2.765 < .05
BIQ weighted discrepancy (Final model: F [7, 1111 = 4.652, p < .001,
[R.sup.2] = .23)
1 Hewitt-MPS .157 7.116 < .001
2 Sport-MPS .070 2.522 < .05
Table 4. Canonical Loadings of Attitudinal Body Image and Perfectionism
Subscales for Significant Canonical Functions
Canonical loadings
Sport perfectionism Global perfectionism
Variables Function 1 Function 2 Function 1
Attitudinal body
image
Appearance .830 .213 .740
orientation
Appearance -.450 .738 -.391
evaluation
Overweight .861 .093 .852
preoccupation
Self-classified .600 -.271 .674
weight
Body areas -.448 .716 -.534
satisfaction
BIQ weighted .691 -.490 .752
discrepancy
Perfectionism
(Sport-MPS)
Personal standards .770 .611 --
Concern over .946 -.305 --
mistakes
Perceived parental .547 -.268 --
pressure
Perceived coach .485 -.407 --
pressure
Perfectionism
(Hewitt-MPS)
Self-oriented -- -- .754
perfectionism
Socially -- -- .902
prescribed
perfectionism
Other-oriented -- -- .373
perfectionism
Note. N = 118 (one multivariate outlier removed).